心理学报 ›› 2022, Vol. 54 ›› Issue (9): 1009-1020.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2022.01009
• 研究报告 • 下一篇
收稿日期:
2021-04-12
发布日期:
2022-07-21
出版日期:
2022-09-25
通讯作者:
宋晓蕾
E-mail:songxiaolei@snnu.edu.cn
基金资助:
Received:
2021-04-12
Online:
2022-07-21
Published:
2022-09-25
Contact:
SONG Xiaolei
E-mail:songxiaolei@snnu.edu.cn
摘要:
本研究采用地图学习任务范式考察了追踪手势对视空间学习的影响和增强, 并试图从追踪手势的角度寻找能够有效促进操作者视空间学习的方法。实验1探究了产生追踪手势对视空间学习的增强作用; 实验2采用遮挡范式考察了产生追踪手势增强视空间学习的作用机制, 发现产生追踪手势所提供的视觉信息和感觉运动信息共同作用于视空间学习过程; 实验3从具身认知的视角探索了基于追踪手势的视空间学习增强方式, 发现以自我为参照产生追踪手势可以更好地增强个体的视空间学习。本研究结果很好地支持并补充了手势的具身认知理论。
中图分类号:
贾筱倩, 宋晓蕾. (2022). 追踪手势对视空间学习的增强作用. 心理学报, 54(9), 1009-1020.
JIA Xiaoqian, SONG Xiaolei. (2022). The enhancing effect of tracking gesture on visuo-spatial learning. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 54(9), 1009-1020.
类型 | 定义 |
---|---|
追踪手势 | 追踪手势也称为指示性手势(deictic gesture), 主要是指向某方向或某物体的手势, 可以用来表示事物的相对位置, 也可以是指向的动作, 与空间信息相关, 可以支持视觉空间处理(So et al., |
描述性手势 | 描述性手势也称为表象性手势(representational gestures), 主要通过提供某种视觉动作来描述某个具体对象。描述性手势提供了额外的信息, 能帮助处理更深层的意义或语境(Alibali & Nathan, |
隐喻性手势 | 隐喻性手势传达的是一种没有物理形式抽象的信息, 比如, “故事一直在继续”, 同时说话人的手模仿着滚动的动作。 |
节拍手势 | 节拍手势是一种具有节奏性的动作, 不包含明显的语义信息, 强调语音的韵律或节奏。例如, 在讲“书”一词的同时, 说话者的手也在快速移动。这种手势也被称为强调手势, 它具有元叙事功能, 反映了话语的结构。 |
表1 手势类型
类型 | 定义 |
---|---|
追踪手势 | 追踪手势也称为指示性手势(deictic gesture), 主要是指向某方向或某物体的手势, 可以用来表示事物的相对位置, 也可以是指向的动作, 与空间信息相关, 可以支持视觉空间处理(So et al., |
描述性手势 | 描述性手势也称为表象性手势(representational gestures), 主要通过提供某种视觉动作来描述某个具体对象。描述性手势提供了额外的信息, 能帮助处理更深层的意义或语境(Alibali & Nathan, |
隐喻性手势 | 隐喻性手势传达的是一种没有物理形式抽象的信息, 比如, “故事一直在继续”, 同时说话人的手模仿着滚动的动作。 |
节拍手势 | 节拍手势是一种具有节奏性的动作, 不包含明显的语义信息, 强调语音的韵律或节奏。例如, 在讲“书”一词的同时, 说话者的手也在快速移动。这种手势也被称为强调手势, 它具有元叙事功能, 反映了话语的结构。 |
[1] |
Alibali, M. W., Heath, D. C., & Myers, H. J. (2001). Effects of visibility between speaker and listener on gesture production: Some gestures are meant to be seen. Journal of Memory and Language, 44(2), 169-188.
doi: 10.1006/jmla.2000.2752 URL |
[2] |
Alibali, M. W., & Nathan, M. J. (2012). Embodiment in mathematics teaching and learning: Evidence from learners’ and teachers’ gestures. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 21(2), 247-286.
doi: 10.1080/10508406.2011.611446 URL |
[3] |
Austin, E. E., & Sweller, N. (2014). Presentation and production: The role of gesture in spatial communication. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 122, 92-103.
doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2013.12.008 URL |
[4] |
Austin, E. E., & Sweller, N. (2017). Getting to the elephants: Gesture and preschoolers' comprehension of route direction information. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 163, 1-14.
doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2017.05.016 URL |
[5] |
Ayaz, H., Allen, S., Platek, S., & Onaral, B. (2008). Maze Suite 1.0: A complete set of tools to prepare, present, and analyze navigational and spatial cognitive neuroscience experiments. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 353-359.
doi: 10.3758/BRM.40.1.353 URL |
[6] |
Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory: Looking back and looking forward. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4(10), 829-839.
pmid: 14523382 |
[7] |
Bredo, E. (1994). Reconstructing educational psychology: Situated cognition and Deweyian pragmatism. Educational Psychologist, 29(1), 23-35.
doi: 10.1207/s15326985ep2901_3 URL |
[8] |
Brunyé, T. T., Rapp, D. N., & Taylor, H. A. (2008). Representational flexibility and specificity following spatial descriptions of real world environments. Cognition, 108(2), 418-443.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2008.03.005 pmid: 18430411 |
[9] | Buccino, G., Binkofski, F., & Riggioa, L. (2004). The mirror neuron system and action recognition. Brain & Language, 89(2), 370-376. |
[10] |
Cassell, J., & McNeill, D. (1991). Gesture and the poetics of prose. Poetics Today, 12(3), 375-404.
doi: 10.2307/1772644 URL |
[11] | Cattaneo, Z., Fastame, M., Cornoldi, C., & Vecchi, T. (2006). Working memory, imagery and visuo-spatial mechanisms. Imagery and Spatial Cognition: Methods, Models and Cognitive Assessment, 66, 101-137. |
[12] | Clark, A. (2008). Supersizing the mind: Embodiment, action, and cognitive extension. New York: Oxford University Press. |
[13] | Clark, A. (2013). The hand, an organ of the mind: what the manual tells the mental. Cambridge: MIT Press. |
[14] |
Coluccia, E., & Louse, G. (2004). Gender differences in spatial orientation: A review. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 24(3), 329-340.
doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2004.08.006 URL |
[15] |
Demiris, Y., & Simmons, G. (2006). Perceiving the unusual: Temporal properties of hierarchical motor representations for action perception. Neural Networks, 19(3), 272-284.
doi: 10.1016/j.neunet.2006.02.005 URL |
[16] |
de Nooijer, J. A., van Gog, T., Paas, F., & Zwaan, R. A. (2013). Effects of imitating gestures during encoding or during retrieval of novel verbs on children's test performance. Acta Psychologica, 144(1), 173-179.
doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.05.013 URL |
[17] |
Gazzola, V., Aziz-Zadeh, L., & Keysers, C. (2006). Empathy and the somatotopic auditory mirror system in humans. Current Biology, 16(18), 1824-1829.
doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.07.072 URL |
[18] |
Goldenberg, G., & Karnath, H.-O. (2006). The neural basis of imitation is body part specific. Journal of Neuroscience, 26(23), 6282-6287.
doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0638-06.2006 pmid: 16763035 |
[19] |
Goldin-Meadow, S. (2011). Learning through gesture. WIREs Cognitive Science, 2(6), 595-607.
doi: 10.1002/wcs.132 URL |
[20] |
Goldin-Meadow, S., Beilock, S. L. (2010). Action’s influence on thought: The case of Gesture. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5(6), 664-674.
doi: 10.1177/1745691610388764 pmid: 21572548 |
[21] | Jeffreys, H. (1961). Theory of probability (3rd ed). Oxford: Clarendon Press. |
[22] | Li, H. Y., & Lin, C. D. (2005). A structural research on the spatial-cognitive ability of high school students. Journal of Psychological Science, 28(2), 269-271. |
[李洪玉, 林崇德. (2005). 中学生空间认知能力结构的研究. 心理科学, 28(2), 269-271.] | |
[23] |
Mathewson, J. H. (1999). Visual-spatial thinking: An aspect of science overlooked by educators. Science Education, 83(1), 33-54.
doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-237X(199901)83:1<33::AID-SCE2>3.0.CO;2-Z URL |
[24] |
Marstaller, L., & Buríanová, H. (2013). Individual differences in the gesture effect on working memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20, 496-500.
doi: 10.3758/s13423-012-0365-0 URL |
[25] |
Meneghetti, C., Zancada-Menéndez, C., Sampedro-Piquero, P., Lopez, L., Martinelli, M., Ronconi, L., & Rossi, B. (2016). Mental representations derived from navigation: The role of visuo-spatial abilities and working memory. Learning and Individual Differences, 49, 314-322.
doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2016.07.002 URL |
[26] | Pouw, W., & Hostetter, A. (2016). Gesture as predictive action. Reti, saperi, linguaggi: Italian Journal of Cognitive Sciences, 3(1), 57-80. |
[27] |
Pouw, W. T. J. L., van Gog, T., & Paas, F. (2014). An embedded and embodied cognition review of instructional manipulatives. Educational Psychology Review, 26, 51-72.
doi: 10.1007/s10648-014-9255-5 URL |
[28] | Ruddle, R. A., & Lessels, S. (2009). The benefits of using a walking interface to navigate virtual environments. ACM Transactions on Computer-Human Interaction, 16(1), 135-152. |
[29] |
Sandstrom, N. J., Kaufman, J., & Huettel, S. A. (1998). Males and females use different distal cues in a virtual environment navigation task. Cognitive Brain Research, 6(4), 351-360.
pmid: 9593991 |
[30] | So, W. C., Ching, T. H., Lim, P. E., Cheng, X., & Ip, K. Y. (2014). Producing gestures facilitates route learning. PLoS ONE, 9, 1-21. |
[31] |
So, W. C., Shum, P. L. C., & Wong, M. K. Y. (2015). Gesture is more effective than spatial language in encoding spatial information. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 68(12), 2384-2401.
doi: 10.1080/17470218.2015.1015431 URL |
[32] | Uttal, D. H., & O’Doherty, K. (2008). Comprehending and learning from ‘visualizations’:A developmental perspective. In J. K. Gilbert, M. Reiner, & M. Nakhleh (Eds.), Visualization: theory and practice in science education: models and modeling in science education (Vol. 3, pp. 53-72). Springer, Dordrecht. |
[33] |
van der Ham, I. J. M., Claessen, M. H. G., Evers, A. W. M., & van der Kuil, M. N. A. (2020). Large-scale assessment of human navigation ability across the lifespan. Scientific Reports, 10(1), 3299.
doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-60302-0 URL |
[34] |
Wai, J., Lubinski, D., & Benbow, C. P. (2009). Spatial ability for STEM domains: Aligning over 50 years of cumulative psychological knowledge solidifies its importance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101(4), 817-835.
doi: 10.1037/a0016127 URL |
[35] |
Waller, D., Loomis, J. M., & Haun, D. B. (2004). Body-based senses enhance knowledge of directions in large-scale environments. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11(1), 157-163.
doi: 10.3758/BF03206476 URL |
[36] |
Wermeskerken, M., Fijan, N., Eielts, C., & Pouw, W. T. J. L. (2016). Observation of depictive versus tracing gestures selectively aids verbal versus visual-spatial learning in primary school children. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 30(5), 806-814.
doi: 10.1002/acp.3256 URL |
[37] |
Wesp, R. K., Hesse, J., Keutmann, D., & Wheaton, K. (2001). Gestures maintain spatial imagery. The American Journal of Psychology, 114(4), 591-600.
doi: 10.2307/1423612 URL |
[38] |
Wilson, M. (2002). Six views of embodied cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9(4), 625.
doi: 10.3758/BF03196322 URL |
[39] | Zhao, W., & Tian, C. (2008). The CHC theory and its application in assessment and educational intervention to children with learning disabilities. Chinese Journal of Special Education, (5), 47-52. |
[赵微, 田创. (2008). CHC理论及其在学习困难儿童评估与教育干预中的应用. 中国特殊教育, (5), 47-52.] |
[1] | 朱一鸣, 赵阳, 唐宁, 周吉帆, 沈模卫. 笔画节点在手写体汉字识别中的作用[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(12): 1903-1916. |
[2] | 尹华站, 张丽, 刘鹏玉, 李丹. 负性情绪的动机维度对时距知觉的影响:注意控制和注意偏向的中介作用[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(12): 1917-1931. |
[3] | 钟毅平, 牛娜娜, 范伟, 任梦梦, 李梅. 动作自主性与社会距离对主动控制感的影响:来自行为与ERPs的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(12): 1932-1948. |
[4] | 徐楚言, 朱麟, 王芸萍, 王瑞冰, 刘聪慧. 外语口语焦虑对言语互动质量的影响:fNIRS超扫描研究[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(12): 1949-1965. |
[5] | 汪海玲, 陈恩光, 连玉净, 李晶晶, 王丽薇. 面孔宽高比的自动加工[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(11): 1745-1761. |
[6] | 张航, 王婷, 冯晓慧, 韦义平, 张积家. 铜鼓经验对壮族鼓手的节奏知觉和执行功能的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(11): 1762-1779. |
[7] | 孙亚茹, 刘泽军, 段亚杰, 陈宁, 刘伟. 协作如何减少记忆错误:一项元分析研究[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(11): 1780-1792. |
[8] | 胡馨允, 沈悦, 戴俊毅. 系列决策任务中的策略转换:来自爱荷华赌博任务的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(11): 1793-1805. |
[9] | 邱慧燕, 吕勇. 情绪显著性对情绪诱发视盲的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(11): 1806-1814. |
[10] | 李梅, 李琎, 张冠斐, 钟毅平, 李红. 承诺水平与社会距离对信任投资的影响:来自行为与ERPs的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(11): 1859-1871. |
[11] | 王阳, 张琳爽, 崔楠楠, 吴岩. 4~6岁幼儿口语产生中句法结构和动词重复的作用:来自句法启动的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(10): 1608-1619. |
[12] | 李庆功, 方溦, 胡超, 石德君, 胡晓晴, 傅根跃, 王乾东. 灰姑娘能变成白雪公主吗?感知到的信任对他人面孔表征的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(9): 1518-1528. |
[13] | 庞超, 陈颜璋, 王莉, 杨喜端, 贺雅, 李芷莹, 欧阳小钰, 傅世敏, 南威治. 客体信息在视觉工作记忆编码和维持阶段的不同注意选择模式[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(9): 1397-1410. |
[14] | 蔡文琦, 张向阳, 王小娟, 杨剑峰. 汉语复合词语素意义与整词语义整合加工的时间进程[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(8): 1207-1219. |
[15] | 祖光耀, 李舒淇, 张天阳, 王爱君, 张明. 返回抑制对视听跨通道对应的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(8): 1220-1233. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||