心理学报 ›› 2022, Vol. 54 ›› Issue (9): 1009-1020.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2022.01009 cstr: 32110.14.2022.01009
• 研究报告 • 下一篇
收稿日期:
2021-04-12
发布日期:
2022-07-21
出版日期:
2022-09-25
基金资助:
Received:
2021-04-12
Online:
2022-07-21
Published:
2022-09-25
摘要:
本研究采用地图学习任务范式考察了追踪手势对视空间学习的影响和增强, 并试图从追踪手势的角度寻找能够有效促进操作者视空间学习的方法。实验1探究了产生追踪手势对视空间学习的增强作用; 实验2采用遮挡范式考察了产生追踪手势增强视空间学习的作用机制, 发现产生追踪手势所提供的视觉信息和感觉运动信息共同作用于视空间学习过程; 实验3从具身认知的视角探索了基于追踪手势的视空间学习增强方式, 发现以自我为参照产生追踪手势可以更好地增强个体的视空间学习。本研究结果很好地支持并补充了手势的具身认知理论。
中图分类号:
贾筱倩, 宋晓蕾. (2022). 追踪手势对视空间学习的增强作用. 心理学报, 54(9), 1009-1020.
JIA Xiaoqian, SONG Xiaolei. (2022). The enhancing effect of tracking gesture on visuo-spatial learning. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 54(9), 1009-1020.
类型 | 定义 |
---|---|
追踪手势 | 追踪手势也称为指示性手势(deictic gesture), 主要是指向某方向或某物体的手势, 可以用来表示事物的相对位置, 也可以是指向的动作, 与空间信息相关, 可以支持视觉空间处理(So et al., |
描述性手势 | 描述性手势也称为表象性手势(representational gestures), 主要通过提供某种视觉动作来描述某个具体对象。描述性手势提供了额外的信息, 能帮助处理更深层的意义或语境(Alibali & Nathan, |
隐喻性手势 | 隐喻性手势传达的是一种没有物理形式抽象的信息, 比如, “故事一直在继续”, 同时说话人的手模仿着滚动的动作。 |
节拍手势 | 节拍手势是一种具有节奏性的动作, 不包含明显的语义信息, 强调语音的韵律或节奏。例如, 在讲“书”一词的同时, 说话者的手也在快速移动。这种手势也被称为强调手势, 它具有元叙事功能, 反映了话语的结构。 |
表1 手势类型
类型 | 定义 |
---|---|
追踪手势 | 追踪手势也称为指示性手势(deictic gesture), 主要是指向某方向或某物体的手势, 可以用来表示事物的相对位置, 也可以是指向的动作, 与空间信息相关, 可以支持视觉空间处理(So et al., |
描述性手势 | 描述性手势也称为表象性手势(representational gestures), 主要通过提供某种视觉动作来描述某个具体对象。描述性手势提供了额外的信息, 能帮助处理更深层的意义或语境(Alibali & Nathan, |
隐喻性手势 | 隐喻性手势传达的是一种没有物理形式抽象的信息, 比如, “故事一直在继续”, 同时说话人的手模仿着滚动的动作。 |
节拍手势 | 节拍手势是一种具有节奏性的动作, 不包含明显的语义信息, 强调语音的韵律或节奏。例如, 在讲“书”一词的同时, 说话者的手也在快速移动。这种手势也被称为强调手势, 它具有元叙事功能, 反映了话语的结构。 |
[1] |
Alibali, M. W., Heath, D. C., & Myers, H. J. (2001). Effects of visibility between speaker and listener on gesture production: Some gestures are meant to be seen. Journal of Memory and Language, 44(2), 169-188.
doi: 10.1006/jmla.2000.2752 URL |
[2] |
Alibali, M. W., & Nathan, M. J. (2012). Embodiment in mathematics teaching and learning: Evidence from learners’ and teachers’ gestures. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 21(2), 247-286.
doi: 10.1080/10508406.2011.611446 URL |
[3] |
Austin, E. E., & Sweller, N. (2014). Presentation and production: The role of gesture in spatial communication. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 122, 92-103.
doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2013.12.008 URL |
[4] |
Austin, E. E., & Sweller, N. (2017). Getting to the elephants: Gesture and preschoolers' comprehension of route direction information. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 163, 1-14.
doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2017.05.016 URL |
[5] |
Ayaz, H., Allen, S., Platek, S., & Onaral, B. (2008). Maze Suite 1.0: A complete set of tools to prepare, present, and analyze navigational and spatial cognitive neuroscience experiments. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 353-359.
doi: 10.3758/BRM.40.1.353 URL |
[6] |
Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory: Looking back and looking forward. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4(10), 829-839.
pmid: 14523382 |
[7] |
Bredo, E. (1994). Reconstructing educational psychology: Situated cognition and Deweyian pragmatism. Educational Psychologist, 29(1), 23-35.
doi: 10.1207/s15326985ep2901_3 URL |
[8] |
Brunyé, T. T., Rapp, D. N., & Taylor, H. A. (2008). Representational flexibility and specificity following spatial descriptions of real world environments. Cognition, 108(2), 418-443.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2008.03.005 pmid: 18430411 |
[9] | Buccino, G., Binkofski, F., & Riggioa, L. (2004). The mirror neuron system and action recognition. Brain & Language, 89(2), 370-376. |
[10] |
Cassell, J., & McNeill, D. (1991). Gesture and the poetics of prose. Poetics Today, 12(3), 375-404.
doi: 10.2307/1772644 URL |
[11] | Cattaneo, Z., Fastame, M., Cornoldi, C., & Vecchi, T. (2006). Working memory, imagery and visuo-spatial mechanisms. Imagery and Spatial Cognition: Methods, Models and Cognitive Assessment, 66, 101-137. |
[12] | Clark, A. (2008). Supersizing the mind: Embodiment, action, and cognitive extension. New York: Oxford University Press. |
[13] | Clark, A. (2013). The hand, an organ of the mind: what the manual tells the mental. Cambridge: MIT Press. |
[14] |
Coluccia, E., & Louse, G. (2004). Gender differences in spatial orientation: A review. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 24(3), 329-340.
doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2004.08.006 URL |
[15] |
Demiris, Y., & Simmons, G. (2006). Perceiving the unusual: Temporal properties of hierarchical motor representations for action perception. Neural Networks, 19(3), 272-284.
doi: 10.1016/j.neunet.2006.02.005 URL |
[16] |
de Nooijer, J. A., van Gog, T., Paas, F., & Zwaan, R. A. (2013). Effects of imitating gestures during encoding or during retrieval of novel verbs on children's test performance. Acta Psychologica, 144(1), 173-179.
doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.05.013 URL |
[17] |
Gazzola, V., Aziz-Zadeh, L., & Keysers, C. (2006). Empathy and the somatotopic auditory mirror system in humans. Current Biology, 16(18), 1824-1829.
doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.07.072 URL |
[18] |
Goldenberg, G., & Karnath, H.-O. (2006). The neural basis of imitation is body part specific. Journal of Neuroscience, 26(23), 6282-6287.
doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0638-06.2006 pmid: 16763035 |
[19] |
Goldin-Meadow, S. (2011). Learning through gesture. WIREs Cognitive Science, 2(6), 595-607.
doi: 10.1002/wcs.132 URL |
[20] |
Goldin-Meadow, S., Beilock, S. L. (2010). Action’s influence on thought: The case of Gesture. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5(6), 664-674.
doi: 10.1177/1745691610388764 pmid: 21572548 |
[21] | Jeffreys, H. (1961). Theory of probability (3rd ed). Oxford: Clarendon Press. |
[22] | Li, H. Y., & Lin, C. D. (2005). A structural research on the spatial-cognitive ability of high school students. Journal of Psychological Science, 28(2), 269-271. |
[李洪玉, 林崇德. (2005). 中学生空间认知能力结构的研究. 心理科学, 28(2), 269-271.] | |
[23] |
Mathewson, J. H. (1999). Visual-spatial thinking: An aspect of science overlooked by educators. Science Education, 83(1), 33-54.
doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-237X(199901)83:1<33::AID-SCE2>3.0.CO;2-Z URL |
[24] |
Marstaller, L., & Buríanová, H. (2013). Individual differences in the gesture effect on working memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20, 496-500.
doi: 10.3758/s13423-012-0365-0 URL |
[25] |
Meneghetti, C., Zancada-Menéndez, C., Sampedro-Piquero, P., Lopez, L., Martinelli, M., Ronconi, L., & Rossi, B. (2016). Mental representations derived from navigation: The role of visuo-spatial abilities and working memory. Learning and Individual Differences, 49, 314-322.
doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2016.07.002 URL |
[26] | Pouw, W., & Hostetter, A. (2016). Gesture as predictive action. Reti, saperi, linguaggi: Italian Journal of Cognitive Sciences, 3(1), 57-80. |
[27] |
Pouw, W. T. J. L., van Gog, T., & Paas, F. (2014). An embedded and embodied cognition review of instructional manipulatives. Educational Psychology Review, 26, 51-72.
doi: 10.1007/s10648-014-9255-5 URL |
[28] | Ruddle, R. A., & Lessels, S. (2009). The benefits of using a walking interface to navigate virtual environments. ACM Transactions on Computer-Human Interaction, 16(1), 135-152. |
[29] |
Sandstrom, N. J., Kaufman, J., & Huettel, S. A. (1998). Males and females use different distal cues in a virtual environment navigation task. Cognitive Brain Research, 6(4), 351-360.
pmid: 9593991 |
[30] | So, W. C., Ching, T. H., Lim, P. E., Cheng, X., & Ip, K. Y. (2014). Producing gestures facilitates route learning. PLoS ONE, 9, 1-21. |
[31] |
So, W. C., Shum, P. L. C., & Wong, M. K. Y. (2015). Gesture is more effective than spatial language in encoding spatial information. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 68(12), 2384-2401.
doi: 10.1080/17470218.2015.1015431 URL |
[32] | Uttal, D. H., & O’Doherty, K. (2008). Comprehending and learning from ‘visualizations’:A developmental perspective. In J. K. Gilbert, M. Reiner, & M. Nakhleh (Eds.), Visualization: theory and practice in science education: models and modeling in science education (Vol. 3, pp. 53-72). Springer, Dordrecht. |
[33] |
van der Ham, I. J. M., Claessen, M. H. G., Evers, A. W. M., & van der Kuil, M. N. A. (2020). Large-scale assessment of human navigation ability across the lifespan. Scientific Reports, 10(1), 3299.
doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-60302-0 URL |
[34] |
Wai, J., Lubinski, D., & Benbow, C. P. (2009). Spatial ability for STEM domains: Aligning over 50 years of cumulative psychological knowledge solidifies its importance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101(4), 817-835.
doi: 10.1037/a0016127 URL |
[35] |
Waller, D., Loomis, J. M., & Haun, D. B. (2004). Body-based senses enhance knowledge of directions in large-scale environments. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11(1), 157-163.
doi: 10.3758/BF03206476 URL |
[36] |
Wermeskerken, M., Fijan, N., Eielts, C., & Pouw, W. T. J. L. (2016). Observation of depictive versus tracing gestures selectively aids verbal versus visual-spatial learning in primary school children. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 30(5), 806-814.
doi: 10.1002/acp.3256 URL |
[37] |
Wesp, R. K., Hesse, J., Keutmann, D., & Wheaton, K. (2001). Gestures maintain spatial imagery. The American Journal of Psychology, 114(4), 591-600.
doi: 10.2307/1423612 URL |
[38] |
Wilson, M. (2002). Six views of embodied cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9(4), 625.
doi: 10.3758/BF03196322 URL |
[39] | Zhao, W., & Tian, C. (2008). The CHC theory and its application in assessment and educational intervention to children with learning disabilities. Chinese Journal of Special Education, (5), 47-52. |
[赵微, 田创. (2008). CHC理论及其在学习困难儿童评估与教育干预中的应用. 中国特殊教育, (5), 47-52.] |
[1] | 华山, 姜欣桐, 高扬震宇, 穆妍, 杜忆. 音乐训练与音乐素养对共情能力的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2025, 57(4): 544-558. |
[2] | 张环, 秦锡权, 刘雨, 林琳, 吴捷. 不同强度趋近动机积极情绪对基于语义关联性错误记忆的影响及其神经机制[J]. 心理学报, 2025, 57(3): 349-362. |
[3] | 张俐娟, 江妍雪, 马建平, 崔博洋, 张锦坤. 提取难度对困难材料提取练习效应的促进作用:来自行为和fNIRS的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2025, 57(3): 363-379. |
[4] | 谢晓玲, 潘文谊, 张纯纯, 林静远, 李红. 连续反馈影响主导感的心理与脑电机制[J]. 心理学报, 2025, 57(3): 380-397. |
[5] | 李春好, 刘荣媛, 刘远豪. 经典和对偶共结果效应对前景集结果区间的依赖性:基于概率权重的视角[J]. 心理学报, 2025, 57(3): 398-414. |
[6] | 连浩敏, 张倩, 谷雪敏, 李寿欣. 持续性视觉注意对视觉工作记忆项目优先加工的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2025, 57(2): 191-206. |
[7] | 史滋福, 夏笔奇, 刘欣, 陈火红, 靳紫阳, 彭玲艺. 时间预测中的任务分解效应:未来边界和思维焦点的作用[J]. 心理学报, 2025, 57(2): 207-217. |
[8] | 郝磊, 许天委, 周文龙, 杨杰, 彭思雅, 刘明兰, 徐家华, 王延培, 谭淑平, 高家红, 贺永, 陶沙, 董奇, 秦绍正. 多种情感与认知任务驱动下大脑可泛化神经表征的发育模式[J]. 心理学报, 2025, 57(2): 218-231. |
[9] | 孙岩, 王艺锦, 侯沛雨, 冯雪, 兰帆. 抑郁倾向对自我关注重评和情境关注重评影响的脑网络研究[J]. 心理学报, 2025, 57(2): 232-246. |
[10] | 夏莲香, 刘凯歌, 李新宇, 叶群. 编码方式与叙事连贯性调节情绪对时间顺序记忆的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2025, 57(1): 1-17. |
[11] | 俞梦霞, 宋宜颖, 刘嘉. 实地导航训练提高大脑功能连接模式稳定性[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(12): 1661-1675. |
[12] | 赵文博, 曹宇琪, 徐木子, 杨春亮, 罗良. 学习判断对虚假记忆的反应性效应[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(12): 1691-1705. |
[13] | 王铖铖, 赵宇飞, 盛缨莹, 赵庆柏, 肖梦施, 韩磊. 数字SNARC效应的发生阶段[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(12): 1706-1717. |
[14] | 李想, 贾丽娜, 魏士琳, 陈俊涛, 夏尧远, 王芹, 金花. 运动特征对抽象动词具身表征的影响:来自fMRI及EMG的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(12): 1718-1733. |
[15] | 梁菲菲, 刘瑛, 贺斐, 冯琳琳, 王峥, 白学军. 中文阅读伴随词汇学习中的视觉复杂性效应:基于笔画数和词长的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(12): 1734-1750. |
阅读次数 | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
全文 5285
|
|
|||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
摘要 |
|
|||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||