[1] |
Abbas, Z.-A., & Duchaine, B. (2008). The role of holistic processing in judgments of facial attractiveness. Perception, 37(8), 1187-1196.
|
[2] |
Alvarez, G. A. (2011). Representing multiple objects as an ensemble enhances visual cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(3), 122-131.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.01.003
URL
|
[3] |
Alvarez, G. A., & Oliva, A. (2008). The representation of simple ensemble visual features outside the focus of attention. Psychological Science, 19(4), 392-398.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02098.x
pmid: 18399893
|
[4] |
Anderson, N. H. (1965). Averaging versus adding as a stimulus-combination rule in impression formation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70(4), 394-400.
pmid: 5826027
|
[5] |
Anderson, N. H., Lindner, R., & Lopes, L. L. (1973). Integration theory applied to judgments of group attractiveness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 26(3), 400-408.
pmid: 4710110
|
[6] |
Ariely, D. (2001). Seeing sets: Representation by statistical properties. Psychological Science, 12(2), 157-162.
pmid: 11340926
|
[7] |
Bauer, B. (2009). Does Stevens’s power law for brightness extend to perceptual brightness averaging? Psychological Record, 59(2), 171-185.
doi: 10.1007/BF03395657
URL
|
[8] |
Bauer, B. (2017). Perceptual averaging of line length: Effects of concurrent digit memory load. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 79(8), 2510-2522.
|
[9] |
Brady, T. F., & Alvarez, G. A. (2015). No evidence for a fixed object limit in working memory: Spatial ensemble representations inflate estimates of working memory capacity for complex objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 41(3), 921-929.
doi: 10.1037/xlm0000075
URL
|
[10] |
Carragher, D. J., Lawrence, B. J., Thomas, N. A., & Nicholls, M. E. R. (2018). Visuospatial asymmetries do not modulate the cheerleader effect. Scientific Reports, 8(1), 2548.
doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-20784-5
URL
|
[11] |
de Fockert, J.W., & Marchant, A. P. (2008). Attention modulates set representation by statistical properties. Perception & Psychophysics, 70(5), 789-794.
doi: 10.3758/PP.70.5.789
URL
|
[12] |
Haberman, J., Brady, T. F., & Alvarez, G. A. (2015). Individual differences in ensemble perception reveal multiple, independent levels of ensemble representation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144(2), 432-446.
doi: 10.1037/xge0000053
URL
|
[13] |
Haberman, J., & Whitney, D. (2007). Rapid extraction of mean emotion and gender from sets of faces. Current Biology, 17(17), R751-R753.
|
[14] |
Haberman, J., & Whitney, D. (2009). Seeing the mean: Ensemble coding for sets of faces. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 35(3), 718-734.
|
[15] |
Haberman, J., & Whitney, D. (2012). Ensemble perception: Summarizing the scene and broadening the limits of visual processing. In J. Wolfe & L. Robertson (Eds.), Oxford series in visual cognition. From perception to consciousness: Searching with Anne Treisman (pp. 339-349). Oxford University Press.
|
[16] |
Hochstein, S., & Ahissar, M. (2002). View from the top: Hierarchies and reverse hierarchies in the visual system. Neuron, 36(5), 791-804.
pmid: 12467584
|
[17] |
Hochstein, S., Pavlovskaya, M., Bonneh, Y. S., & Soroker, N. (2015). Global statistics are not neglected. Journal of Vision, 15(4), 7.
doi: 10.1167/15.4.7
pmid: 26288033
|
[18] |
Huang, L. (2015). Statistical properties demand as much attention as object features. Plos One, 10(8), e0131191.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0131191
URL
|
[19] |
Ji, L., Chen, W., Loeys, T., & Pourtois, G. (2018). Ensemble representation for multiple facial expressions: Evidence for a capacity limited perceptual process. Journal of Vision, 18(3), 1-19.
|
[20] |
Komori, M., Kawamura, S., & Ishihara, S. (2009). Averageness or symmetry: Which is more important for facial attractiveness? Acta Psychologica, 131(2), 136-142.
doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2009.03.008
URL
|
[21] |
Langlois, J. H., & Roggman, L. A. (1990). Attractive faces are only average. Psychological Science, 1(2), 115-121.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1990.tb00079.x
URL
|
[22] |
Li, H., Ji, L., Tong, K., Ren, N., Chen, W., Liu, C. H., & Fu, X. (2016). Processing of individual items during ensemble coding of facial expressions. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1332.
|
[23] |
Luo, A. X., & Zhou, G. (2018). Ensemble perception of facial attractiveness. Journal of Vision, 18(8), 1-19.
|
[24] |
Maule, J., & Franklin, A. (2015). Effects of ensemble complexity and perceptual similarity on rapid averaging of hue. Journal of Vision, 15(4), 6.
doi: 10.1167/15.4.6
pmid: 26114595
|
[25] |
Myczek, K., & Simons, D. J. (2008). Better than average: Alternatives to statistical summary representations for rapid judgments of average size. Perception & Psychophysics, 70(5), 772-788.
doi: 10.3758/PP.70.5.772
URL
|
[26] |
Neumann, M. F., Schweinberger, S. R., & Burton, A. M. (2013). Viewers extract mean and individual identity from sets of famous faces. Cognition, 128(1), 56-63.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.03.006
pmid: 23587844
|
[27] |
O’Toole, A. J., Price, T., Vetter, T., Bartlett, J. C., & Blanz, V. (1999). 3D shape and 2D surface textures of human faces: The role of “averages” in attractiveness and age. Image and Vision Computing, 18(1), 9-19.
doi: 10.1016/S0262-8856(99)00012-8
URL
|
[28] |
Parkes, L., Lund, J., Angelucci, A., Solomon, J. A., & Morgan, M. (2001). Compulsory averaging of crowded orientation signals in human vision. Nature Neuroscience, 4(7), 739-744.
doi: 10.1038/89532
URL
|
[29] |
Ratcliff, R. (1978). A theory of memory retrieval. Psychological Review, 85(2), 59-108.
doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.85.2.59
URL
|
[30] |
Ratcliff, R., & McKoon, G. (2008). The diffusion decision model: Theory and data for two-choice decision tasks. Neural Computation, 20(4), 873-922.
pmid: 18085991
|
[31] |
Rhodes, G., Yoshikawa, S., Clark, A., Lee, K., McKay, R., & Akamatsu, S. (2001). Attractiveness of facial averageness and symmetry in non-western cultures: In search of biologically based standards of beauty. Perception, 30(5), 611-625.
pmid: 11430245
|
[32] |
Vandekerckhove, J., Tuerlinckx, F., & Lee, M. D. (2011). Hierarchical diffusion models for two-choice response times. Psychological Methods, 16(1), 44-62.
doi: 10.1037/a0021765
pmid: 21299302
|
[33] |
van Osch, Y., Blanken, I., Meijs, M. H. J., & van Wolferen, J. (2015). A group’s physical attractiveness is greater than the average attractiveness of its members: the group attractiveness effect. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 41(4), 559-574.
doi: 10.1177/0146167215572799
URL
|
[34] |
Vehtari, A., Gelman, A., Simpson, D., Carpenter, B., & Bürkner, P.-C. (2021). Rank-normalization, folding, and localization: An improved $\widehat{R}$ for assessing convergence of MCMC. Bayesian Analysis, Advance publication (2019). doi: 10.1214/20-BA1221
doi: 10.1214/20-BA1221
|
[35] |
Voss, A., Nagler, M., & Lerche, V. (2013). Diffusion models in experimental psychology: A practical introduction. Experimental Psychology, 60(6), 385-402.
doi: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000218
URL
|
[36] |
Walker, D., & Vul, E. (2014). Hierarchical encoding makes individuals in a group seem more attractive. Psychological Science, 25(1), 230-235.
doi: 10.1177/0956797613497969
pmid: 24163333
|
[37] |
Wang, Y., & Luo, Y. J. (2005). Standardization and assessment of college students’ facial expression of emotion. Chinese Journal of Clinical Psychology, 13(4), 396-398.
|
|
[王妍, 罗跃嘉. (2005). 大学生面孔表情材料的标准化及其评定. 中国临床心理学杂志, 13(4), 396-398.]
|
[38] |
Whitney, D., & Leib, A. Y. (2018). Ensemble perception. Annual Review of Psychology, 69(1), 105-129.
doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010416-044232
URL
|
[39] |
Willis, R. H. (1960). Stimulus pooling and social perception. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 60(3), 365-373.
doi: 10.1037/h0048912
URL
|
[40] |
Ying, H., Burns, E., Choo, A. M., & Xu, H. (2020). Temporal and spatial ensemble statistics are formed by distinct mechanisms. Cognition, 195, 104128. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2019.104128
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2019.104128
URL
|