心理学报 ›› 2023, Vol. 55 ›› Issue (5): 711-725.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2023.00711
收稿日期:
2022-06-23
发布日期:
2023-02-14
出版日期:
2023-05-25
通讯作者:
王振宏, E-mail: wangzhenhong@snnu.edu.cn
基金资助:
Received:
2022-06-23
Online:
2023-02-14
Published:
2023-05-25
摘要:
基于个体多重敏感性因素与环境交互的视角, 本研究探讨了亲子关系、感觉加工敏感性(Sensory Processing Sensitivity, SPS)与COMT Val158Met多态性的交互作用对学前儿童亲社会行为的影响。研究以507名学前儿童为被试(M年龄 = 4.83岁, SD = 0.90岁), 分别采用父母报告的亲子关系量表、高敏感儿童量表、长处与困难问卷中的亲社会分问卷测量了亲子关系、儿童的SPS和亲社会行为, 采集唾液样本进行基因位点的分型。研究结果发现, 亲子冲突、SPS与COMT Val158Met多态性的三重交互作用对儿童亲社会行为影响显著。在携带Val/Val基因型的儿童中, 高SPS儿童比低SPS儿童在低亲子冲突条件下表现更多的亲社会行为, 但在高亲子冲突条件下表现更少的亲社会行为; 在携带Met等位基因的儿童中, 亲子冲突与SPS的交互作用对儿童亲社会行为的影响不显著。研究结果表明同时具有高气质敏感性和高基因敏感性儿童的亲社会行为更容易受到亲子冲突的影响, 这为深入理解个体多重敏感性因素与家庭环境交互影响儿童发展提供了证据。
中图分类号:
刘倩文, 王振宏. (2023). 亲子关系、感觉加工敏感性与COMT Val158Met多态性对学前儿童亲社会行为的交互影响. 心理学报, 55(5), 711-725.
LIU Qianwen, WANG Zhenhong. (2023). The interactive effects of parent-child relationship, sensory processing sensitivity, and the COMT Val158Met polymorphism on preschoolers’ prosocial behaviors. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 55(5), 711-725.
变量 | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1 性别 | − | |||||||
2 年龄 | 0.01 | − | ||||||
3 家庭社会经济地位 | −0.02 | −0.01 | − | |||||
4 亲子亲密 | −0.08 | 0.09* | 0.02 | − | ||||
5 亲子冲突 | 0.03 | 0.02 | −0.05 | −0.16*** | − | |||
6 SPS | −0.06 | −0.01 | −0.02 | 0.13** | 0.01 | − | ||
7 COMT Val158Met多态性 | 0.07 | −0.06 | −0.04 | 0.01 | −0.04 | 0.02 | − | |
8 亲社会行为 | −0.11* | 0.16*** | −0.09* | 0.35*** | −0.23*** | 0.15** | −0.01 | − |
M | − | 4.83 | 0.00 | 43.35 | 24.92 | 63.74 | − | 7.39 |
SD | − | 0.90 | 2.27 | 3.70 | 6.82 | 7.39 | − | 1.95 |
表1 各变量间的相关系数
变量 | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1 性别 | − | |||||||
2 年龄 | 0.01 | − | ||||||
3 家庭社会经济地位 | −0.02 | −0.01 | − | |||||
4 亲子亲密 | −0.08 | 0.09* | 0.02 | − | ||||
5 亲子冲突 | 0.03 | 0.02 | −0.05 | −0.16*** | − | |||
6 SPS | −0.06 | −0.01 | −0.02 | 0.13** | 0.01 | − | ||
7 COMT Val158Met多态性 | 0.07 | −0.06 | −0.04 | 0.01 | −0.04 | 0.02 | − | |
8 亲社会行为 | −0.11* | 0.16*** | −0.09* | 0.35*** | −0.23*** | 0.15** | −0.01 | − |
M | − | 4.83 | 0.00 | 43.35 | 24.92 | 63.74 | − | 7.39 |
SD | − | 0.90 | 2.27 | 3.70 | 6.82 | 7.39 | − | 1.95 |
变量 | 亲社会行为 | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Bootstrap 95% CI | |||||
b | SE | p | 下限 | 上限 | |
第一步 (ΔR2 = 0.10) | |||||
性别 | −0.21 | 0.09 | 0.012 | −0.375 | −0.043 |
年龄 | 0.16 | 0.05 | <0.001 | 0.078 | 0.252 |
家庭社会经济地位 | −0.05 | 0.02 | 0.018 | −0.081 | −0.008 |
亲子冲突 | −0.23 | 0.05 | <0.001 | −0.324 | −0.142 |
第二步 (ΔR2 = 0.10) | |||||
亲子亲密 | 0.29 | 0.04 | <0.001 | 0.206 | 0.380 |
SPS | 0.11 | 0.04 | 0.009 | 0.027 | 0.191 |
COMT Val158Met多态性 | −0.04 | 0.08 | 0.664 | −0.192 | 0.122 |
第三步(ΔR2 = 0.01) | |||||
亲子亲密×SPS | 0.06 | 0.04 | 0.095 | −0.008 | 0.147 |
亲子亲密×COMT Val158Met多态性 | 0.05 | 0.09 | 0.604 | −0.131 | 0.217 |
SPS×COMT Val158Met多态性 | 0.01 | 0.09 | 0.922 | −0.163 | 0.181 |
第四步(ΔR2 = 0.001) | |||||
亲子亲密×SPS×COMT Val158Met多态性 | 0.06 | 0.08 | 0.412 | −0.113 | 0.211 |
表2 亲子亲密、SPS和COMT Val158Met多态性对学前儿童亲社会行为的影响
变量 | 亲社会行为 | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Bootstrap 95% CI | |||||
b | SE | p | 下限 | 上限 | |
第一步 (ΔR2 = 0.10) | |||||
性别 | −0.21 | 0.09 | 0.012 | −0.375 | −0.043 |
年龄 | 0.16 | 0.05 | <0.001 | 0.078 | 0.252 |
家庭社会经济地位 | −0.05 | 0.02 | 0.018 | −0.081 | −0.008 |
亲子冲突 | −0.23 | 0.05 | <0.001 | −0.324 | −0.142 |
第二步 (ΔR2 = 0.10) | |||||
亲子亲密 | 0.29 | 0.04 | <0.001 | 0.206 | 0.380 |
SPS | 0.11 | 0.04 | 0.009 | 0.027 | 0.191 |
COMT Val158Met多态性 | −0.04 | 0.08 | 0.664 | −0.192 | 0.122 |
第三步(ΔR2 = 0.01) | |||||
亲子亲密×SPS | 0.06 | 0.04 | 0.095 | −0.008 | 0.147 |
亲子亲密×COMT Val158Met多态性 | 0.05 | 0.09 | 0.604 | −0.131 | 0.217 |
SPS×COMT Val158Met多态性 | 0.01 | 0.09 | 0.922 | −0.163 | 0.181 |
第四步(ΔR2 = 0.001) | |||||
亲子亲密×SPS×COMT Val158Met多态性 | 0.06 | 0.08 | 0.412 | −0.113 | 0.211 |
变量 | 亲社会行为 | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Bootstrap 95% CI | |||||
b | SE | p | 下限 | 上限 | |
第一步 (ΔR2 = 0.16) | |||||
性别 | −0.17 | 0.08 | 0.041 | −0.328 | −0.008 |
年龄 | 0.13 | 0.04 | 0.002 | 0.047 | 0.213 |
家庭社会经济地位 | −0.04 | 0.02 | 0.013 | −0.075 | −0.009 |
亲子亲密 | 0.34 | 0.04 | <0.001 | 0.253 | 0.421 |
第二步 (ΔR2 = 0.05) | |||||
亲子冲突 | −0.19 | 0.04 | <0.001 | −0.272 | −0.102 |
SPS | 0.11 | 0.04 | 0.010 | 0.026 | 0.193 |
COMT Val158Met多态性 | −0.04 | 0.08 | 0.660 | −0.191 | 0.126 |
第三步(ΔR2 = 0.01) | |||||
亲子冲突×SPS | −0.06 | 0.04 | 0.200 | −0.144 | 0.024 |
亲子冲突×COMT Val158Met多态性 | −0.11 | 0.09 | 0.213 | −0.287 | 0.066 |
SPS×COMT Val158Met多态性 | 0.01 | 0.08 | 0.953 | −0.156 | 0.171 |
第四步(ΔR2 = 0.02) | |||||
亲子冲突×SPS×COMT Val158Met多态性 | −0.24 | 0.08 | 0.003 | −0.399 | −0.076 |
表3 亲子冲突、SPS和COMT Val158Met多态性对学前儿童亲社会行为的影响
变量 | 亲社会行为 | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Bootstrap 95% CI | |||||
b | SE | p | 下限 | 上限 | |
第一步 (ΔR2 = 0.16) | |||||
性别 | −0.17 | 0.08 | 0.041 | −0.328 | −0.008 |
年龄 | 0.13 | 0.04 | 0.002 | 0.047 | 0.213 |
家庭社会经济地位 | −0.04 | 0.02 | 0.013 | −0.075 | −0.009 |
亲子亲密 | 0.34 | 0.04 | <0.001 | 0.253 | 0.421 |
第二步 (ΔR2 = 0.05) | |||||
亲子冲突 | −0.19 | 0.04 | <0.001 | −0.272 | −0.102 |
SPS | 0.11 | 0.04 | 0.010 | 0.026 | 0.193 |
COMT Val158Met多态性 | −0.04 | 0.08 | 0.660 | −0.191 | 0.126 |
第三步(ΔR2 = 0.01) | |||||
亲子冲突×SPS | −0.06 | 0.04 | 0.200 | −0.144 | 0.024 |
亲子冲突×COMT Val158Met多态性 | −0.11 | 0.09 | 0.213 | −0.287 | 0.066 |
SPS×COMT Val158Met多态性 | 0.01 | 0.08 | 0.953 | −0.156 | 0.171 |
第四步(ΔR2 = 0.02) | |||||
亲子冲突×SPS×COMT Val158Met多态性 | −0.24 | 0.08 | 0.003 | −0.399 | −0.076 |
变量 | 亲社会行为 | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Bootstrap 95% CI | |||||
b | SE | p | 下限 | 上限 | |
第一步 (ΔR2 = 0.18) | |||||
性别 | −0.22 | 0.11 | 0.044 | −0.430 | −0.005 |
年龄 | 0.11 | 0.06 | 0.064 | −0.006 | 0.218 |
家庭社会经济地位 | −0.05 | 0.02 | 0.028 | −0.092 | −0.004 |
亲子亲密 | 0.37 | 0.05 | <0.001 | 0.259 | 0.471 |
第二步 (ΔR2 = 0.07) | |||||
亲子冲突 | −0.22 | 0.05 | <0.001 | −0.328 | −0.119 |
SPS | 0.12 | 0.06 | 0.051 | 0.000 | 0.236 |
第三步(ΔR2 = 0.03) | |||||
亲子冲突×SPS | −0.18 | 0.06 | 0.002 | −0.304 | −0.069 |
表4 亲子冲突和SPS在携带Val/Val基因儿童亲社会行为中的交互作用
变量 | 亲社会行为 | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Bootstrap 95% CI | |||||
b | SE | p | 下限 | 上限 | |
第一步 (ΔR2 = 0.18) | |||||
性别 | −0.22 | 0.11 | 0.044 | −0.430 | −0.005 |
年龄 | 0.11 | 0.06 | 0.064 | −0.006 | 0.218 |
家庭社会经济地位 | −0.05 | 0.02 | 0.028 | −0.092 | −0.004 |
亲子亲密 | 0.37 | 0.05 | <0.001 | 0.259 | 0.471 |
第二步 (ΔR2 = 0.07) | |||||
亲子冲突 | −0.22 | 0.05 | <0.001 | −0.328 | −0.119 |
SPS | 0.12 | 0.06 | 0.051 | 0.000 | 0.236 |
第三步(ΔR2 = 0.03) | |||||
亲子冲突×SPS | −0.18 | 0.06 | 0.002 | −0.304 | −0.069 |
变量 | 亲社会行为 | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Bootstrap 95% CI | |||||
b | SE | p | 下限 | 上限 | |
第一步 (ΔR2 = 0.14) | |||||
性别 | −0.11 | 0.13 | 0.414 | −0.353 | 0.143 |
年龄 | 0.15 | 0.06 | 0.015 | 0.028 | 0.278 |
家庭社会经济地位 | −0.04 | 0.03 | 0.159 | −0.088 | 0.016 |
亲子亲密 | 0.31 | 0.07 | <0.001 | 0.165 | 0.441 |
第二步 (ΔR2 = 0.02) | |||||
亲子冲突 | −0.13 | 0.08 | 0.087 | −0.280 | 0.020 |
SPS | 0.10 | 0.06 | 0.110 | −0.024 | 0.225 |
第三步(ΔR2 = 0.004) | |||||
亲子冲突×SPS | 0.06 | 0.06 | 0.286 | −0.052 | 0.167 |
表5 亲子冲突和SPS在携带Met等位基因儿童亲社会行为中的交互作用
变量 | 亲社会行为 | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Bootstrap 95% CI | |||||
b | SE | p | 下限 | 上限 | |
第一步 (ΔR2 = 0.14) | |||||
性别 | −0.11 | 0.13 | 0.414 | −0.353 | 0.143 |
年龄 | 0.15 | 0.06 | 0.015 | 0.028 | 0.278 |
家庭社会经济地位 | −0.04 | 0.03 | 0.159 | −0.088 | 0.016 |
亲子亲密 | 0.31 | 0.07 | <0.001 | 0.165 | 0.441 |
第二步 (ΔR2 = 0.02) | |||||
亲子冲突 | −0.13 | 0.08 | 0.087 | −0.280 | 0.020 |
SPS | 0.10 | 0.06 | 0.110 | −0.024 | 0.225 |
第三步(ΔR2 = 0.004) | |||||
亲子冲突×SPS | 0.06 | 0.06 | 0.286 | −0.052 | 0.167 |
变量 | 亲社会行为 | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Bootstrap 95% CI | |||||
b | SE | p | 下限 | 上限 | |
第一步 (ΔR2 = 0.15) | |||||
性别 | −0.15 | 0.12 | 0.223 | −0.380 | 0.092 |
年龄 | 0.16 | 0.06 | 0.009 | 0.039 | 0.274 |
家庭社会经济地位 | −0.03 | 0.03 | 0.166 | −0.083 | 0.016 |
亲子亲密 | 0.34 | 0.07 | <0.001 | 0.200 | 0.464 |
第二步 (ΔR2 = 0.03) | |||||
亲子冲突 | −0.15 | 0.06 | 0.018 | −0.275 | −0.026 |
SPS | 0.08 | 0.06 | 0.162 | −0.034 | 0.188 |
COMT Val158Met多态性 | −0.04 | 0.12 | 0.759 | −0.278 | 0.196 |
第三步(ΔR2 = 0.01) | |||||
亲子冲突×SPS | 0.05 | 0.05 | 0.376 | −0.056 | 0.147 |
亲子冲突×COMT Val158Met多态性 | −0.08 | 0.13 | 0.511 | −0.328 | 0.167 |
SPS×COMT Val158Met多态性 | 0.09 | 0.12 | 0.440 | −0.136 | 0.323 |
第四步(ΔR2 = 0.01) | |||||
亲子冲突×SPS×COMT Val158Met多态性 | −0.20 | 0.10 | 0.041 | −0.412 | −0.006 |
表6 亲子冲突、SPS和COMT Val158Met多态性对学前儿童亲社会行为的影响(子样本1)
变量 | 亲社会行为 | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Bootstrap 95% CI | |||||
b | SE | p | 下限 | 上限 | |
第一步 (ΔR2 = 0.15) | |||||
性别 | −0.15 | 0.12 | 0.223 | −0.380 | 0.092 |
年龄 | 0.16 | 0.06 | 0.009 | 0.039 | 0.274 |
家庭社会经济地位 | −0.03 | 0.03 | 0.166 | −0.083 | 0.016 |
亲子亲密 | 0.34 | 0.07 | <0.001 | 0.200 | 0.464 |
第二步 (ΔR2 = 0.03) | |||||
亲子冲突 | −0.15 | 0.06 | 0.018 | −0.275 | −0.026 |
SPS | 0.08 | 0.06 | 0.162 | −0.034 | 0.188 |
COMT Val158Met多态性 | −0.04 | 0.12 | 0.759 | −0.278 | 0.196 |
第三步(ΔR2 = 0.01) | |||||
亲子冲突×SPS | 0.05 | 0.05 | 0.376 | −0.056 | 0.147 |
亲子冲突×COMT Val158Met多态性 | −0.08 | 0.13 | 0.511 | −0.328 | 0.167 |
SPS×COMT Val158Met多态性 | 0.09 | 0.12 | 0.440 | −0.136 | 0.323 |
第四步(ΔR2 = 0.01) | |||||
亲子冲突×SPS×COMT Val158Met多态性 | −0.20 | 0.10 | 0.041 | −0.412 | −0.006 |
变量 | 亲社会行为 | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Bootstrap 95% CI | |||||
b | SE | p | 下限 | 上限 | |
第一步 (ΔR2 = 0.17) | |||||
性别 | −0.19 | 0.12 | 0.101 | −0.412 | 0.038 |
年龄 | 0.10 | 0.06 | 0.089 | −0.015 | 0.223 |
家庭社会经济地位 | −0.05 | 0.02 | 0.036 | −0.096 | −0.003 |
亲子亲密 | 0.34 | 0.06 | <0.001 | 0.226 | 0.449 |
第二步 (ΔR2 = 0.07) | |||||
亲子冲突 | −0.23 | 0.06 | <0.001 | −0.357 | −0.110 |
SPS | 0.14 | 0.06 | 0.022 | 0.022 | 0.269 |
COMT Val158Met多态性 | −0.03 | 0.11 | 0.762 | −0.254 | 0.182 |
第三步(ΔR2 = 0.05) | |||||
亲子冲突×SPS | −0.23 | 0.07 | 0.001 | −0.378 | −0.107 |
亲子冲突×COMT Val158Met多态性 | −0.11 | 0.12 | 0.381 | −0.364 | 0.123 |
SPS×COMT Val158Met多态性 | −0.13 | 0.12 | 0.290 | −0.361 | 0.122 |
第四步(ΔR2 = 0.02) | |||||
亲子冲突×SPS×COMT Val158Met多态性 | −0.28 | 0.13 | 0.017 | −0.512 | −0.014 |
表7 亲子冲突、SPS和COMT Val158Met多态性对学前儿童亲社会行为的影响(子样本2)
变量 | 亲社会行为 | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Bootstrap 95% CI | |||||
b | SE | p | 下限 | 上限 | |
第一步 (ΔR2 = 0.17) | |||||
性别 | −0.19 | 0.12 | 0.101 | −0.412 | 0.038 |
年龄 | 0.10 | 0.06 | 0.089 | −0.015 | 0.223 |
家庭社会经济地位 | −0.05 | 0.02 | 0.036 | −0.096 | −0.003 |
亲子亲密 | 0.34 | 0.06 | <0.001 | 0.226 | 0.449 |
第二步 (ΔR2 = 0.07) | |||||
亲子冲突 | −0.23 | 0.06 | <0.001 | −0.357 | −0.110 |
SPS | 0.14 | 0.06 | 0.022 | 0.022 | 0.269 |
COMT Val158Met多态性 | −0.03 | 0.11 | 0.762 | −0.254 | 0.182 |
第三步(ΔR2 = 0.05) | |||||
亲子冲突×SPS | −0.23 | 0.07 | 0.001 | −0.378 | −0.107 |
亲子冲突×COMT Val158Met多态性 | −0.11 | 0.12 | 0.381 | −0.364 | 0.123 |
SPS×COMT Val158Met多态性 | −0.13 | 0.12 | 0.290 | −0.361 | 0.122 |
第四步(ΔR2 = 0.02) | |||||
亲子冲突×SPS×COMT Val158Met多态性 | −0.28 | 0.13 | 0.017 | −0.512 | −0.014 |
[1] |
Acar, I. H., Evans, M. Y. Q., Rudasill, K. M., & Yildiz, S. (2018). The contributions of relationships with parents and teachers to Turkish children’s antisocial behavior. Educational Psychology, 38(7), 877-897.
doi: 10.1080/01443410.2018.1441377 URL |
[2] | Acevedo, B. P. (2020). The highly sensitive brain: Research, assessment, and treatment of sensory processing sensitivity. Academic Press. |
[3] |
Acevedo, B. P., Aron, E. N., Aron, A., Sangster, M. D., Collins, N., & Brown, L. L. (2014). The highly sensitive brain: An fMRI study of sensory processing sensitivity and response to others’ emotions. Brain and Behavior, 4(4), 580-594.
doi: 10.1002/brb3.242 pmid: 25161824 |
[4] | Acevedo, B., Aron, E., Pospos, S., & Jessen, D. (2018). The functional highly sensitive brain: A review of the brain circuits underlying sensory processing sensitivity and seemingly related disorders. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences, 373(1744), 20170161. |
[5] |
Andersen, S. L., & Teicher, M. H. (2008). Stress, sensitive periods and maturational events in adolescent depression. Trends in Neurosciences, 31(4), 183-191.
doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2008.01.004 pmid: 18329735 |
[6] |
Aron, E. N., & Aron, A. (1997). Sensory processing sensitivity and its relation to introversion and emotionality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73(2), 345-368.
pmid: 9248053 |
[7] |
Aron, E. N., Aron, A., & Jagiellowicz, J. (2012). Sensory processing sensitivity: A review in the light of the evolution of biological responsivity. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 16(3), 262-282.
doi: 10.1177/1088868311434213 pmid: 22291044 |
[8] |
Assary, E., Zavos, H. M. S., Krapohl, E., Keers, R., & Pluess, M. (2020). Genetic architecture of environmental sensitivity reflects multiple heritable components: A twin study with adolescents. Molecular Psychiatry, 26 (9), 4896-4904.
doi: 10.1038/s41380-020-0783-8 |
[9] |
Belsky, J., & Pluess, M. (2009). Beyond diathesis stress: Differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Psychological Bulletin, 135(6), 885-908.
doi: 10.1037/a0017376 pmid: 19883141 |
[10] |
Belsky, J., Jonassaint, C., Pluess, M., Stanton, M., Brummett, B., & Williams, R. (2009). Vulnerability genes or plasticity genes? Molecular Psychiatry, 14(8), 746-754.
doi: 10.1038/mp.2009.44 pmid: 19455150 |
[11] |
Berke, J. D. (2018). What does dopamine mean? Nature Neuroscience, 21(6), 787-793.
doi: 10.1038/s41593-018-0152-y pmid: 29760524 |
[12] |
Brett, B. E., Stern, J. A., Gross, J. T., & Cassidy, J. (2020). Maternal depressive symptoms and preschoolers' helping, sharing, and comforting: The moderating role of child attachment. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 51(5), 623-636.
doi: 10.1080/15374416.2020.1738235 URL |
[13] | Bronfenbrenner, U., & Morris, P. A. (1998). The ecology of developmental processes.In: Handbook of child psychology(Vol. 1, pp. 563-634, pp. 563-634). Wiley. |
[14] |
Cao, C., Wang, M., Cao, Y., Ji, L., & Zhang, W. (2017). The interactive effects of monoamine oxidase A (MAOA) gene and peer victimization on depressive symptoms in early adolescent boys: The moderating role of catechol-O- methyltransferase (COMT) gene. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 49(2), 206-218.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2017.00206 URL |
[曹丛, 王美萍, 曹衍淼, 纪林芹, 张文新. (2017). MAOA基因T941G多态性与同伴侵害对男青少年早期抑郁的交互作用: COMT基因Val158Met多态性的调节效应. 心理学报, 49(2), 206-218.] | |
[15] | Cao, Y., Lin, X., Ji, L., Zhang, Y., & Zhang, W. (2017). The interaction between Val158Met polymorphism in the COMT gene and peer relationship on adolescents’ depression. Psychological Development and Education, 33(2), 216-227. |
[曹衍淼, 林小楠, 纪林芹, 张粤萍, 张文新. (2017). COMT基因Val158Met多态性与同伴关系对青少年抑郁的影响. 心理发展与教育, 33(2), 216-227. ] | |
[16] |
Cao, Y., & Zhang, W. (2019). The influence of dopaminergic genetic variants and maternal parenting on adolescent depressive symptoms: A multilocus genetic study. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 51(10), 1102-1115.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2019.01102 |
[曹衍淼, 张文新. (2019). 多巴胺系统基因与母亲教养行为对青少年抑郁的影响:一项多基因研究. 心理学报, 51(10), 1102-1115.]
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2019.01102 |
|
[17] |
Cohen, S., Doyle, W. J., & Baum, A. (2006). Socioeconomic status is associated with stress hormones. Psychosomatic Medicine, 68(3), 414-420.
doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000221236.37158.b9 pmid: 16738073 |
[18] | Cowell, J. M., Lee, K., Malcolm-Smith, S., Selcuk, B., Zhou, X., & Decety, J. (2017). The development of generosity and moral cognition across five cultures. Developmental Science, 20(4), e12403. |
[19] |
Drabant, E. M., Hariri, A. R., Meyer-Lindenberg, A., Munoz, K. E., Mattay, V S., Kolachana, B. S., … Weinberger, D. R. (2006). Catechol-O-methyltransferase ValI58Met genotype and neural mechanisms related to affective arousal and regulation. Archives of General Psychiatry, 63(12), 1396-1406.
pmid: 17146014 |
[20] | Driscoll, K., & Pianta, R. C. (2011). Mothers' and fathers' perceptions of conflict and closeness in parent-child relationships during early childhood. Journal of Early Childhood & Infant Psychology, 7, 1-24. |
[21] |
Drury, S. S., Gleason, M. M., Theall, K. P., Smyke, A. T., Nelson, C. A., Fox, N. A., & Zeanah, C. H. (2012). Genetic sensitivity to the caregiving context: The influence of 5HTTLPR and BDNF Val66Met on indiscriminate social behavior. Physiology & Behavior, 106(5), 728-735.
doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.11.014 URL |
[22] |
Drury, S. S., Theall, K. P., Smyke, A. T., Keats, B. J. B., Egger, H. L., Nelson, C. A., … Zeanah, C. H. (2010). Modification of depression by COMT Val158Met polymorphism in children exposed to early severe psychosocial deprivation. Child Abuse & Neglect, 34(6), 387-395.
doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2009.09.021 URL |
[23] |
Ellis, B. J., Boyce, W. T., Belsky, J., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (2011). Differential susceptibility to the environment: An evolutionary- neurodevelopmental theory. Development and Psychopathology, 23(1), 7-28.
doi: 10.1017/S0954579410000611 URL |
[24] |
Eisenberg, N., & Miller, P. A. (1987). The relation of empathy to prosocial and related behaviors. Psychological Bulletin, 101(1), 91-119.
pmid: 3562705 |
[25] | Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., & Knafo-Noam, A. (2015). Prosocial development. In M. E. Lamb & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology and developmental science (Vol. 3, pp. 610-656). Wiley |
[26] |
Eivers, A. R., Brendgen, M., Vitaro, F., & Borge, A. (2012). Concurrent and longitudinal links between children's and their friends' antisocial and prosocial behavior in preschool. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 27(1), 137-146.
doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2011.05.001 URL |
[27] |
Fang, S., Galambos, N. L., & Johnson, M. D. (2021). Parent-child contact, closeness, and conflict across the transition to adulthood. Journal of Marriage and Family, 83(4), 1176-1193.
doi: 10.1111/jomf.v83.4 URL |
[28] |
Flouri, E., & Sarmadi, Z. (2016). Prosocial behavior and childhood trajectories of internalizing and externalizing problems: The role of neighborhood and school contexts. Developmental Psychology, 52(2), 253-258.
doi: 10.1037/dev0000076 pmid: 26619321 |
[29] | Gao, X., Shi, W., Zhai, Y., He, L., & Shi, X. (2013). Results of the parent-rated Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire in 22, 108 primary school students from 8 provinces of China. Shanghai Archives of Psychiatry, 25(6), 364-374. |
[高欣, 石文惠, 翟屹, 何柳, 施小明. (2013). 长处与困难问卷(父母版)在中国8个省份22, 108名小学生中的调查结果(英文). 上海精神医学, 25(6), 364-374.] | |
[30] |
Goodman, R. (1999). The extended version of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire as a guide to child psychiatric caseness and consequent burden. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40(5), 791-799.
pmid: 10433412 |
[31] |
Greven, C. U., Lionetti, F., Booth, C., Aron, E. N., Fox, E., Schendan, H. E., ... Homberg, J. (2019). Sensory processing sensitivity in the context of environmental sensitivity: A critical review and development of research agenda. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 98, 287-305.
doi: S0149-7634(18)30625-0 pmid: 30639671 |
[32] | Hoffman, M. L. (2007). The origins of empathic morality in toddlerhood. In C. A.Brownell & C. B.Kopp (Eds.), Socioemotional development in the toddler years: Transitions and transformations (pp. 132-145). The Guilford Press. |
[33] |
Holland, A. S., & McElwain, N. L. (2013). Maternal and paternal perceptions of coparenting as a link between marital quality and the parent-toddler relationship. Journal of Family Psychology, 27(1), 117-126.
doi: 10.1037/a0031427 pmid: 23421839 |
[34] |
Hygen, B. W., Belsky, J., Stenseng, F., Lydersen, S., Guzey, I. C., & Wichstrøm, L. (2015). Child exposure to serious life events, COMT, and aggression: Testing differential susceptibility theory. Developmental Psychology, 51(8), 1098-1104.
doi: 10.1037/dev0000020 pmid: 26053146 |
[35] |
Hygen, B. W., Guzey, I. C., Belsky, J., Berg-Nielsen, T. S., & Wichstrøm, L. (2014). Catechol-O-methyltransferase Val158Met genotype moderates the effect of disorganized attachment on social development in young children. Development and Psychopathology, 26(4), 947-961.
doi: 10.1017/S0954579414000492 URL |
[36] |
Iimura, S., & Kibe, C. (2020). Highly sensitive adolescent benefits in positive school transitions: Evidence for vantage sensitivity in Japanese high-schoolers. Developmental Psychology, 56(8), 1565-1581.
doi: 10.1037/dev0000991 pmid: 32525329 |
[37] |
Knafo, A., Israel, S., & Ebstein, R. P. (2011). Heritability of children's prosocial behavior and differential susceptibility to parenting by variation in the dopamine receptor D4 gene. Development and Psychopathology, 23(1), 53-67.
doi: 10.1017/S0954579410000647 pmid: 21262039 |
[38] |
Knafo, A., & Plomin, R. (2006). Prosocial behavior from early to middle childhood: Genetic and environmental influences on stability and change. Developmental Psychology, 42(5), 771-786.
doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.42.5.771 pmid: 16953685 |
[39] | Kwon, A., Min, D., Kim, Y., Jin, M. J., & Lee, S. H. (2019). Interaction between catechol-O-methyltransferase polymorphism and childhood trauma in suicidal ideation of patients with post-traumatic stress disorder. Brain and Behavior, 10(8), e01733. |
[40] |
Lachman, H. M., Papolos, D. F., Saito, T., Yu, Y. M., Szumlanski, C. L., & Weinshilboum, R. M. (1996). Human catechol-O-methyltransferase pharmacogenetics: Description of a functional polymorphism and its potential application to neuropsychiatric disorders. Pharmacogenetics, 6(3), 243-250.
doi: 10.1097/00008571-199606000-00007 pmid: 8807664 |
[41] | Laursen, B., & Collins, W. A. (2009). Parent-child relationships during adolescence. In R. M.Lerner & L.Steinberg (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 3-42). Wiley. |
[42] | Lester, K. J., Hudson, J. L., Tropeano, M., Creswell, C., Collier, D. A., Farmer, A., … Eley, T. C. (2012). Neurotrophic gene polymorphisms and response to psychological therapy. Translational Psychiatry, 2, e108. |
[43] |
Li, Z., Sturge-Apple, M. L., & Davies, P. T. (2021). Family context in association with the development of child sensory processing sensitivity. Developmental Psychology, 57(12), 2165-2178.
doi: 10.1037/dev0001256 pmid: 34928666 |
[44] |
Li, Z., Sturge-Apple, M. L., Jones-Gordils, H. R., & Davies, P. T. (2022). Sensory processing sensitivity behavior moderates the association between environmental harshness, unpredictability, and child socioemotional functioning. Development and Psychopathology, 34 (2), 675-688.
doi: 10.1017/S0954579421001188 URL |
[45] | Licht, C. L., Mortensen, E. L., & Knudsen, G. M. (2011). Association between sensory processing sensitivity and the serotonin transporter polymorphism 5-HTTLPR short/short genotype. Biological Psychiatry, 69(9), 152S-153S. |
[46] | Li, X. (2021). The moderating role of sensory processing sensitivity in the influence of parenting behavior on children’s prosocial behaviors (Unpublished master’s thesis). Southwest University, China. |
[李喜乐. (2021). 教养行为对儿童亲社会行为的影响:感觉加工敏感性的调节作用 (硕士学位论文). 西南大学.] | |
[47] |
Lionetti, F., Aron, E. N., Aron, A., Klein, D. N., & Pluess, M. (2019). Observer-rated environmental sensitivity moderates children’s response to parenting quality in early childhood. Developmental Psychology, 55(11), 2389-2402.
doi: 10.1037/dev0000795 URL |
[48] | Lionetti, F., Spinelli, M., Moscardino, U., Ponzetti, S., Garito, M., Dellagiulia, A., ... Pluess, M. (2022). The interplay between parenting and environmental sensitivity in the prediction of children’s externalizing and internalizing behaviors during COVID-19. Development and Psychopathology. |
[49] | Meyer, B. M., Huemer, J., Rabl, U., Boubela, R. N., Kalcher, K., Berger, A., ... Pezawas, L. (2016). Oppositional COMT Val158Met effects on resting state functional connectivity in adolescents and adults. Brain Structure & Function, 221(1), 103-114. |
[50] |
Monroe, S. M., & Simons, A. D. (1991). Diathesis-stress theories in the context of life stress research: Implications for the depressive disorders. Psychological Bulletin, 110(3), 406-425.
doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.110.3.406 pmid: 1758917 |
[51] |
Moore, S. R., & Depue, R. A. (2016). Neurobehavioral foundation of environmental reactivity. Psychological Bulletin, 142(2), 107-164.
doi: 10.1037/bul0000028 pmid: 26479069 |
[52] | Nava-Gonzalez, E. J., Gallegos-Cabriales, E. C., Leal- Berumen, I., & Bastarrachea, R. A. (2017). Mini-review: The contribution of intermediate phenotypes to G×E effects on disorders of body composition in the new omics era. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 14(9), 1079. |
[53] |
Obradović, J., & Boyce, W. T. (2009). Individual differences in behavioral, physiological, and genetic sensitivities to contexts: Implications for development and adaptation. Developmental Neuroscience, 31(4), 300-308.
doi: 10.1159/000216541 pmid: 19546567 |
[54] |
O’Brien, T. C., Mustanski, B. S., Skol, A., Cook, E. H., & Wakschlag, L. S. (2013). Do dopamine gene variants and prenatal smoking interactively predict youth externalizing behavior? Neurotoxicology and Teratology, 40, 67-73.
doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2013.09.002 pmid: 24064458 |
[55] | Pianta, R. C. (1992). Child-Parent Relationship Scale. Unpublished measure, University of Virginia, Charlottesville. |
[56] |
Pianta, R. C., & Lothman, D. J. (1994). Predicting behavior problems in children with epilepsy: Child factors, disease factors, family stress, and child-mother interaction. Child Development, 65(5), 1415-1428.
pmid: 7982359 |
[57] |
Pluess, M. (2015). Individual differences in environmental sensitivity. Child Development Perspectives, 9(3), 138-143.
doi: 10.1111/cdep.2015.9.issue-3 URL |
[58] |
Pluess, M., Assary, E., Lionetti, F., Lester, K. J., Krapohl, E., Aron, E. N., & Aron, A. (2018). Environmental sensitivity in children: Development of the Highly Sensitive Child Scale and identification of sensitivity groups. Developmental Psychology, 54(1), 51-70.
doi: 10.1037/dev0000406 pmid: 28933890 |
[59] |
Pluess, M., & Belsky, J. (2013). Vantage sensitivity: Individual differences in response to positive experiences. Psychological Bulletin, 139(4), 901-916.
doi: 10.1037/a0030196 pmid: 23025924 |
[60] |
Podsakoff, P. M., Mackenzie, S. B., Lee, J. Y., & Podsakoff, N. P. (2003). Common method biases in behavioral research: A critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88(5), 879-903.
doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.88.5.879 pmid: 14516251 |
[61] |
Poletti, S., Radaelli, D., Cavallaro, R., Bosia, M., Lorenzi, C., Pirovano, A., ... Benedetti, F. (2013). Catechol-O- methyltransferase (COMT) genotype biases neural correlates of empathy and perceived personal distress in schizophrenia. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 54(2), 181-186.
doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2012.06.008 pmid: 22901597 |
[62] |
Reuter, M., Frenzel, C., Walter, N. T., Markett, S., & Montag, C. (2011). Investigating the genetic basis of altruism: The role of the COMT Val158Met polymorphism. Social Cognitive Affect Neuroscience, 6(5), 662-668.
doi: 10.1093/scan/nsq083 URL |
[63] |
Roisman, G. I., Newman, D. A., Fraley, R. C., Haltigan, J. D., Groh, A. M., & Haydon, K. C. (2012). Distinguishing differential susceptibility from diathesis-stress: Recommendations for evaluating interaction effects. Development and Psychopathology, 24(2), 389-409.
doi: 10.1017/S0954579412000065 pmid: 22559121 |
[64] |
Ru, W., Fang, P., Wang, B., Yang, X., Zhu, X., Xue, M., ... Gong, P. (2017). The impacts of Val158Met in catechol-o-methyltransferase (COMT) gene on moral permissibility and empathic concern. Personality and Individual Differences, 106, 52-56.
doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2016.10.041 URL |
[65] |
Sáez, I., Zhu, L., Set, E., Kayser, A., & Hsu M. (2015). Dopamine modulates egalitarian behavior in humans. Current Biology. 25(7), 912-919.
doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.01.071 pmid: 25802148 |
[66] |
Slagt, M., Dubas, J. S., van Aken, M. A., Ellis, B. J., & Dekovic, M. (2018). Sensory processing sensitivity as a marker of differential susceptibility to parenting. Developmental Psychology, 54(3), 543-558.
doi: 10.1037/dev0000431 pmid: 29154642 |
[67] |
Smolka, M. N., Gunter, S., Jana, W., Sabine, M., Grüsser, H. F., Karl, M., ... Heinz, A. (2005). Catechol-o-methyltransferase Val158Met genotype affects processing of emotional stimuli in the amygdala and prefrontal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 25(4), 836-842.
doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1792-04.2005 pmid: 15673663 |
[68] |
Starr, L. R., Hammen, C., Conway, C. C., Raposa, E., & Brennan, P. A. (2014). Sensitizing effect of early adversity on depressive reactions to later proximal stress: Moderation by polymorphisms in serotonin transporter and corticotropin releasing hormone receptor genes in a 20-year longitudinal study. Development and Psychopathology, 26(4), 1241-1254.
doi: 10.1017/S0954579414000996 URL |
[69] |
Stein, D. J., Newman, T. K., Savitz, J., & Ramesar, R. (2006). Warriors versus worriers: The role of COMT gene variants. CNS Spectr, 11(10), 745-748.
doi: 10.1017/S1092852900014863 URL |
[70] | Wang, M., & Zhang, W. (2010). Association between aggressive behavior and a functional polymorphism in the COMT gene. Advances in Psychological Science, 18(8), 1256-1262. |
[王美萍, 张文新. (2010). COMT基因多态性与攻击行为的关系. 心理科学进展, 18(8), 1256-1262.] | |
[71] |
Wang, M., Zheng, X., Xia, G., Liu, D., Chen, P., & Zhang, W. (2019). Association between negative life events and early adolescents’ depression: The moderating effects of Catechol- O-methyltransferase (COMT) Gene Val158Met polymorphism and parenting behavior. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 51(8), 903-913.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2019.00903 URL |
[王美萍, 郑晓洁, 夏桂芝, 刘迪迪, 陈翩, 张文新. (2019). 负性生活事件与青少年早期抑郁的关系:COMT基因Val158Met多态性与父母教养行为的调节作用. 心理学报, 51(8), 903-913.]
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2019.00903 |
|
[72] | Wang, Z. (2022). Environmental sensitivity model on child development: An integrated view. Journal of Psychological Science, 45(6), 1367-1374. |
[王振宏. (2022). 儿童发展的环境敏感性模型:一种整合的观点. 心理科学, 45(6), 1367-1374.] | |
[73] | Wang, Z., Wang, X., & Li, C. (2020). Different environmental sensitivity of children: Developmental theory and empirical evidence. Journal of Beijing Normal University (Social Sciences), 4, 36-47. |
[王振宏, 王笑笑, 李彩娜. (2020). 儿童发展的不同环境敏感性: 理论与实证. 北京师范大学学报(社会科学版), 4, 36-47.] | |
[74] |
Weinshilboum, R. (1988). Pharmacogenetics of methylation: Relationship to drug metabolism. Clinical Biochemistry, 21(4), 201-210.
pmid: 3044645 |
[75] |
Widaman, K. F., Helm, J. L., Castro-Schilo, L., Pluess, M., Stallings, M. C., & Belsky, J. (2012). Distinguishing ordinal and disordinal interactions. Psychological Methods, 17(4), 615-622.
doi: 10.1037/a0030003 pmid: 22984788 |
[76] |
Wilson, S., Stroud, C. B., & Durbin, C. E. (2017). Interpersonal dysfunction in personality disorders: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 143(7), 677-734.
doi: 10.1037/bul0000101 pmid: 28447827 |
[77] |
Wise, R. A. (2004). Dopamine, learning and motivation. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5(6), 483-494.
doi: 10.1038/nrn1406 pmid: 15152198 |
[78] |
Yan, J., Schoppe-Sullivan, S., & Feng, X. (2019). Trajectories of mother-child and father-child relationships across middle childhood and associations with depressive symptoms. Development and Psychopathology, 31(4), 1381-1393.
doi: 10.1017/S0954579418000809 pmid: 30556510 |
[79] | Zhang, W., Cao, C., Wang, M., Ji, L. Q., & Cao, Y. (2016). Monoamine oxidase a (MAOA) and catechol-o-methyltransferase (COMT) gene polymorphisms interact with maternal parenting in association with adolescent reactive aggression but not proactive aggression: Evidence of differential susceptibility. Journal of Youth & Adolescence, 45(4), 812-829. |
[80] | Zhang, R., & Wang, Z. (2020). Inhibitory control moderates the quadratic association between resting respiratory sinus arrhythmia and prosocial behaviors in children. Psychophysiology, 57(4), e13491. |
[81] |
Zhang, W., Li, X., Chen, G., & Cao, Y. (2021). The relationship between positive parenting and adolescent prosocial behaviour: The mediating role of empathy and the moderating role of the oxytocin receptor gene. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 53(9), 976-991.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00976 |
[张文新, 李曦, 陈光辉, 曹衍淼. (2021). 母亲积极教养与青少年亲社会行为:共情的中介作用与OXTR基因的调节作用. 心理学报, 53(9), 976-991.]
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00976 |
|
[82] |
Zhang, X. (2011). Parent-child and teacher-child relationships in Chinese preschoolers: The moderating role of preschool experiences and the mediating role of social competence. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 26(2), 192-204.
doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2010.09.001 URL |
[83] |
Zhang, X., Chen, H., Zhang, G., Zhou, B., & Wu, W. (2008). A longitudinal study of parent-child relationships and problem behaviors in early childhood: Transactional models. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 40(5), 571-582.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2008.00571 URL |
[张晓, 陈会昌, 张桂芳, 周博芳, 吴巍. (2008). 亲子关系与问题行为的动态相互作用模型: 对儿童早期的追踪研究. 心理学报, 40(5), 571-582.] | |
[84] |
Zhang, Y., Yang, X., & Wang, Z. (2021). The COMT rs4680 polymorphism, family functioning and preschoolers' attentional control indexed by intraindividual reaction time variability. Developmental Psychobiology, 63(4), 713-724.
doi: 10.1002/dev.v63.4 URL |
[85] | Zeng, S., & Wang, Z. (2022). Revision of the parent reported Highly Sensitive Child Scale-Chinese version. Studies of Psychology and Behavior, 20(1), 101-107. |
[ 曾思瑶, 王振宏. (2022). 中文版父母报告高敏感儿童量表的修订. 心理与行为研究, 20(1), 101-107.] |
[1] | 张玮玮, 陈逸群, 朱莉琪. “近朱者赤”:同伴捐赠决策信息对青少年亲社会行为的影响及公正世界信念的作用[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(9): 1453-1464. |
[2] | 胡义秋, 曾子豪, 彭丽仪, 王宏才, 刘双金, 杨琴, 方晓义. 亲子关系和父母教育卷入对青少年抑郁、自伤和自杀意念的影响:挫败感和人生意义感的作用[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(1): 129-141. |
[3] | 段文婷, 孙启武, 王铭, 吴才智, 陈真珍. 青少年早期亲社会行为倾向、内化问题和外化问题发展级联的个体内分析[J]. 心理学报, 2022, 54(7): 813-827. |
[4] | 刘文兴, 祝养浩, 柏阳, 王海江, 韩翼. 孤芳自赏还是乐于助人?员工自恋对亲社会行为的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2022, 54(3): 300-312. |
[5] | 张文新, 李曦, 陈光辉, 曹衍淼. 母亲积极教养与青少年亲社会行为:共情的中介作用与OXTR基因的调节作用[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(9): 976-991. |
[6] | 王文超, 伍新春. 共情对灾后青少年亲社会行为的影响:感恩、社会支持和创伤后成长的中介作用[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(3): 307-316. |
[7] | 王燕, 侯博文, 刘文锦. 童年亲子关系与“好资源”对未婚男性性开放态度的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(2): 207-215. |
[8] | 姚琦, 吴章建, 张常清, 符国群. 权力感对炫耀性亲社会行为的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(12): 1421-1435. |
[9] | 王建峰, 戴冰. “追名弃利”:权力动机与社会存在对亲社会行为的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(1): 55-65. |
[10] | 王美萍, 郑晓洁, 夏桂芝, 刘迪迪, 陈翩, 张文新. 负性生活事件与青少年早期抑郁的关系:COMT基因Val158Met多态性与父母教养行为的调节作用[J]. 心理学报, 2019, 51(8): 903-913. |
[11] | 郝叶芳, 王争艳, 董书阳, 刘斯漫, 武萌, 卢珊. 儿童早期的母亲生活压力对其5岁行为问题的预测效应:链式中介分析[J]. 心理学报, 2019, 51(1): 85-95. |
[12] | 刘雁伶, 曾晓青, 左玲, 黄乐辉, 陈水平, 胡竹菁. 证词自信度和自主探索综合影响 5岁儿童的因果推理[J]. 心理学报, 2018, 50(5): 494-503. |
[13] | 邢淑芬, 李倩倩, 高鑫, 马园园, 傅锐. 不同睡眠时间参数对学前儿童执行功能的差异化影响[J]. 心理学报, 2018, 50(11): 1269-1281. |
[14] | 王燕, 钱啸云, 田芊, 高隽, 李晓煦. 亲子在子女择偶偏好上的一致性比较 及亲子关系的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2018, 50(1): 91-100. |
[15] | 王艳辉;李董平;孙文强;赵力燕;赖雪芬;周月月. 亲子依恋与初中生亲社会行为:有调节的中介效应[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(5): 663-679. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||