[1] |
Addleman, D. A., & Jiang, Y. V. (2019). Experience-driven auditory attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 23(11), 927-937. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2019.08.002
doi: S1364-6613(19)30203-7
URL
pmid: 31521482
|
[2] |
Allen, R. J., & Ueno, T. (2018). Multiple high-reward items can be prioritized in working memory but with greater vulnerability to interference. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 80(7), 1731-1743. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-018-1543-6
doi: 10.3758/s13414-018-1543-6
URL
|
[3] |
Atkinson, A. L., Allen, R. J., Baddeley, A. D., Hitch, G. J., & Waterman, A. H. (2021). Can valuable information be prioritized in verbal working memory? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 47(5), 747-764. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000979
doi: 10.1037/xlm0000979
URL
|
[4] |
Atkinson, A. L., Berry, E. D. J., Waterman, A. H., Baddeley, A. D., Hitch, G. J., & Allen, R. J. (2018). Are there multiple ways to direct attention in working memory? Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1424(1), 115-126. https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.13634
|
[5] |
Atkinson, A. L., Oberauer, K., Allen, R. J., & Souza, A. S. (2022). Why does the probe value effect emerge in working memory? Examining the biased attentional refreshing account. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 29(3), 891-900. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-022-02056-6
doi: 10.3758/s13423-022-02056-6
URL
|
[6] |
Atkinson, A. L., Waterman, A. H., & Allen, R. J. (2019). Can children prioritize more valuable information in working memory? An exploration into the effects of motivation and memory load. Developmental Psychology, 55(5), 967-980. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000692
doi: 10.1037/dev0000692
URL
pmid: 30816725
|
[7] |
Awh, E., Jonides, J., & Reuter-Lorenz, P. A. (1998). Rehearsal in spatial working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24(3), 780-790. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.24.3.780
doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.24.3.780
URL
|
[8] |
Barrouillet, P., & Camos, V. (2007). The time-based resource-sharing model of working memory. In N.Osaka, R. H.Logie, & M. D'Esposito (Eds.), The cognitive neuroscience of working memory. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198570394.003.0004
|
[9] |
Barrouillet, P., & Camos, V. (2012). As time goes by: Temporal constraints in working memory. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(6), 413-419. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721412459513
doi: 10.1177/0963721412459513
URL
|
[10] |
Camos, V., Johnson, M., Loaiza, V., Portrat, S., Souza, A., & Vergauwe, E. (2018). What is attentional refreshing in working memory? Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1424(1), 19-32. https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.13616
|
[11] |
Castel, A. D., Benjamin, A. S., Craik, F. I., & Watkins, M. J. (2002). The effects of aging on selectivity and control in short-term recall. Memory & Cognition, 30(7), 1078-1085. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03194325
doi: 10.3758/BF03194325
URL
|
[12] |
Cohen, M. S., Rissman, J., Hovhannisyan, M., Castel, A. D., & Knowlton, B. J. (2017). Free recall test experience potentiates strategy-driven effects of value on memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 43(10), 1581-1601. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000395
doi: 10.1037/xlm0000395
URL
|
[13] |
Hennessee, J. P., Patterson, T. K., Castel, A. D., & Knowlton, B. J. (2019). Forget me not: Encoding processes in value-directed remembering. Journal of Memory and Language, 106, 29-39. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2019.02.001
doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2019.02.001
URL
|
[14] |
Hitch, G. J., Hu, Y., Allen, R. J., & Baddeley, A. D. (2018). Competition for the focus of attention in visual working memory:Perceptual recency versus executive control. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1424(1), 64-75. https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.13631
|
[15] |
Hoover, M. A., & Richardson, D. C. (2008). When facts go down the rabbit hole: Contrasting features and objecthood as indexes to memory. Cognition, 108(2), 533-542. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2008.02.011.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2008.02.011
URL
pmid: 18423431
|
[16] |
Hu, Y., Allen, R. J., Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J. (2016). Executive control of stimulus-driven and goal-directed attention in visual working memory. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 78(7), 2164-2175. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-016-1106-7
doi: 10.3758/s13414-016-1106-7
URL
|
[17] |
Jafarpour, A., Penny, W., Barnes, G., Knight, R. T., & Duzel, E. (2017). Working memory replay prioritizes weakly attended events. eNeuro, 4(4), https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0171-17.2017
|
[18] |
Káldi, T., & Babarczy, A. (2021). Linguistic focus guides attention during the encoding and refreshing of working memory content. Journal of Memory and Language, 116, 104187. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2020.104187
doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2020.104187
URL
|
[19] |
Kaur, S., Norris, D. G., & Gathercole, S. E. (2020). The time course of updating in running span. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 46(12), 2397-2409. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000800
doi: 10.1037/xlm0000800
URL
|
[20] |
Kuo, B. C., Stokes, M. G., & Nobre, A. C. (2012). Attention modulates maintenance of representations in visual short-term memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24(1), 51-60. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00087.
doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00087
URL
|
[21] |
Lemaire, B., Pageot, A., Plancher, G., & Portrat, S. (2018). What is the time course of working memory attentional refreshing? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25(1), 370-385. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-017-1282-z
doi: 10.3758/s13423-017-1282-z
URL
|
[22] |
Martarelli, C. S., Chiquet, S., Laeng, B., & Mast, F. W. (2017). Using space to represent categories: Insights from gaze position. Psychological Research, 81(4), 721-729. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-016-0781-2.
doi: 10.1007/s00426-016-0781-2
URL
pmid: 27306547
|
[23] |
Oberauer, K., & Lewandowsky, S. (2011). Modeling working memory: A computational implementation of the time-based resource-sharing theory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18(1), 10-45. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-010-0020-6
doi: 10.3758/s13423-010-0020-6
URL
|
[24] |
Ricker, T. J., Sandry, J., Vergauwe, E., & Cowan, N. (2020). Do familiar memory items decay? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 46(1), 60-76. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000719
doi: 10.1037/xlm0000719
URL
|
[25] |
Sandry, J., & Ricker, T. J. (2020). Prioritization within visual working memory reflects a flexible focus of attention. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 82(6), 2985-3004. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-020-02049-4
doi: 10.3758/s13414-020-02049-4
URL
|
[26] |
Sandry, J., Schwark, J. D., & MacDonald, J. (2014). Flexibility within working memory and the focus of attention for sequential verbal information does not depend on active maintenance. Memory & Cognition, 42(7), 1130-1142. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-014-0422-1
doi: 10.3758/s13421-014-0422-1
URL
|
[27] |
Scholz, A., Klichowicz, A., & Krems, J. F. (2018). Covert shifts of attention can account for the functional role of "eye movements to nothing". Memory & Cognition, 46(2), 230-243. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-017-0760-x
doi: 10.3758/s13421-017-0760-x
URL
|
[28] |
Scholz, A., Mehlhorn, K., & Krems, J. F. (2016). Listen up, eye movements play a role in verbal memory retrieval. Psychological Research, 80(1), 149-158. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-014-0639-4.
doi: 10.1007/s00426-014-0639-4
URL
pmid: 25527078
|
[29] |
Spivey, M. J., & Geng, J. J. (2001). Oculomotor mechanisms activated by imagery and memory: Eye movements to absent objects. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 65(4), 235-241. https://doi.org/10.1007/s004260100059.
doi: 10.1007/s004260100059
URL
|
[30] |
Theeuwes, J. (2019). Goal-driven, stimulus-driven, and history-driven selection. Current Opinion in Psychology, 29, 97-101. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.12.024
doi: S2352-250X(18)30197-0
URL
pmid: 30711911
|
[31] |
Theeuwes, J., Belopolsky, A., & Olivers, C. N. L. (2009). Interactions between working memory, attention and eye movements. Acta Psychologica, 132(2), 106-114. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2009.01.005.
doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2009.01.005
URL
pmid: 19233340
|
[32] |
Thomas, P. M. J., Fitz Gibbon, L., & Raymond, J. E. (2016). Value conditioning modulates visual working memory processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42(1), 6-10. https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000144
doi: 10.1037/xhp0000144
URL
|
[33] |
Vankov, I. (2009). Mind the gap:The cost of looking at nothing, or the performance implications of memory- induced attention shifts. Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, 31, 1318-1323. https://escholarship.org/uc/item/7gm932vb
|
[34] |
van Moorselaar, D., Battistoni, E., Theeuwes, J., & Olivers, C. N. (2015). Rapid influences of cued visual memories on attentional guidance. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1339(1), 1-10. https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.12574
|
[35] |
Vergauwe, E., Camos, V., & Barrouillet, P. (2014). The impact of storage on processing: How is information maintained in working memory? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 40(4), 1072-1095. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035779
doi: 10.1037/a0035779
URL
|
[36] |
Yin, S., Sui, J., Chiu, Y. C., Chen, A., & Egner, T. (2019). Automatic prioritization of self-referential stimuli in working memory. Psychological Science, 30(3), 415-423. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797618818483
doi: 10.1177/0956797618818483
URL
pmid: 30653399
|