ISSN 0439-755X
CN 11-1911/B
主办:中国心理学会
   中国科学院心理研究所
出版:科学出版社

心理学报 ›› 2023, Vol. 55 ›› Issue (8): 1220-1233.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2023.01220

• 研究报告 • 上一篇    下一篇

返回抑制对视听跨通道对应的影响

祖光耀1, 李舒淇1, 张天阳2(), 王爱君1(), 张明1   

  1. 1苏州大学心理学系, 心理与行为科学研究中心, 江苏 苏州 215123
    2苏州大学公共卫生学院, 江苏 苏州 215123
  • 收稿日期:2022-09-27 发布日期:2023-05-12 出版日期:2023-08-25
  • 通讯作者: 王爱君, E-mail: ajwang@suda.edu.cn; 张天阳, E-mail: tyzhang@suda.edu.cn
  • 作者简介:第一联系人:

    李舒淇和祖光耀同为第一作者

  • 基金资助:
    教育部人文社会科学研究项目(22YJCZH243);苏州市医疗卫生科技创新项目(SKY2022113);国家自然科学基金项目(31800907);江苏高校哲学社会科学研究项目(2022SJYB1454);苏州大学人文社科青年交叉团队(2022)

Effect of inhibition of return on audiovisual cross-modal correspondence

ZU Guangyao1, LI Shuqi1, ZHANG Tianyang2(), WANG Aijun1(), ZHANG Ming1   

  1. 1Department of Psychology, Research Center for Psychology and Behavioral Sciences, Soochow University, Suzhou 215123, China
    2School of Public Health, Soochow University, Suzhou 215123, China
  • Received:2022-09-27 Online:2023-05-12 Published:2023-08-25

摘要:

视听跨通道对应(audiovisual crossmodal correspondence)在不同类型的视、听刺激之间被广泛发现, 但其发生阶段尚不明确。研究采用线索-靶子范式, 探究返回抑制(inhibition of return, IOR)对于视听跨通道对应的影响。实验1操纵线索和目标之间的空间位置一致性和听觉音调与视觉目标位置之间的跨通道对应一致性, 结果发现IOR效应和视听跨通道对应存在交互作用, 线索化位置存在稳定的视听跨通道对应效应, 而非线索化位置的视听跨通道对应效应消失。实验2操纵无关听觉刺激是否出现, 发现IOR效应与单独的声音出现与否不存在交互作用, 排除了警觉效应对于结果的混淆。实验3通过延长线索和目标的间隔时间(stimulus onset asynchronies, SOA), 发现伴随着IOR效应减弱, 线索化位置的视听跨通道对应效应也相应减弱, 且IOR效应对于视听跨通道对应的调节作用减弱。研究表明, 仅当听觉刺激与视觉空间位置之间发生跨通道对应时, 才会与同样发生在知觉水平的IOR效应产生交互, 支持了视听跨通道对应发生在知觉阶段。同时研究结果支持视听跨通道对应的发生符合反比效应原则。

关键词: 视听跨通道对应, 返回抑制, 线索-靶子范式, 警觉效应

Abstract:

Different dimensions of visual and auditory stimuli can map to each other to influence human behavioral responses, a phenomenon known as audiovisual cross-modal correspondence. A common audiovisual cross- modal correspondence is between auditory tones and visual spatial locations, with individuals tending to map high-pitched sounds to high spatial location and low-pitched sounds to low spatial location. When a high-pitch sound is accompanied or preceded by a visual stimulus, the participants respond faster to visual stimuli presented in the high spatial location than to visual stimuli presented in the low spatial location, and vice versa. Researchers have different views on the level at which audiovisual cross-modal correspondence occurs. Some argue that audiovisual cross-modal correspondence occurs at the perceptual level, increasing the perceptual saliency of the stimulus, while others argue that audiovisual cross-modal correspondence occurs at a later semantic or decision level. As inhibition of return (IOR) in the attentional system can affect human perception, this study used a cue-target paradigm to explore the interaction between IOR and audiovisual cross-modal correspondence to elucidate the occurrence level and mechanism of audiovisual cross-modal correspondence. Audiovisual cross-modal correspondence between auditory tones and visual spatial locations was expected to occur at the perceptual level and therefore would be subject to the IOR effect occurring at the same processing level.

The present study consisted of 3 experiments. Experiment 1 had a 2 × 2 within-subjects design; we manipulated the spatial cue validity (valid cue vs. invalid cue) and audiovisual cross-modal correspondence (congruent vs. incongruent). During the experiment, a fixation point was first presented in the middle of the screen for 750 ms. The box above or below the fixation point was then bolded for 50 ms, but this cue was not predictive of the spatial location of the target. After a time interval of 250 ms, a fixation point was presented in bold as a central cue. A central cue is commonly used in spatial IOR research, as it facilitates stable occurrence of IOR. The central cue was presented for 50 ms, and then the auditory stimulus (either high or low pitch) was presented for 50 ms. After a 200-ms interval, the visual target was presented for 100 ms in the box above or below the fixation point. The participants were instructed to perform a detection task for the presence of a visual target. The experimental design and procedure of Experiment 2 were identical to those of Experiment 1, except that the sound presented before the visual target was a single tone that was present or absent. Experiment 3 had a 2 × 2 × 2 within-subjects design. Experiment 3 added a factor to Experiment 1, namely, stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) between the cue and the target (600 ms vs. 1300 ms).

In all three experiments, the overall accuracy (ACC) was very high; thus, no further statistical analysis was conducted for the ACC. In terms of reaction time (RT), the results of Experiment 1 showed that both spatial IOR and audiovisual cross-modal correspondence occurred. Importantly, there was an interaction between spatial cue validity and audiovisual cross-modal correspondence. Specifically, when the cue was valid, audiovisual cross-modal correspondence occurred, and when the cue was invalid, there was no audiovisual cross-modal correspondence. The results of Experiment 2 showed that the interaction between cue validity and sound presentation was not significant, and there was no evidence that IOR influenced the sound-induced facilitation effect. The results of Experiment 3 showed that the interaction among spatial cue validity, cross-modal correspondence congruency, and SOA was significant. Specifically, at an SOA of 600 ms, the interaction between spatial cue validity and cross-modal correspondence congruency was significant. When the cue was valid, audiovisual cross-modal correspondence occurred, and when the cue was invalid, there was no audiovisual cross-modal correspondence. At an SOA of 1300 ms, the interaction between cue validity and cross-modal correspondence congruency was not significant, and cross-modal correspondence occurred in both valid-cue and invalid-cue conditions. The results of the analysis of the IOR effect showed that the IOR effect under the 600-ms SOA condition (22 ms) was significantly larger than that under the 1300-ms SOA condition (16 ms). As the SOA increased, the IOR effect size decreased.

In conclusion, the present results suggested that the IOR effect, occurring at the perceptual level, moderated audiovisual cross-modal correspondence. When the IOR effect occurred, audiovisual cross-modal correspondence occurred in the cued location, but not in the non-cued location. The alerting effect induced by the sound did not interact with IOR. With the weakening of the IOR effect, the audiovisual cross-modal correspondence in the cued location decreased, and the moderating effect of the IOR effect on audiovisual cross-modal correspondence weakened. The present results support that the audiovisual cross-modal correspondence between auditory tones and visual spatial locations occurs at the perceptual level, and the occurrence of audiovisual cross-modal correspondence conforms to the principle of inverse effectiveness.

Key words: audiovisual cross-modal correspondence, inhibition of return, cue-target paradigm, alerting effect

中图分类号: