心理学报 ›› 2021, Vol. 53 ›› Issue (7): 729-745.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00729
吴三美1,2, 田良苏3, 陈家侨3, 陈广耀4(), 王敬欣1()
收稿日期:
2020-09-14
发布日期:
2021-05-24
出版日期:
2021-07-25
通讯作者:
陈广耀,王敬欣
E-mail:ccggyy86@163.com;wjxpsy@126.com
基金资助:
WU Sanmei1,2, TIAN Liangsu3, CHEN Jiaqiao3, CHEN Guangyao4(), WANG Jingxin1()
Received:
2020-09-14
Online:
2021-05-24
Published:
2021-07-25
Contact:
CHEN Guangyao,WANG Jingxin
E-mail:ccggyy86@163.com;wjxpsy@126.com
摘要:
采用眼动追踪技术探讨中文阅读中无关言语效应的影响机制, 操纵了背景音的类型(有意义背景音、无意义背景音、无声), 通过3个实验考察其对简单句、复杂句、篇章阅读的影响。结果发现, 被试在无意义背景音条件下加工三类阅读材料时的眼动指标与无声条件相比差异均不显著; 但是, 在有意义背景音条件下, 被试在进行复杂句和篇章阅读时的眼动指标与无声条件相比差异显著, 而在简单句阅读中差异不显著。结果表明, 语义属性才是影响无关言语效应产生的关键因素, 其所起作用受到阅读任务难度的调节, 而对阅读过程的影响主要体现在晚期的词汇加工和语义整合阶段, 本研究结果支持了语义干扰假说。
中图分类号:
吴三美, 田良苏, 陈家侨, 陈广耀, 王敬欣. (2021). 中文阅读中无关言语效应的认知机制探究:眼动证据. 心理学报, 53(7), 729-745.
WU Sanmei, TIAN Liangsu, CHEN Jiaqiao, CHEN Guangyao, WANG Jingxin. (2021). Exploring the cognitive mechanism of irrelevant speech effect in Chinese reading: Evidence from eye movements. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 53(7), 729-745.
背景音条件 | 平均注视时间(ms) | 句子阅读时间(ms) | 句子注视次数 | 回视次数 |
---|---|---|---|---|
无声 | 224 (1) | 4819 (66) | 12.67 (0.14) | 3.22 (0.06) |
无意义语音 | 224 (1) | 4674 (60) | 12.38 (0.13) | 3.29 (0.06) |
有意义语音 | 225 (1) | 4899 (66) | 13.22 (0.16) | 3.62 (0.07) |
表1 整体分析各眼动指标的均值与标准误
背景音条件 | 平均注视时间(ms) | 句子阅读时间(ms) | 句子注视次数 | 回视次数 |
---|---|---|---|---|
无声 | 224 (1) | 4819 (66) | 12.67 (0.14) | 3.22 (0.06) |
无意义语音 | 224 (1) | 4674 (60) | 12.38 (0.13) | 3.29 (0.06) |
有意义语音 | 225 (1) | 4899 (66) | 13.22 (0.16) | 3.62 (0.07) |
变量 | 平均注视时间 | 句子阅读时间 | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
β | SE | t | p | 95% CI | β | SE | t | p | 95% CI | |
截距 | 224.03 | 4.32 | 51.91 | < 0.001 | [5.15, 5.43] | 4788.43 | 232.69 | 20.58 | < 0.001 | [8.28, 8.56] |
无意义语音vs无声 | -0.38 | 2.24 | -0.17 | 0.87 | [-0.12, 0.32] | -146.26 | 118.41 | -1.24 | 0.22 | [-0.27, 0.13] |
有意义语音vs无声 | 0.39 | 1.67 | 0.23 | 0.82 | [0.17, 0.42] | 58.82 | 103.85 | 0.57 | 0.57 | [0.26, 0.55] |
有意义语音vs无意义语音 | 0.56 | 1.9 | 0.29 | 0.77 | [0.23, 0.58] | 190 | 124.65 | 1.52 | 0.13 | [0.15, 0.46] |
变量 | 句子注视次数 | 回视次数 | ||||||||
β | SE | t | p | 95% CI | β | SE | t | p | 95% CI | |
截距 | 12.66 | 0.48 | 26.12 | < 0.001 | [2.19, 2.54] | 3.28 | 0.18 | 18.37 | < 0.001 | [3.47, 3.86] |
无意义语音vs无声 | -0.31 | 0.3 | -1.01 | 0.32 | [-0.17, 0.33] | 0.02 | 0.13 | 0.17 | 0.87 | [0.07, 0.28] |
有意义语音vs无声 | 0.45 | 0.28 | 1.59 | 0.12 | [0.02, 0.37] | 0.35 | 0.11 | 3.12 | < 0.001 | [0.05, 0.42] |
有意义语音vs无意义语音 | 0.69 | 0.34 | 1.83 | 0.06 | [0.11, 0.62] | 0.30 | 0.15 | 2.08 | 0.04 | [0.19, 0.67] |
表2 实验1句子指标上的线性混合模型分析结果
变量 | 平均注视时间 | 句子阅读时间 | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
β | SE | t | p | 95% CI | β | SE | t | p | 95% CI | |
截距 | 224.03 | 4.32 | 51.91 | < 0.001 | [5.15, 5.43] | 4788.43 | 232.69 | 20.58 | < 0.001 | [8.28, 8.56] |
无意义语音vs无声 | -0.38 | 2.24 | -0.17 | 0.87 | [-0.12, 0.32] | -146.26 | 118.41 | -1.24 | 0.22 | [-0.27, 0.13] |
有意义语音vs无声 | 0.39 | 1.67 | 0.23 | 0.82 | [0.17, 0.42] | 58.82 | 103.85 | 0.57 | 0.57 | [0.26, 0.55] |
有意义语音vs无意义语音 | 0.56 | 1.9 | 0.29 | 0.77 | [0.23, 0.58] | 190 | 124.65 | 1.52 | 0.13 | [0.15, 0.46] |
变量 | 句子注视次数 | 回视次数 | ||||||||
β | SE | t | p | 95% CI | β | SE | t | p | 95% CI | |
截距 | 12.66 | 0.48 | 26.12 | < 0.001 | [2.19, 2.54] | 3.28 | 0.18 | 18.37 | < 0.001 | [3.47, 3.86] |
无意义语音vs无声 | -0.31 | 0.3 | -1.01 | 0.32 | [-0.17, 0.33] | 0.02 | 0.13 | 0.17 | 0.87 | [0.07, 0.28] |
有意义语音vs无声 | 0.45 | 0.28 | 1.59 | 0.12 | [0.02, 0.37] | 0.35 | 0.11 | 3.12 | < 0.001 | [0.05, 0.42] |
有意义语音vs无意义语音 | 0.69 | 0.34 | 1.83 | 0.06 | [0.11, 0.62] | 0.30 | 0.15 | 2.08 | 0.04 | [0.19, 0.67] |
背景音条件 | 首次注视时间(ms) | 凝视时间(ms) | 回视路径时间(ms) | 总注视时间(ms) |
---|---|---|---|---|
无声 | 244 (3) | 265 (4) | 331 (9) | 352 (7) |
无意义语音 | 242 (3) | 264 (4) | 328 (9) | 349 (7) |
有意义语音 | 240 (3) | 266 (5) | 336 (10) | 357 (7) |
表3 局部分析各眼动指标的平均数与标准误
背景音条件 | 首次注视时间(ms) | 凝视时间(ms) | 回视路径时间(ms) | 总注视时间(ms) |
---|---|---|---|---|
无声 | 244 (3) | 265 (4) | 331 (9) | 352 (7) |
无意义语音 | 242 (3) | 264 (4) | 328 (9) | 349 (7) |
有意义语音 | 240 (3) | 266 (5) | 336 (10) | 357 (7) |
变量 | 首次注视时间 | 凝视时间 | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
β | SE | t | p | 95% CI | β | SE | t | p | 95% CI | |
截距 | 237.15 | 5.47 | 43.36 | < 0.001 | [5.37, 5.61] | 256.56 | 8.12 | 31.6 | < 0.001 | [5.25, 5.72] |
无意义语音vs无声 | -1.21 | 4.44 | -0.27 | 0.79 | [-0.13, 0.23] | -1.09 | 6.05 | -0.18 | 0.86 | [-0.14, 0.29] |
有意义语音vs无声 | -4.31 | 4.46 | -0.97 | 0.34 | [-0.05, 0.41] | -1.79 | 7.11 | -0.25 | 0.8 | [-0.16, 0.32] |
有意义语音vs无意义语音 | -3.52 | 4.3 | -0.82 | 0.42 | [-0.07, 0.50] | -3.82 | 5.77 | -0.66 | 0.51 | [-0.23, 0.47] |
变量 | 回视路径时间 | 总注视时间 | ||||||||
β | SE | t | p | 95% CI | β | SE | t | p | 95% CI | |
截距 | 323.66 | 12.45 | 26.01 | < 0.001 | [5.33, 5.68] | 343.01 | 12.16 | 28.21 | < 0.001 | [5.42, 5.74] |
无意义语音vs无声 | 0.46 | 12.83 | 0.04 | 0.97 | [0.26, 0.44] | -4.04 | 11.4 | -0.35 | 0.72 | [-0.09, 0.32] |
有意义语音vs无声 | 1.94 | 14.81 | 0.13 | 0.90 | [0.11, 0.34] | 2.89 | 9.98 | 0.29 | 0.77 | [0.23, 0.55] |
有意义语音vs无意义语音 | 2.04 | 15.54 | 0.13 | 0.91 | [0.16, 0.57] | 8.25 | 13.85 | 0.60 | 0.55 | [0.21, 0.72] |
表4 实验1目标词上的线性混合模型分析结果
变量 | 首次注视时间 | 凝视时间 | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
β | SE | t | p | 95% CI | β | SE | t | p | 95% CI | |
截距 | 237.15 | 5.47 | 43.36 | < 0.001 | [5.37, 5.61] | 256.56 | 8.12 | 31.6 | < 0.001 | [5.25, 5.72] |
无意义语音vs无声 | -1.21 | 4.44 | -0.27 | 0.79 | [-0.13, 0.23] | -1.09 | 6.05 | -0.18 | 0.86 | [-0.14, 0.29] |
有意义语音vs无声 | -4.31 | 4.46 | -0.97 | 0.34 | [-0.05, 0.41] | -1.79 | 7.11 | -0.25 | 0.8 | [-0.16, 0.32] |
有意义语音vs无意义语音 | -3.52 | 4.3 | -0.82 | 0.42 | [-0.07, 0.50] | -3.82 | 5.77 | -0.66 | 0.51 | [-0.23, 0.47] |
变量 | 回视路径时间 | 总注视时间 | ||||||||
β | SE | t | p | 95% CI | β | SE | t | p | 95% CI | |
截距 | 323.66 | 12.45 | 26.01 | < 0.001 | [5.33, 5.68] | 343.01 | 12.16 | 28.21 | < 0.001 | [5.42, 5.74] |
无意义语音vs无声 | 0.46 | 12.83 | 0.04 | 0.97 | [0.26, 0.44] | -4.04 | 11.4 | -0.35 | 0.72 | [-0.09, 0.32] |
有意义语音vs无声 | 1.94 | 14.81 | 0.13 | 0.90 | [0.11, 0.34] | 2.89 | 9.98 | 0.29 | 0.77 | [0.23, 0.55] |
有意义语音vs无意义语音 | 2.04 | 15.54 | 0.13 | 0.91 | [0.16, 0.57] | 8.25 | 13.85 | 0.60 | 0.55 | [0.21, 0.72] |
背景音条件 | 平均注视时间(ms) | 句子阅读时间(ms) | 句子注视次数 | 回视次数 |
---|---|---|---|---|
无声 | 262 (1.17) | 11063 (188) | 32.98 (0.55) | 9.72 (0.21) |
无意义语音 | 260 (1.12) | 10755 (171) | 31.99 (0.48) | 9.05 (0.17) |
有意义语音 | 266 (1.21) | 13664 (228) | 40.68 (0.67) | 12.86 (0.27) |
表5 整体分析各眼动指标的平均数与标准误
背景音条件 | 平均注视时间(ms) | 句子阅读时间(ms) | 句子注视次数 | 回视次数 |
---|---|---|---|---|
无声 | 262 (1.17) | 11063 (188) | 32.98 (0.55) | 9.72 (0.21) |
无意义语音 | 260 (1.12) | 10755 (171) | 31.99 (0.48) | 9.05 (0.17) |
有意义语音 | 266 (1.21) | 13664 (228) | 40.68 (0.67) | 12.86 (0.27) |
变量 | 平均注视时间 | 句子阅读时间 | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
β | SE | t | p | 95% CI | β | SE | t | p | 95% CI | |
截距 | 262.59 | 4.67 | 56.21 | < 0.001 | [6.21, 6.57] | 8877.49 | 754.56 | 15.74 | < 0.001 | [7.17, 7.55] |
无意义语音vs无声 | -2.20 | 1.33 | -1.66 | 0.10 | [-0.61, -0.28] | -332.92 | 333.05 | -1.00 | 0.32 | [-0.79, -0.41] |
有意义语音vs无声 | 4.06 | 2.08 | 1.96 | 0.05 | [0.51, 0.59] | 2661.43 | 511.56 | 5.20 | < 0.001 | [0.21, 0.62] |
有意义语音vs无意义语音 | 6.26 | 2.04 | 3.06 | 0.003 | [0.12, 0.60] | 2999.39 | 403.68 | 7.43 | < 0.001 | [0.29, 0.75] |
变量 | 句子注视次数 | 回视次数 | ||||||||
β | SE | t | p | 95% CI | β | SE | t | p | 95% CI | |
截距 | 35.37 | 2.17 | 16.28 | < 0.001 | [4.16, 4.65] | 10.60 | 0.78 | 13.64 | < 0.001 | [4.21, 4.68] |
无意义语音vs无声 | -1.07 | 1.05 | -1.02 | 0.31 | [-0.69, -0.35] | -0.67 | 0.46 | -1.44 | 0.15 | [-0.51, -0.17] |
有意义语音vs无声 | 7.86 | 1.51 | 5.20 | < 0.001 | [0.17, 0.61] | 3.22 | 0.62 | 5.21 | < 0.001 | [0.24, 0.63] |
有意义语音vs无意义语音 | 8.91 | 1.21 | 7.38 | < 0.001 | [0.27, 0.77] | 3.87 | 0.56 | 6.91 | < 0.001 | [0.15, 0.68] |
表6 实验2句子指标的线性混合模型分析结果
变量 | 平均注视时间 | 句子阅读时间 | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
β | SE | t | p | 95% CI | β | SE | t | p | 95% CI | |
截距 | 262.59 | 4.67 | 56.21 | < 0.001 | [6.21, 6.57] | 8877.49 | 754.56 | 15.74 | < 0.001 | [7.17, 7.55] |
无意义语音vs无声 | -2.20 | 1.33 | -1.66 | 0.10 | [-0.61, -0.28] | -332.92 | 333.05 | -1.00 | 0.32 | [-0.79, -0.41] |
有意义语音vs无声 | 4.06 | 2.08 | 1.96 | 0.05 | [0.51, 0.59] | 2661.43 | 511.56 | 5.20 | < 0.001 | [0.21, 0.62] |
有意义语音vs无意义语音 | 6.26 | 2.04 | 3.06 | 0.003 | [0.12, 0.60] | 2999.39 | 403.68 | 7.43 | < 0.001 | [0.29, 0.75] |
变量 | 句子注视次数 | 回视次数 | ||||||||
β | SE | t | p | 95% CI | β | SE | t | p | 95% CI | |
截距 | 35.37 | 2.17 | 16.28 | < 0.001 | [4.16, 4.65] | 10.60 | 0.78 | 13.64 | < 0.001 | [4.21, 4.68] |
无意义语音vs无声 | -1.07 | 1.05 | -1.02 | 0.31 | [-0.69, -0.35] | -0.67 | 0.46 | -1.44 | 0.15 | [-0.51, -0.17] |
有意义语音vs无声 | 7.86 | 1.51 | 5.20 | < 0.001 | [0.17, 0.61] | 3.22 | 0.62 | 5.21 | < 0.001 | [0.24, 0.63] |
有意义语音vs无意义语音 | 8.91 | 1.21 | 7.38 | < 0.001 | [0.27, 0.77] | 3.87 | 0.56 | 6.91 | < 0.001 | [0.15, 0.68] |
背景音条件 | 首次注视时间(ms) | 凝视时间(ms) | 回视路径时间(ms) | 总注视时间(ms) |
---|---|---|---|---|
无声 | 263 (1.17) | 311 (1.96) | 754 (11.69) | 541 (4.21) |
无意义语音 | 259 (1.16) | 300 (1.82) | 761 (11.90) | 520 (3.93) |
有意义语音 | 266 (1.29) | 315 (2.22) | 1025 (17.61) | 669 (5.43) |
表7 局部分析各眼动指标的平均数与标准误
背景音条件 | 首次注视时间(ms) | 凝视时间(ms) | 回视路径时间(ms) | 总注视时间(ms) |
---|---|---|---|---|
无声 | 263 (1.17) | 311 (1.96) | 754 (11.69) | 541 (4.21) |
无意义语音 | 259 (1.16) | 300 (1.82) | 761 (11.90) | 520 (3.93) |
有意义语音 | 266 (1.29) | 315 (2.22) | 1025 (17.61) | 669 (5.43) |
变量 | 首次注视时间 | 凝视时间 | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
β | SE | t | p | 95% CI | β | SE | t | p | 95% CI | |
截距 | 260.32 | 5.08 | 51.21 | < 0.001 | [4.34, 4.65] | 302.86 | 9.44 | 32.08 | < 0.001 | [4.27, 4.59] |
无意义语音vs无声 | -1.95 | 2.62 | -0.75 | 0.46 | [-0.63, -0.27] | -4.56 | 5.14 | -0.89 | 0.38 | [-0.58, -0.22] |
有意义语音vs无声 | 2.18 | 3.41 | 0.64 | 0.52 | [0.19, 0.56] | 5.71 | 5.21 | 1.10 | 0.28 | [0.18, 0.53] |
有意义语音vs无意义语音 | 4.14 | 3.40 | 1.22 | 0.23 | [0.17, 0.58] | 10.27 | 6.44 | 1.60 | 0.12 | [0.31, 0.66] |
变量 | 回视路径时间 | 总注视时间 | ||||||||
β | SE | t | p | 95% CI | β | SE | t | p | 95% CI | |
截距 | 898.64 | 52.56 | 17.10 | < 0.001 | [5.22, 5.53] | 571.76 | 36.12 | 15.83 | < 0.001 | [5.17, 5.45] |
无意义语音vs无声 | 11.94 | 34.10 | 0.35 | 0.73 | [0.19, 0.64] | -21.16 | 17.32 | -1.22 | 0.23 | [-0.43, -0.11] |
有意义语音vs无声 | 298.03 | 44.10 | 6.55 | < 0.001 | [0.11, 0.50] | 125.60 | 24.70 | 5.09 | < 0.001 | [0.15, 0.51] |
有意义语音vs无意义语音 | 275.97 | 47.68 | 5.79 | < 0.001 | [0.18, 0.62] | 146.75 | 21.01 | 6.99 | < 0.001 | [0.31, 0.82] |
表8 实验2目标词的线性混合模型分析结果
变量 | 首次注视时间 | 凝视时间 | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
β | SE | t | p | 95% CI | β | SE | t | p | 95% CI | |
截距 | 260.32 | 5.08 | 51.21 | < 0.001 | [4.34, 4.65] | 302.86 | 9.44 | 32.08 | < 0.001 | [4.27, 4.59] |
无意义语音vs无声 | -1.95 | 2.62 | -0.75 | 0.46 | [-0.63, -0.27] | -4.56 | 5.14 | -0.89 | 0.38 | [-0.58, -0.22] |
有意义语音vs无声 | 2.18 | 3.41 | 0.64 | 0.52 | [0.19, 0.56] | 5.71 | 5.21 | 1.10 | 0.28 | [0.18, 0.53] |
有意义语音vs无意义语音 | 4.14 | 3.40 | 1.22 | 0.23 | [0.17, 0.58] | 10.27 | 6.44 | 1.60 | 0.12 | [0.31, 0.66] |
变量 | 回视路径时间 | 总注视时间 | ||||||||
β | SE | t | p | 95% CI | β | SE | t | p | 95% CI | |
截距 | 898.64 | 52.56 | 17.10 | < 0.001 | [5.22, 5.53] | 571.76 | 36.12 | 15.83 | < 0.001 | [5.17, 5.45] |
无意义语音vs无声 | 11.94 | 34.10 | 0.35 | 0.73 | [0.19, 0.64] | -21.16 | 17.32 | -1.22 | 0.23 | [-0.43, -0.11] |
有意义语音vs无声 | 298.03 | 44.10 | 6.55 | < 0.001 | [0.11, 0.50] | 125.60 | 24.70 | 5.09 | < 0.001 | [0.15, 0.51] |
有意义语音vs无意义语音 | 275.97 | 47.68 | 5.79 | < 0.001 | [0.18, 0.62] | 146.75 | 21.01 | 6.99 | < 0.001 | [0.31, 0.82] |
背景音条件 | 平均注视时间(ms) | 篇章阅读时间(ms) | 篇章注视次数 | 回视次数 | 阅读速度(字/分钟) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
无声 | 224 (2) | 48519 (1452) | 153.03 (4.3) | 41.78 (1.61) | 408 (13) |
无意义语音 | 224 (2) | 50241 (1496) | 156.98 (4.43) | 44.63 (1.64) | 387 (11) |
有意义语音 | 225 (2) | 51687 (1516) | 160.69 (4.41) | 45.72 (1.71) | 380 (11) |
表9 整体分析各眼动指标的平均数与标准误
背景音条件 | 平均注视时间(ms) | 篇章阅读时间(ms) | 篇章注视次数 | 回视次数 | 阅读速度(字/分钟) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
无声 | 224 (2) | 48519 (1452) | 153.03 (4.3) | 41.78 (1.61) | 408 (13) |
无意义语音 | 224 (2) | 50241 (1496) | 156.98 (4.43) | 44.63 (1.64) | 387 (11) |
有意义语音 | 225 (2) | 51687 (1516) | 160.69 (4.41) | 45.72 (1.71) | 380 (11) |
变量 | 平均注视时间 | 篇章阅读时间 | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
β | SE | t | p | 95% CI | β | SE | t | p | 95% CI | ||
截距 | 224.48 | 4.5 | 49.91 | < 0.001 | [4.18, 4.56] | 20101.93 | 2762.11 | 18.14 | < 0.001 | [16.26, 16.75] | |
无意义语音vs无声 | 0.38 | 1.36 | 0.28 | 0.79 | [0.29, 0.54] | 1945.7 | 1512.15 | 1.29 | 0.22 | [0.24, 0.61] | |
有意义语音vs无声 | 1.41 | 1.57 | 0.90 | 0.38 | [0.26, 0.67] | 3309.55 | 1334.74 | 2.48 | 0.02 | [0.31, 0.74] | |
有意义语音vs无意义语音 | 1.03 | 1.3 | 0.79 | 0.44 | [0.14, 0.62] | 1292.62 | 1600.98 | 0.81 | 0.43 | [0.25, 0.81] | |
变量 | 篇章注视次数 | 回视次数 | |||||||||
β | SE | t | 95% CI | β | SE | t | p | 95% CI | |||
截距 | 156.73 | 8.08 | 19.4 | < 0.001 | [6.52, 6.81] | 44.03 | 3.18 | 13.85 | < 0.001 | [6.12, 6.51] | |
无意义语音vs无声 | 4.55 | 4.9 | 0.93 | 0.37 | [0.16, 0.53] | 3.05 | 1.71 | 1.79 | 0.10 | [0.18, 0.47] | |
有意义语音vs无声 | 7.97 | 4.34 | 1.84 | 0.08 | [0.19, 0.65] | 3.98 | 1.80 | 2.22 | 0.05 | [0.15, 0.62] | |
有意义语音vs无意义语音 | 3.08 | 5.33 | 0.58 | 0.57 | [0.21, 0.77] | 0.82 | 1.89 | 0.44 | 0.67 | [0.28, 0.73] | |
变量 | 阅读速度 | ||||||||||
β | SE | t | p | 95% CI | |||||||
截距 | 392.8 | 23.39 | 16.79 | < 0.001 | [4.28, 4.86] | ||||||
无意义语音vs无声 | -22.59 | 10.89 | -2.07 | 0.06 | [-0.72, -0.45] | ||||||
有意义语音vs无声 | -29.42 | 9.8 | -3.00 | 0.01 | [-0.12, 0.38] | ||||||
有意义语音vs无意义语音 | -6.43 | 9.36 | -0.69 | 0.50 | [0.21, 0.56] |
表10 实验3篇章指标的线性混合模型分析结果
变量 | 平均注视时间 | 篇章阅读时间 | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
β | SE | t | p | 95% CI | β | SE | t | p | 95% CI | ||
截距 | 224.48 | 4.5 | 49.91 | < 0.001 | [4.18, 4.56] | 20101.93 | 2762.11 | 18.14 | < 0.001 | [16.26, 16.75] | |
无意义语音vs无声 | 0.38 | 1.36 | 0.28 | 0.79 | [0.29, 0.54] | 1945.7 | 1512.15 | 1.29 | 0.22 | [0.24, 0.61] | |
有意义语音vs无声 | 1.41 | 1.57 | 0.90 | 0.38 | [0.26, 0.67] | 3309.55 | 1334.74 | 2.48 | 0.02 | [0.31, 0.74] | |
有意义语音vs无意义语音 | 1.03 | 1.3 | 0.79 | 0.44 | [0.14, 0.62] | 1292.62 | 1600.98 | 0.81 | 0.43 | [0.25, 0.81] | |
变量 | 篇章注视次数 | 回视次数 | |||||||||
β | SE | t | 95% CI | β | SE | t | p | 95% CI | |||
截距 | 156.73 | 8.08 | 19.4 | < 0.001 | [6.52, 6.81] | 44.03 | 3.18 | 13.85 | < 0.001 | [6.12, 6.51] | |
无意义语音vs无声 | 4.55 | 4.9 | 0.93 | 0.37 | [0.16, 0.53] | 3.05 | 1.71 | 1.79 | 0.10 | [0.18, 0.47] | |
有意义语音vs无声 | 7.97 | 4.34 | 1.84 | 0.08 | [0.19, 0.65] | 3.98 | 1.80 | 2.22 | 0.05 | [0.15, 0.62] | |
有意义语音vs无意义语音 | 3.08 | 5.33 | 0.58 | 0.57 | [0.21, 0.77] | 0.82 | 1.89 | 0.44 | 0.67 | [0.28, 0.73] | |
变量 | 阅读速度 | ||||||||||
β | SE | t | p | 95% CI | |||||||
截距 | 392.8 | 23.39 | 16.79 | < 0.001 | [4.28, 4.86] | ||||||
无意义语音vs无声 | -22.59 | 10.89 | -2.07 | 0.06 | [-0.72, -0.45] | ||||||
有意义语音vs无声 | -29.42 | 9.8 | -3.00 | 0.01 | [-0.12, 0.38] | ||||||
有意义语音vs无意义语音 | -6.43 | 9.36 | -0.69 | 0.50 | [0.21, 0.56] |
背景音条件 | 首次注视时间(ms) | 凝视时间(ms) | 回视路径时间(ms) | 总注视时间(ms) |
---|---|---|---|---|
无声 | 231 (2) | 250 (2) | 397 (7) | 255 (3) |
无意义语音 | 227 (2) | 244 (2) | 413 (8) | 261 (4) |
有意义语音 | 228 (1) | 246 (2) | 440 (9) | 276 (4) |
表11 局部分析各眼动指标的平均数与标准误
背景音条件 | 首次注视时间(ms) | 凝视时间(ms) | 回视路径时间(ms) | 总注视时间(ms) |
---|---|---|---|---|
无声 | 231 (2) | 250 (2) | 397 (7) | 255 (3) |
无意义语音 | 227 (2) | 244 (2) | 413 (8) | 261 (4) |
有意义语音 | 228 (1) | 246 (2) | 440 (9) | 276 (4) |
变量 | 首次注视时间 | 凝视时间 | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
β | SE | t | p | 95% CI | β | SE | t | p | 95% CI | |
截距 | 225.57 | 4.6 | 49.03 | < 0.001 | [5.48, 5.75] | 241.17 | 5.63 | 42.83 | < 0.001 | [5.29, 5.73] |
无意义语音vs无声 | -3.28 | 3.67 | -0.90 | 0.40 | [-0.07, 0.26] | -3.24 | 4.87 | -0.67 | 0.52 | [-0.16, 0.28] |
有意义语音vs无声 | -1.58 | 2.86 | -0.55 | 0.59 | [-0.19, 0.34] | -1.24 | 3.48 | -0.36 | 0.73 | [-0.08, 0.43] |
有意义语音vs无意义语音 | 1.19 | 2.64 | 0.45 | 0.66 | [0.15, 0.57] | 1.38 | 3.11 | 0.45 | 0.66 | [0.19, 0.65] |
变量 | 回视路径时间 | 总注视时间 | ||||||||
β | SE | t | p | 95% CI | β | SE | t | p | 95% CI | |
截距 | 410.01 | 18.11 | 22.64 | < 0.001 | [6.16, 6.49] | 261.31 | 15.23 | 17.16 | < 0.001 | [5.28.5.65] |
无意义语音vs无声 | 24.22 | 18.06 | 1.34 | 0.19 | [0.31, 0.63] | 7.93 | 8.88 | 0.89 | 0.38 | [0.25, 0.58] |
有意义语音vs无声 | 41.11 | 17.77 | 2.31 | 0.03 | [0.04, 0.52] | 22.53 | 9.47 | 2.38 | 0.02 | [0.21, 0.66] |
有意义语音vs无意义语音 | 28.32 | 21.09 | 1.36 | 0.18 | [0.16, 0.77] | 15.05 | 10.59 | 1.42 | 0.16 | [0.14, 0.62] |
表12 实验3目标词的线性混合模型分析结果
变量 | 首次注视时间 | 凝视时间 | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
β | SE | t | p | 95% CI | β | SE | t | p | 95% CI | |
截距 | 225.57 | 4.6 | 49.03 | < 0.001 | [5.48, 5.75] | 241.17 | 5.63 | 42.83 | < 0.001 | [5.29, 5.73] |
无意义语音vs无声 | -3.28 | 3.67 | -0.90 | 0.40 | [-0.07, 0.26] | -3.24 | 4.87 | -0.67 | 0.52 | [-0.16, 0.28] |
有意义语音vs无声 | -1.58 | 2.86 | -0.55 | 0.59 | [-0.19, 0.34] | -1.24 | 3.48 | -0.36 | 0.73 | [-0.08, 0.43] |
有意义语音vs无意义语音 | 1.19 | 2.64 | 0.45 | 0.66 | [0.15, 0.57] | 1.38 | 3.11 | 0.45 | 0.66 | [0.19, 0.65] |
变量 | 回视路径时间 | 总注视时间 | ||||||||
β | SE | t | p | 95% CI | β | SE | t | p | 95% CI | |
截距 | 410.01 | 18.11 | 22.64 | < 0.001 | [6.16, 6.49] | 261.31 | 15.23 | 17.16 | < 0.001 | [5.28.5.65] |
无意义语音vs无声 | 24.22 | 18.06 | 1.34 | 0.19 | [0.31, 0.63] | 7.93 | 8.88 | 0.89 | 0.38 | [0.25, 0.58] |
有意义语音vs无声 | 41.11 | 17.77 | 2.31 | 0.03 | [0.04, 0.52] | 22.53 | 9.47 | 2.38 | 0.02 | [0.21, 0.66] |
有意义语音vs无意义语音 | 28.32 | 21.09 | 1.36 | 0.18 | [0.16, 0.77] | 15.05 | 10.59 | 1.42 | 0.16 | [0.14, 0.62] |
[1] |
Armstrong, G. B., Boiarsky, G. A., & Mares, M.-L. (1991). Background television and reading performance. Communications Monographs, 58(3), 235-253.
doi: 10.1080/03637759109376228 URL |
[2] | Bai, X. J., & Yan, G. L. (2017). Psychology of reading. Shanghai, China: East China Normal University Press. |
[白学军, 闫国利. (2017). 阅读心理学. 上海: 华东师范大学出版社.] | |
[3] |
Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(4), 390-412.
doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.12.005 URL |
[4] | Baddeley, A. D. (2007). Working memory, thought, and action. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. |
[5] | Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J. (1974). Working memory. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 8, 47-89. |
[6] |
Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J. (1994). Developments in the concept of working memory. Neuropsychology, 8(4), 485-493.
doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.8.4.485 URL |
[7] |
Baker, R. W., & Madell, T. O. (1965). A continued investigation of susceptibility to distraction in academically underachieving and achieving male college students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 56(5), 254-258.
pmid: 5825844 |
[8] | Bates, D., Maechler, M., & Bolker, B. (2011). LME4: Linear mixed-effects models using S4 classes. R Package Version 0.999375-39. Retrieved from http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lme4 |
[9] |
Boyle, R., & Coltheart, V. (1996). Effects of irrelevant sounds on phonological coding in reading comprehension and short-term memory. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, Section A: Human Experimental Psychology, 49(2), 398-416.
doi: 10.1080/713755630 URL |
[10] |
Cauchard, F., Cane, J. E., & Weger, U. W. (2012). Influence of background speech and music in interrupted reading: An eye-tracking study. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 26(3), 381-390.
doi: 10.1002/acp.v26.3 URL |
[11] |
Franconeri, S. L., Alvarez, G. A., & Cavanagh, P. (2013). Flexible cognitive resources: Competitive content maps for attention and memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(3), 134-141.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.01.010 URL |
[12] |
Gao, Q., & Bai, X. J. (2018). The influence of Chinese and English background pop music to the memory of Chinese and English words in Chinese undergraduates. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 50(1), 1-8.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2018.00001 URL |
[高淇, 白学军. (2018). 中英流行背景音乐对大学生中英词汇记忆的影响. 心理学报, 50(1), 1-8.] | |
[13] |
Garcia-Madruga, J. A., Elosua, M. R., Gil, L., Gomez-Veiga, I., Vila, J. O., Orjales, I., ... Duque, G. (2013). Reading comprehension and working memory's executive processes: An intervention study in primary school students. Reading Research Quarterly, 48(2), 155-174.
doi: 10.1002/rrq.2013.48.issue-2 URL |
[14] | Gernsbacher, M. A., & Foertsch, J. A. (2000). Three models of discourse comprehension. In S. Garrod & M. J. Pickering (Eds.), Language processing (pp.283-299). East Sussex, UK: Psychology Press. |
[15] |
Haapakangas, A., Kankkunen, E., Hongisto, V., Virjonen, P., Oliva, D., & Keskinen, E. (2011). Effects of five speech masking sounds on performance and acoustic satisfaction. Implications for open-plan offices. Acta Acustica United With Acustica, 97, 641-655.
doi: 10.3813/AAA.918444 URL |
[16] |
Haka, M., Haapakangas, A., Keränen, J., Hakala, J., Keskinen, E., & Hongisto, V. (2009). Performance effects and subjective disturbance of speech in acoustically different office types—A laboratory experiment. Indoor Air, 19(6), 454-467.
doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0668.2009.00608.x pmid: 19702627 |
[17] | Halin, N. (2016). Distracted while reading? Changing to a hard-to-read font shields against the effects of environmental noise and speech on text memory. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, Article 1196. |
[18] |
Halin, N., Marsh, J. E., Haga, A., Holmgren, M., & Sörqvist, P. (2014). Effects of speech on proofreading: Can task-engagement manipulations shield against distraction? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 20(1), 69-80.
doi: 10.1037/xap0000002 URL |
[19] |
Halin, N., Marsh, J. E., Hellman, A., Hellström, I., & Sörqvist, P. (2014). A shield against distraction. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 3(1), 31-36.
doi: 10.1016/j.jarmac.2014.01.003 URL |
[20] | He, L. Y., Huang, Y. Y., Wang, M. X., Meng, Z., & Yan, G. L. (2015). The effects of background noise on Chinese passage reading: An eye movement study. Journal of Psychological Science, 38(6), 1290-1295. |
[何立媛, 黄有玉, 王梦轩, 孟珠, 闫国利. (2015). 不同背景音对中文篇章阅读影响的眼动研究. 心理科学, 38(6), 1290-1295.] | |
[21] | Hyönä, J., & Ekholm, M. (2016). Background speech effects on sentence processing during reading: An eye movement study. PloS One, 11(3), Article e0152133. |
[22] |
Jahncke, H., Hygge, S., Halin, N., Green, A. M., & Dimberg, K. (2011). Open-plan office noise: Cognitive performance and restoration. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 31(4), 373-382.
doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2011.07.002 URL |
[23] |
Jones, D., Madden, C., & Miles, C. (1992). Privileged access by irrelevant speech to short-term memory: The role of changing state. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 44(4), 645-669.
doi: 10.1080/14640749208401304 URL |
[24] | Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort (Vol. 1063). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. |
[25] | Kehler, A. (2004). Discourse coherence. In L. R. Horn & G. Ward (Eds.), The handbook of pragmatics (pp.241-265). Oxford, UK: Blackwell Publishing. |
[26] | Li, X., Bai X. J., & Yan G. L. (2011). The role of word boundary and frequency during Chinese reading. Studies of Psychology and Behavior, 9(2), 133-139. |
[李馨, 白学军, 闫国利. (2011). 词边界信息和词频在汉语阅读中的作用. 心理与行为研究, 9(2), 133-139.] | |
[27] | Ma, X., Liu, J., Liu, Y., Tao, Y., Zhang, Q. Y., & Chen, R. (2015). Effect of background music on Chinese and English reading comprehension. Studies of Psychology and Behavior, 13(4), 472-478. |
[马谐, 刘佳, 刘艳, 陶云, 张秋月, 陈睿. (2015). 背景音乐对中—英文阅读理解的影响效应. 心理与行为研究, 13(4), 472-478.] | |
[28] |
Marsh, J. E., Hughes, R. W., & Jones, D. M. (2008). Auditory distraction in semantic memory: A process-based approach. Journal of Memory and Language, 58(3), 682-700.
doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.05.002 URL |
[29] |
Marsh, J. E., Hughes, R. W., & Jones, D. M. (2009). Interference by process, not content, determines semantic auditory distraction. Cognition, 110(1), 23-38.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2008.08.003 pmid: 19081558 |
[30] | Marsh, J. E., Perham, N., Sörqvist, P., & Jones, D. M. (2014). Boundaries of semantic distraction: Dominance and lexicality act at retrieval. Memory & Cognition, 42(8), 1285-1301. |
[31] |
Martin, R. C., Wogalter, M. S., & Forlano, J. G. (1988). Reading comprehension in the presence of unattended speech and music. Journal of Memory and Language, 27(4), 382-398.
doi: 10.1016/0749-596X(88)90063-0 URL |
[32] | Meng, Z., Lan, Z., Yan, G., Marsh, J. E., Liversedge, S. P. (2020). Task demands modulate the effects of speech on text processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 46(10), 1892-1905. |
[33] |
Meng, Z., & Yan, G. L. (2018). Mechanism of the irrelevant speech effect in reading: Is the interference determined by content, or process?. Advances in Psychological Science, 26(2), 262-269.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2018.00262 URL |
[孟珠, 闫国利. (2018). 阅读任务中无关言语效应的作用机制: 干扰基于内容还是过程? 心理科学进展, 26(2), 262-269.] | |
[34] | O’Brien, E. J., & Cook, A. E. (2015). Models of discourse comprehension. In A. Pollatsek & R. Treiman (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of reading (pp.217-231). New York, USA: Oxford University Press. |
[35] |
Oswald, C. J. P., Tremblay, S., & Jones, D. M. (2000). Disruption of comprehension by the meaning of irrelevant sound. Memory, 8(5), 345-350.
pmid: 11045242 |
[36] | Peng, D. L. (2004). Cognitive processing of the Chinese language and its neural mechanism. Contemporary Linguistics, 6(4), 302-320. |
[彭聃龄. (2004). 汉语信息加工及其认知神经机制的研究——20年研究工作的回顾. 当代语言学, 6(4), 302-320.] | |
[37] | Rayner, K. (2009). Eye movements and attention in reading, scene perception, and visual search. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62(8), 1457-1506. |
[38] | Rayner, K., Pollatsek, A., Ashby, J., & Clifton, C. J. (2012). Psychology of reading (2nd ed.). New York, USA: Psychology Press. |
[39] | R Development Core Team. (2016). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Retrieved from https://www.R-project.org/ |
[40] |
Robert, G., & Hockey, J. (1997). Compensatory control in the regulation of human performance under stress and high workload: A cognitive-energetical framework. Biological Psychology, 45(1-3),73-93.
doi: 10.1016/S0301-0511(96)05219-2 URL |
[41] |
Salamé, P., & Baddeley, A. (1982). Disruption of short-term memory by unattended speech: Implications for the structure of working memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 21(2), 150-164.
doi: 10.1016/S0022-5371(82)90521-7 URL |
[42] | Salamé, P., & Baddeley, A. (1987). Noise, unattended speech and short-term memory. Ergonomics, 30(8), 1185-1194. |
[43] |
Salamé, P., & Baddeley, A. (1989). Effects of background music on phonological short-term memory. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 41(1), 107-122.
doi: 10.1080/14640748908402355 URL |
[44] |
Ünal, A. B., Steg, L., & Epstude, K. (2012). The influence of music on mental effort and driving performance. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 48(9), 271-278.
doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2012.01.022 URL |
[45] |
Vasilev, M. R., Kirkby, J. A., & Bernhard, A. (2018). Auditory distraction during reading: A Bayesian meta-analysis of a continuing controversy. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 13(5), 567-597.
doi: 10.1177/1745691617747398 pmid: 29958067 |
[46] |
Vasilev, M. R., Liversedge, S. P., Rowan, D., Kirkby, J. A., & Angele, B. (2019). Reading is disrupted by intelligible background speech: Evidence from eye-tracking. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 45(11), 1-93.
doi: 10.1037/xhp0000578 URL |
[47] | Vasilev, M. R., Parmentier, F. B. R., Angele, B., & Kirkby, J. A. (2019). Distraction by deviant sounds during reading: An eye-movement study. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 72(7), 1863-1875. |
[48] | Venetjoki, N., Kaarlela-Tuomaala, A., Keskinen, E., & Hongisto, V. (2006). The effect of speech and speech intelligibility on task performance. Ergonomics, 49(11), 1068-1091. |
[49] |
Wickens, C. (2002). Multiple resources and performance prediction. Theoretical Issues in Ergonomics Science, 3(2), 159-177.
doi: 10.1080/14639220210123806 URL |
[50] |
Wolf, F., & Gibson, E. (2005). Representing discourse coherence: A corpus-based study. Computational Linguistics, 31(2), 249-287.
doi: 10.1162/0891201054223977 URL |
[51] | Yan, G., Meng, Z., Liu, N., He, L., & Paterson, K. B. (2017). Effects of irrelevant background speech on eye movements during reading. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71(6), 1270-1275. |
[52] | Yan, G. L., Fu, G., & Bai., X. J. (2008). The perceptual span and eye movements in reading Chinese materials of different degrees of difficulty. Journal of Psychological Science, 31(6), 1287-1290. |
[闫国利, 伏干, 白学军. (2008). 不同难度阅读材料对阅读知觉广度影响的眼动研究. 心理科学. 31(6), 1287-1290.] | |
[53] |
Yan, G. L., Xiong, J. P., Zang, C. L., Yu, L. L., Cui, l., & Bai, X. J. (2013). Review of eye-movement measures in reading research. Advances in Psychological Science, 21(4), 589-605.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2013.00589 URL |
[闫国利, 熊建萍, 臧传丽, 余莉莉, 崔磊, 白学军. (2013). 阅读研究中的主要眼动指标评述. 心理科学进展, 21(4), 589-605.] |
[1] | 程亚华, 冯瑶, 李宜逊, 马嘉琪, 沈岚岚, 张文建, 伍新春, 冯秋迪. 小学儿童口语词汇知识的发展轨迹及其对阅读能力的预测:一个潜变量增长模型[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(7): 1074-1086. |
[2] | 龙翼婷, 姜英杰, 崔璨, 岳阳. 奖赏预测误差对项目和联结记忆影响的分离:元记忆的作用[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(6): 877-891. |
[3] | 周怡彤, 谢瑞波, 伍新春, 阮世芳, 夏月, 喻艳玲, 王振梁. 小学低年级儿童语音意识和语素意识对阅读理解的影响:阅读流畅性的中介作用[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(6): 930-940. |
[4] | 喻艳玲, 谢瑞波, 伍新春, 夏月, 王振梁, 阮世芳. 小学低年级儿童元语言意识与阅读流畅性的关系:汉字识别和词汇知识的中介效应[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(6): 941-953. |
[5] | 黄元娜, 江程铭, 刘洪志, 李纾. 风险、跨期和空间决策的决策策略共享:眼动和主观判断的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(6): 994-1015. |
[6] | 陈发坤, 陈甜, 蔡文琦, 王小娟, 杨剑峰. 左侧额中回参与汉字视觉空间分析的fNIRS证据[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(5): 685-695. |
[7] | 刘洪志, 杨钘兰, 李秋月, 魏子晗. 跨期决策中的维度差异偏好:眼动证据[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(4): 612-625. |
[8] | 曹海波, 兰泽波, 高峰, 于海涛, 李鹏, 王敬欣. 词素位置概率在中文阅读中的作用:词汇判断和眼动研究[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(2): 159-176. |
[9] | 张慢慢, 胡惠兰, 张志超, 李鑫, 汪强, 白学军, 臧传丽. 预测性对快速读者和慢速读者词汇加工的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(1): 79-93. |
[10] | 卢张龙, 刘梦娜, 刘玉洁, 马盼盼, 张瑞平. 内隐序列学习表征机制探究:眼动证据[J]. 心理学报, 2022, 54(7): 779-788. |
[11] | 鹿子佳, 符颖, 张慢慢, 臧传丽, 白学军. 中文词类信息在副中央凹中的加工[J]. 心理学报, 2022, 54(5): 441-452. |
[12] | 刘洪志, 李兴珊, 李纾, 饶俪琳. 基于期望值最大化的理论何时失效:风险决策中为自己-为所有人决策差异的眼动研究[J]. 心理学报, 2022, 54(12): 1517-1531. |
[13] | 唐晓雨, 崔鑫忠, 高敏, 袁梦莹. 视听时、空一致性对Pip-and-Pop效应的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2022, 54(11): 1310-1324. |
[14] | 詹沛达. 引入眼动注视点的联合-交叉负载多模态认知诊断建模[J]. 心理学报, 2022, 54(11): 1416-1423. |
[15] | 张瑞, 王振华, 王小娟, 杨剑峰. 汉字识别中亚词汇语音和语义信息在N170上的神经适应[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(8): 807-820. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||