心理学报 ›› 2025, Vol. 57 ›› Issue (4): 573-583.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2025.0573 cstr: 32110.14.2025.0573
收稿日期:
2023-09-30
发布日期:
2025-02-06
出版日期:
2025-04-25
通讯作者:
李卉, E-mail: huilipsy@ccnu.edu.cn基金资助:
Received:
2023-09-30
Online:
2025-02-06
Published:
2025-04-25
摘要:
本研究考察社交机器人对3岁和5岁儿童亲社会行为的影响。实验1通过操纵观察者(人类、社交机器人、无)这一变量, 探讨其对3岁和5岁儿童分享行为的影响。结果表明在无观察者条件下,5岁儿童分享贴纸数量显著多于3岁儿童。对3岁儿童来说, 人类和机器人观察者条件下分享贴纸数量显著多于无观察者条件, 人类和机器人观察者条件不存在显著的差异。实验2操纵社交机器人心理能动性(有、无、控制组), 结果发现3岁儿童在有心理能动性的机器人观察条件下比其他两种观察条件下分享更多的贴纸,表现出更多的亲社会行为。本研究表明3岁儿童的亲社会行为会受到机器人心理能动性的影响, 这为人机互动领域的未来应用和研究提供了一定视角和依据。
中图分类号:
李卉, 刘思懿, 庞怡. (2025). 社交机器人对3~5岁儿童分享行为的影响. 心理学报, 57(4), 573-583.
LI Hui, LIU Siyi, PANG Yi. (2025). The effect of a social robot on the sharing behavior of 3- to 5-year-old children. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 57(4), 573-583.
年龄 | 人类观察者 | 机器人观察者 | 无观察者 | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | |
3岁 | 3.11 | 2.61 | 3.59 | 2.19 | 1.59 | 2.06 |
5岁 | 3.93 | 1.59 | 3.11 | 2.33 | 3.30 | 1.98 |
表1 儿童在不同观察者条件下分享贴纸的数量
年龄 | 人类观察者 | 机器人观察者 | 无观察者 | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | |
3岁 | 3.11 | 2.61 | 3.59 | 2.19 | 1.59 | 2.06 |
5岁 | 3.93 | 1.59 | 3.11 | 2.33 | 3.30 | 1.98 |
年龄 | 生物 | 心理 | 感知 | 生命 | 人造 | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | |
3岁 | 0.70 | 0.91 | 1.15 | 0.91 | 1.59 | 0.70 | 0.73 | 0.55 | 0.62 | 0.70 |
5岁 | 0.15 | 0.37 | 1.22 | 0.87 | 1.08 | 0.70 | 0.54 | 0.65 | 1.52 | 0.94 |
总计 | 0.43 | 0.74 | 1.18 | 0.89 | 1.33 | 0.74 | 0.64 | 0.61 | 1.07 | 0.94 |
表2 3岁和5岁儿童泛灵论结果
年龄 | 生物 | 心理 | 感知 | 生命 | 人造 | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | |
3岁 | 0.70 | 0.91 | 1.15 | 0.91 | 1.59 | 0.70 | 0.73 | 0.55 | 0.62 | 0.70 |
5岁 | 0.15 | 0.37 | 1.22 | 0.87 | 1.08 | 0.70 | 0.54 | 0.65 | 1.52 | 0.94 |
总计 | 0.43 | 0.74 | 1.18 | 0.89 | 1.33 | 0.74 | 0.64 | 0.61 | 1.07 | 0.94 |
能动性 | 生物 | 心理 | 感知 | 生命 | 人造 | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | |
有 | 0.69 | 0.92 | 1.16 | 0.89 | 1.35 | 0.90 | 1.16 | 0.73 | 1.10 | 0.91 |
无 | 0.43 | 0.70 | 0.84 | 0.93 | 1.27 | 0.98 | 0.40 | 0.49 | 1.20 | 0.86 |
总计 | 0.56 | 0.83 | 1.00 | 0.92 | 1.31 | 0.94 | 0.78 | 0.73 | 1.15 | 0.88 |
表3 3岁儿童的泛灵论测试结果
能动性 | 生物 | 心理 | 感知 | 生命 | 人造 | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | |
有 | 0.69 | 0.92 | 1.16 | 0.89 | 1.35 | 0.90 | 1.16 | 0.73 | 1.10 | 0.91 |
无 | 0.43 | 0.70 | 0.84 | 0.93 | 1.27 | 0.98 | 0.40 | 0.49 | 1.20 | 0.86 |
总计 | 0.56 | 0.83 | 1.00 | 0.92 | 1.31 | 0.94 | 0.78 | 0.73 | 1.15 | 0.88 |
[1] | Belpaeme, T., Baxter, P., De Greeff, J., Kennedy, J., Read, R., Looije, R.,... Zelati, M. C. (2013). Child-robot interaction: Perspectives and challenges. In Social Robotics: 5th International Conference, ICSR 2013, Bristol, UK, October 27-29, 2013, Proceedings 5 (pp. 452-459). Springer International Publishing. |
[2] | Belpaeme, T., Kennedy, J., Ramachandran, A., Scassellati, B., & Tanaka, F. (2018). Social robots for education: A review. Science Robotics, 3(21), eaat5954. https://doi.org/10.1126/scirobotics.aat5954 |
[3] | Benenson, J. F., Pascoe, J., & Radmore, N. (2007). Children’s altruistic behavior in the dictator game. Evolution and Human Behavior, 28(3), 168-175. |
[4] | Beran, T. N., Ramirez-Serrano, A., Kuzyk, R., Fior, M., & Nugent, S. (2011). Understanding how children understand robots: Perceived animism in child-robot interaction. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 69(7-8), 539-550. |
[5] |
Brink, K. A., & Wellman, H. M. (2020). Robot teachers for children? Young children trust robots depending on their perceived accuracy and agency. Developmental Psychology, 56(7), 1268-1277.
doi: 10.1037/dev0000884 pmid: 32463269 |
[6] | Carlo, G., McGinley, M., Hayes, R., Batenhorst, C., & Wilkinson, J. (2007). Parenting styles or practices? Parenting, sympathy, and prosocial behaviors among adolescents. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 168(2), 147-176. |
[7] | Chernyak, N., & Gary, H. E. (2016). Children’s cognitive and behavioral reactions to an autonomous versus controlled social robot dog. Early Education and Development, 27(8), 1175-1189. |
[8] |
Chernyak, N., & Kushnir, T. (2013). Giving preschoolers choice increases sharing behavior. Psychological Science, 24(10), 1971-1979.
doi: 10.1177/0956797613482335 pmid: 23955355 |
[9] | Deng, S. C., Lin, Z. H., Lu, Y. Q., & Li, X. Q. (2023). New playmates in the age of intelligence: Characteristics of children’s interactions with robots and their impact on child development. Advances in Psychological Science, 31(12), 2319-2336. |
[邓士昌, 林子涵, 陆昱谦, 李象千. (2023). 智能时代的新玩伴:儿童与机器人的互动特征及其对儿童发展的影响. 心理科学进展, 31(12), 2319-2336.]
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2023.02319 |
|
[10] |
Dunfield, K. A., & Kuhlmeier, V. A. (2013). Classifying prosocial behavior: Children’s responses to instrumental need, emotional distress, and material desire. Child Development, 84(5), 1766-1776.
doi: 10.1111/cdev.12075 pmid: 23461793 |
[11] |
Eisenberg, N., & Miller, P. A. (1987). The relation of empathy to prosocial and related behaviors. Psychological Bulletin, 101(1), 91-119.
pmid: 3562705 |
[12] |
Eisenberg, N., VanSchyndel, S. K., & Spinrad, T. L. (2016). Prosocial motivation: Inferences from an opaque body of work. Child Development, 87(6), 1668-1678.
doi: 10.1111/cdev.12638 pmid: 28262941 |
[13] | Engelmann, J. M., Herrmann, E., & Tomasello, M. (2017). Concern for group reputation increases prosociality in young children. Psychological Science, 29(2), 181-190. |
[14] |
Engelmann, J. M., Over, H., Herrmann, E., & Tomasello, M. (2013). Young children care more about their reputation with ingroup members and potential reciprocators. Developmental Science, 16(6), 952-958.
doi: 10.1111/desc.12086 pmid: 24118719 |
[15] | Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G* Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175-191. |
[16] | Fehr, E., Bernhard, H., & Rockenbach, B. (2008). Egalitarianism in young children. Nature, 454(7208), 1079-1083. |
[17] | Fujisawa, K. K., Kutsukake, N., & Hasegawa, T. (2008). Reciprocity of prosocial behavior in Japanese preschool children. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 32(2), 89-97. |
[18] | Giannakos, M., Markopoulos, P., Hourcade, J. P., & Antle, A. N. (2022). “Lots done, more to do”: The current state of interaction design and children research and future directions. International Journal of Child-Computer Interaction, 33, 100469. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijcci.2022.100469 |
[19] | Gummerum, M., Hanoch, Y., & Keller, M. (2008). When child development meets economic game theory: An interdisciplinary approach to investigating social development. Human Development, 51(4), 235-261. |
[20] | Hamlin, J. K., Wynn, K., & Bloom, P. (2007). Social evaluation by preverbal infants. Nature, 450(7169), 557-559. |
[21] | House, B. R., Silk, J. B., Henrich, J., Barrett, H. C., Scelza, B. A., Boyette, A. H., … Laurence, S. (2013). Ontogeny of prosocial behavior across diverse societies. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(36), 14586-14591. |
[22] | Jipson, J. L., & Gelman, S. A. (2007). Robots and rodents: Children’s inferences about living and nonliving kinds. Child Development, 78(6), 1675-1688. |
[23] | Jipson, J. L., Gülgöz, S., & Gelman, S. A. (2016). Parent-child conversations regarding the ontological status of a robotic dog. Cognitive Development, 39, 21-35. |
[24] | Kim, M., Yi, S., & Lee, D. (2019). Between living and nonliving: Young children’s animacy judgments and reasoning about humanoid robots. PLoS ONE, 14(6), e0216869. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0216869 |
[25] | Lai, J. X., Yang, H., & Guo, L. P. (2012). On the preschool children’s sharing behavior and its implication. Studies in Early Childhood Education, 206(2), 20-26. |
[赖佳欣, 杨恒, 郭力平. (2012). 学前儿童分享行为的差异性. 学前教育研究, 206(2), 20-26.] | |
[26] |
Liu, B., Huang, Z., Xu, G., Jin, Y., Chen, Y., Li, X., … Jing, J. (2016). Altruistic sharing behavior in children: Role of theory of mind and inhibitory control. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 141, 222-228.
doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.09.010 pmid: 26452508 |
[27] |
Liu, L., Xiao, X., Liu, L., Xu, L., Zhang, X., & Li, Y. (2019). Children’s quality-based resource allocation in different involvement contexts: The role of in-group favoritism. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 51(5), 584-597.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2019.00584 |
[刘璐, 肖雪, 刘丽莎, 徐良苑, 张旭然, 李燕芳. (2019). 儿童在不同卷入情境下基于资源价值的分配行为特点: 内群体偏爱的作用. 心理学报, 51(5), 584-597.]
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2019.00584 |
|
[28] | Liu, Y., Odic, D., Tang, X., Ma, A., Laricheva, M., Chen, G.,... Milner-Bolotin, M. (2023). Effects of robotics education on young children’s cognitive development: A Pilot study with eye-tracking. Journal of Science Education and Technology, 32(3), 295-308. |
[29] | Malti, T., Gummerum, M., Ongley, S., Chaparro, M., Nola, M., & Bae, N. Y. (2016). “Who is worthy of my generosity?” Recipient characteristics and the development of children’s sharing. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 40(1), 31-40. |
[30] | Martin, D. U., MacIntyre, M. I., Perry, C., Clift, G., Pedell, S., & Kaufman, J. (2020). Young children’s indiscriminate helping behavior toward a humanoid robot. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 239. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00239 |
[31] | Michaelis, J. E., & Mutlu, B. (2018). Reading socially: Transforming the in-home reading experience with a learning-companion robot. Science Robotics, 3(21), eaat5999. https://doi.org/10.1126/scirobotics.aat5999 |
[32] | Nijssen, S. R. R., Müller, B. C. N., Bosse, T., & Paulus, M. (2021). You, robot? The role of anthropomorphic emotion attributions in children’s sharing with a robot. International Journal of Child-Computer Interaction, 30, 100319. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijcci.2021.100319 |
[33] | Okanda, M., Taniguchi, K., & Itakura, S. (2019). The role of animism tendencies and empathy in adult evaluations of robot. In Proceedings of the 7th international conference on human-agent interaction (pp. 51-58). Kyoto, Japan: Association for Computing Machinery. |
[34] | Okanda, M., Taniguchi, K., Wang, Y., & Itakura, S. (2021). Preschoolers’ and adults’ animism tendencies toward a humanoid robot. Computers in Human Behavior, 118, 106688. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2021.106688 |
[35] |
Okumura, Y., Hattori, T., Fujita, S., & Kobayashi, T. (2023). A robot is watching me!: Five-year-old children care about their reputation after interaction with a social robot. Child Development, 94(4), 865-873.
doi: 10.1111/cdev.13903 pmid: 36752147 |
[36] | Pang, Y., & Li, H. (2024). When the Recipient is a Social Robot: The impact of negative behavioral valence on 5-year-old children’s sharing. International Journal of Human- Computer Interaction. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1080/10447318.2024.2378247 |
[37] | Peter, J., Kühne, R., & Barco, A. (2021). Can social robots affect children’s prosocial behavior? An experimental study on prosocial robot models. Computers in Human Behavior, 120, 106712. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2021.106712 |
[38] | Piaget, J. (1929). The child’s conception of the world. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul. |
[39] | Rao, N., & Stewart, S. M. (1999). Cultural influences on sharer and recipient behavior: Sharing in Chinese and Indian preschool children. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 30(2), 219-241. |
[40] |
Schlottmann, A., & Ray, E. (2010). Goal attribution to schematic animals: Do 6-month-olds perceive biological motion as animate? Developmental Science, 13(1), 1-10.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00854.x pmid: 20121858 |
[41] | Schmidt, M. F. H., & Sommerville, J. A. (2011). Fairness expectations and altruistic sharing in 15-month-old human infants. PLoS ONE, 6(10), e23223. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0023223 |
[42] |
Somanader, M. C., Saylor, M. M., & Levin, D. T. (2011). Remote control and children’s understanding of robots. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 109(2), 239-247.
doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2011.01.005 pmid: 21349536 |
[43] |
Spinrad, T. L., & Gal, D. E. (2018). Fostering prosocial behavior and empathy in young children. Current Opinion in Psychology, 20, 40-44.
doi: S2352-250X(17)30128-8 pmid: 28830005 |
[44] | Su, Y. J., Zhang, H., & Zhang, K. (2012). Social decision- making: The equilibrium between self interest and the interests of others. Journal of Psychological Science, 35(6), 1423-1428. |
[苏彦捷, 张慧, 张康. (2012). 社会决策: 自我利益与他人利益的权衡. 心理科学, 35(6), 1423-1428.] | |
[45] | Tung, F. W. (2016). Child perception of humanoid robot appearance and behavior. International Journal of Human-Computer Interaction, 32(6), 493-502. |
[46] | Vonk, J., Jett, S. E., Tomeny, T. S., Mercer, S. H., & Cwikla, J. (2020). Young children’s theory of mind predicts more sharing with friends over time. Child Development, 91(1), 63-77. |
[47] |
Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2013). The emergence of contingent reciprocity in young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116(2), 338-350.
doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2013.06.002 pmid: 23917162 |
[48] | Xiong, M., Shi, J., Wu, Z., & Zhang, Z. (2016). Five-year-old preschoolers’ sharing is influenced by anticipated reciprocation. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 460. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00460 |
[1] | 蒯玲, 卫海英, 姚琦, 肖婷文, 谢升成. 时间标志对炫耀性亲社会行为意愿的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2025, 57(4): 526-543. |
[2] | 李曦, 纪林芹, 迟晓慧, 王舒冉, 张文新, 曹衍淼. 多巴胺系统基因调节青少年同伴拒绝与亲社会行为关系: 平行潜增长模型[J]. 心理学报, 2025, 57(4): 584-598. |
[3] | 聂衍刚, 陈沛, 王林欣, 喻承甫, 利振华. 父母情感温暖、自我控制与青少年亲社会行为的关系: 多基因的调节作用及父母差异[J]. 心理学报, 2025, 57(4): 599-613. |
[4] | 韩宪国, 金国敏, 李丹, 刘世宏, 吴琴, 刘俊升, 陈欣银. 父母温暖与儿童晚期亲社会行为的关系: 集体取向的作用[J]. 心理学报, 2025, 57(4): 614-630. |
[5] | 林荣茂, 余巧华, 胡添祥, 张九妹, 叶玉珊, 连榕. 敬畏感与亲社会行为关系的三水平和结构方程模型元分析[J]. 心理学报, 2025, 57(4): 631-651. |
[6] | 孙瑾, 杨静舒. 互惠利他的先行优势:品牌的互惠角色影响消费者亲社会行为[J]. 心理学报, 2025, 57(2): 315-330. |
[7] | 王天鸿, 金珊, 程子鹏, 娄宇, 谢晓非. 利他炫耀中的预测偏差:助人者低估来自旁观者的社会评价[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(9): 1210-1224. |
[8] | 王婧嫣, 张洪. 直觉还是推理?亲社会行为的决策模式与人性感知[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(9): 1225-1238. |
[9] | 石荣, 刘昌, 唐慧琳, 郝俊懿, 沈汪兵. 自发的善行:加工模式和情境紧急性影响亲社会行为[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(9): 1239-1251. |
[10] | 林靓, 徐博雅, 杨莹, 张庆鹏, 寇彧. 青少年亲社会行为的网络分析及核心维度[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(9): 1252-1265. |
[11] | 张环, 王晨, 李俊霞, 林琳, 吴捷. 情绪效价和动机强度对社会分享型提取诱发遗忘的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(8): 999-1014. |
[12] | 刘新燕, 伍海兰, 涂菊, 王璐. 一心多用的双刃剑效应:多任务对亲社会行为的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(12): 1800-1820. |
[13] | 赵纤, 胡义秋, 黎志华. 脱贫家庭青少年社会流动信念与亲社会行为的纵向发展关系:城乡差异的调节作用[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(11): 1499-1511. |
[14] | 张玮玮, 陈逸群, 朱莉琪. “近朱者赤”:同伴捐赠决策信息对青少年亲社会行为的影响及公正世界信念的作用[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(9): 1453-1464. |
[15] | 刘倩文, 王振宏. 亲子关系、感觉加工敏感性与COMT Val158Met多态性对学前儿童亲社会行为的交互影响[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(5): 711-725. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||