心理学报 ›› 2025, Vol. 57 ›› Issue (4): 544-558.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2025.0544 cstr: 32110.14.2025.0544
华山1,2,†, 姜欣桐1,2,†, 高扬震宇4, 穆妍1,2(), 杜忆1,2,3(
)
收稿日期:
2023-11-14
发布日期:
2025-02-06
出版日期:
2025-04-25
通讯作者:
杜忆, E-mail: duyi@psych.ac.cn;作者简介:
第一联系人:†华山和姜欣桐为共同第一作者
基金资助:
HUA Shan1,2,†, JIANG Xintong1,2,†, GAO Yangzhenyu4, MU Yan1,2(), DU Yi1,2,3(
)
Received:
2023-11-14
Online:
2025-02-06
Published:
2025-04-25
摘要:
共情能力是预测和促进亲社会行为的重要因素, 音乐经验对共情能力的发展有着重要作用。本研究以特质共情和状态共情两个方面为切入点, 采用音乐训练和音乐素养两个指标衡量个体音乐经验, 利用问卷调查和行为实验, 探究它们对共情能力的影响路径。结果表明,在特质共情方面, 音乐家的认知共情能力显著高于非音乐家。然而, 在控制了人格、主观社会经济地位和心理健康状态的相关变量后, 音乐训练对共情能力并无显著影响。相反, 音乐素养显著正向预测认知共情能力的想象维度, 甚至可以作为音乐训练影响认知共情中想象维度的中介变量。在状态共情方面, 利用疼痛共情范式研究发现, 相比非音乐家, 音乐家在目睹他人疼痛情境时更能感同身受, 这种状态共情能力受到音乐素养和特质认知共情中想象维度的链式中介影响。综上所述, 音乐训练通过提高音乐素养, 间接增强特质共情中的认知共情能力(想象), 从而使音乐家在感同身受他人疼痛状态时表现出更强的能力。这些发现揭示了音乐经验对共情能力的促进机制。
中图分类号:
华山, 姜欣桐, 高扬震宇, 穆妍, 杜忆. (2025). 音乐训练与音乐素养对共情能力的影响. 心理学报, 57(4), 544-558.
HUA Shan, JIANG Xintong, GAO Yangzhenyu, MU Yan, DU Yi. (2025). The impacts of music training and music sophistication on empathy. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 57(4), 544-558.
变量 | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | 10 | 11 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. 音乐素养 | — | ||||||||||
2. 观点采择 | 0.30*** | — | |||||||||
3. 想象 | 0.39*** | 0.39*** | — | ||||||||
4. 同情关心 | 0.18*** | 0.30*** | 0.58*** | — | |||||||
5. 个人痛苦 | −0.22** | −0.14* | −0.15* | −0.10 | — | ||||||
6. 开放性 | 0.45*** | 0.34*** | 0.36*** | 0.19** | −0.22*** | — | |||||
7. 主观社会经济地位 | 0.56*** | 0.27*** | 0.23*** | 0.16* | −0.17** | 0.39*** | — | ||||
8. 状态焦虑 | −0.29*** | −0.37*** | −0.32*** | −0.24*** | 0.43*** | −0.35*** | −0.29*** | — | |||
9. 特质焦虑 | −0.32*** | −0.36*** | −0.37*** | −0.26*** | 0.56*** | −0.38*** | −0.33*** | 0.85*** | — | ||
10. 述情障碍 | −0.25*** | −0.37*** | −0.39*** | −0.33*** | 0.63*** | −0.30*** | −0.20** | 0.47*** | 0.57*** | — | |
11. 抑郁 | −0.22*** | −0.24*** | −0.21** | −0.21*** | 0.51*** | −0.31*** | −0.29*** | 0.55*** | 0.66*** | 0.54*** | — |
平均值 | 77.08 | 14.75 | 16.73 | 16.34 | 9.37 | 9.92 | 44.76 | 34.67 | 37.68 | 48.38 | 4.09 |
标准差 | 19.59 | 3.25 | 4.42 | 4.28 | 4.91 | 2.64 | 8.35 | 10.66 | 10.29 | 12.67 | 5.34 |
表1 研究变量和控制变量的描述统计及控制性别后的Spearman偏相关系数(n = 251)
变量 | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | 10 | 11 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. 音乐素养 | — | ||||||||||
2. 观点采择 | 0.30*** | — | |||||||||
3. 想象 | 0.39*** | 0.39*** | — | ||||||||
4. 同情关心 | 0.18*** | 0.30*** | 0.58*** | — | |||||||
5. 个人痛苦 | −0.22** | −0.14* | −0.15* | −0.10 | — | ||||||
6. 开放性 | 0.45*** | 0.34*** | 0.36*** | 0.19** | −0.22*** | — | |||||
7. 主观社会经济地位 | 0.56*** | 0.27*** | 0.23*** | 0.16* | −0.17** | 0.39*** | — | ||||
8. 状态焦虑 | −0.29*** | −0.37*** | −0.32*** | −0.24*** | 0.43*** | −0.35*** | −0.29*** | — | |||
9. 特质焦虑 | −0.32*** | −0.36*** | −0.37*** | −0.26*** | 0.56*** | −0.38*** | −0.33*** | 0.85*** | — | ||
10. 述情障碍 | −0.25*** | −0.37*** | −0.39*** | −0.33*** | 0.63*** | −0.30*** | −0.20** | 0.47*** | 0.57*** | — | |
11. 抑郁 | −0.22*** | −0.24*** | −0.21** | −0.21*** | 0.51*** | −0.31*** | −0.29*** | 0.55*** | 0.66*** | 0.54*** | — |
平均值 | 77.08 | 14.75 | 16.73 | 16.34 | 9.37 | 9.92 | 44.76 | 34.67 | 37.68 | 48.38 | 4.09 |
标准差 | 19.59 | 3.25 | 4.42 | 4.28 | 4.91 | 2.64 | 8.35 | 10.66 | 10.29 | 12.67 | 5.34 |
变量 | β | SE | 95% CI of β | t | df | p |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
音乐训练(音乐家−非音乐家) | 0.16 | 0.07 | [0.01, 0.30] | 2.08 | 113 | 0.040* |
音乐素养(控制后) | 0.11 | 0.07 | [−0.04, 0.25] | 1.39 | 113 | 0.167 |
刺激类型(针扎_无表情−针扎_有表情) | −0.30 | 0.05 | [−0.40, −0.20] | −6.01 | 117 | <0.001*** |
音乐训练(音乐家−非音乐家)×刺激类型(针扎_无表情−针扎_有表情) | −0.04 | 0.05 | [−0.14, 0.06] | −0.87 | 117 | 0.384 |
刺激类型(针扎_无表情−针扎_有表情)×音乐素养(控制后) | −0.07 | 0.05 | [−0.17, 0.03] | −1.33 | 117 | 0.185 |
性别(女−男) | 0.07 | 0.07 | [−0.08, 0.22] | 0.94 | 113 | 0.347 |
TAS20 | 0.05 | 0.09 | [−0.13, 0.22] | 0.55 | 113 | 0.581 |
STAI_T | −0.11 | 0.11 | [−0.32, 0.11] | −0.98 | 113 | 0.328 |
BDI | 0.04 | 0.10 | [−0.15, 0.23] | 0.43 | 113 | 0.668 |
表2 音乐训练、音乐素养和刺激类型对疼痛的感同身受程度影响的线性混合效应模型固定效应结果
变量 | β | SE | 95% CI of β | t | df | p |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
音乐训练(音乐家−非音乐家) | 0.16 | 0.07 | [0.01, 0.30] | 2.08 | 113 | 0.040* |
音乐素养(控制后) | 0.11 | 0.07 | [−0.04, 0.25] | 1.39 | 113 | 0.167 |
刺激类型(针扎_无表情−针扎_有表情) | −0.30 | 0.05 | [−0.40, −0.20] | −6.01 | 117 | <0.001*** |
音乐训练(音乐家−非音乐家)×刺激类型(针扎_无表情−针扎_有表情) | −0.04 | 0.05 | [−0.14, 0.06] | −0.87 | 117 | 0.384 |
刺激类型(针扎_无表情−针扎_有表情)×音乐素养(控制后) | −0.07 | 0.05 | [−0.17, 0.03] | −1.33 | 117 | 0.185 |
性别(女−男) | 0.07 | 0.07 | [−0.08, 0.22] | 0.94 | 113 | 0.347 |
TAS20 | 0.05 | 0.09 | [−0.13, 0.22] | 0.55 | 113 | 0.581 |
STAI_T | −0.11 | 0.11 | [−0.32, 0.11] | −0.98 | 113 | 0.328 |
BDI | 0.04 | 0.10 | [−0.15, 0.23] | 0.43 | 113 | 0.668 |
图4 音乐训练与刺激类型对疼痛评分及疼痛感同身受程度的影响。A.音乐家与非音乐家在不同评价视角下的疼痛评分均值。B.不同刺激条件和不同评价视角下的疼痛评分均值。C.音乐家与非音乐家的感同身受程度。D.不同刺激条件下的感同身受程度。 注:*p < 0.05; ***p < 0.001。误差棒为M ± 1 SE。
[1] |
Bagby, R. M., Parker, J. D., & Taylor, G. J. (1994). The twenty-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale—I. Item selection and cross-validation of the factor structure. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 38(1), 23-32.
doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(94)90005-1 pmid: 8126686 |
[2] | Bailey, J., & Penhune, V. (2013). The relationship between the age of onset of musical training and rhythm synchronization performance: Validation of sensitive period effects. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 7, Article 227. |
[3] | Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 1-48. |
[4] |
Batson, C. D., Eklund, J. H., Chermok, V. L., Hoyt, J. L., & Ortiz, B. G. (2007). An additional antecedent of empathic concern: Valuing the welfare of the person in need. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93(1), 65-74.
doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.93.1.65 pmid: 17605589 |
[5] | Burton, L., & Nkwo, C. (2022). Gender differences in the relationship between state and trait anxiety and empathy. Current Psychology, 41(12), 8368-8373. |
[6] |
Chen. W. Y., Lu. J. M., Liu. L. Q., & Lin. W. Y. (2014). Gender differences of empathy. Advances in Psychological Science, 22(9), 1423-1434.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2014.01423 |
[陈武英, 卢家楣, 刘连启, 林文毅. (2014). 共情的性别差异. 心理科学进展, 22(9), 1423-1434.]
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2014.01423 |
|
[7] | Cheng. G., Chen. Y. H., Guan. Y. S., & Zhang. D. J. (2015). On composition of college students’ subjective social status indexes and their characteristics. Journal of Southwest University (Natural Science Edition), 37(6), 156-162. |
[程刚, 陈艳红, 关雨生, 张大均. (2015). 大学生主观社会地位的指标构成及特点. 西南大学学报(自然科学版), 37(6), 156-162.] | |
[8] | Collet, L., & Cottraux, J. (1986). The shortened Beck Depression Inventory (13 items). Study of the concurrent validity with the Hamilton scale and Widlöcher’s retardation scale. L’Encéphale, 12(2), 77-79. |
[9] | Cross, I., Laurence, F., & Rabinowitch, T.-C. (2012). Empathy and creativity in group musical practices:Towards a concept of empathic creativity. In G. E. McPherson & G. F. Welch (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of music education (Vol. 2, pp. 337-353). Oxford University Press. |
[10] | Cuff, B. M. P., Brown, S. J., Taylor, L., & Howat, D. J. (2016). Empathy: A review of the concept. Emotion Review, 8(2), 144-153. |
[11] | Davis, M. H. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a multidimensional approach. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 44(1), 113-126. |
[12] |
Dziobek, I., Rogers, K., Fleck, S., Bahnemann, M., Heekeren, H. R., Wolf, O. T., & Convit, A. (2008). Dissociation of cognitive and emotional empathy in adults with Asperger syndrome using the Multifaceted Empathy Test (MET). Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(3), 464-473.
doi: 10.1007/s10803-007-0486-x pmid: 17990089 |
[13] | Eerola, T., Vuoskoski, J. K., & Kautiainen, H. (2016). Being moved by unfamiliar sad music is associated with high empathy. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, Article 1176. |
[14] |
Eisenberg, N., & Miller, P. A. (1987). The relation of empathy to prosocial and related behaviors. Psychological Bulletin, 101(1), 91-119.
pmid: 3562705 |
[15] | Epperson, G. (1967). The musical symbol: A study of the philosophic theory of music. Wiley-Blackwell. |
[16] | Fan, Y., Duncan, N. W., de Greck, M., & Northoff, G. (2011). Is there a core neural network in empathy? An fMRI based quantitative meta-analysis. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(3), 903-911. |
[17] |
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A.-G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods, 41, 1149-1160.
doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149 pmid: 19897823 |
[18] | Gosling, S. D., Rentfrow, P. J., & Swann, W. B. (2003). A very brief measure of the Big-Five personality domains. Journal of Research in Personality, 37(6), 504-528. |
[19] |
Green, A. D., Tripp, D. A., Sullivan, M. J. L., & Davidson, M. (2009). The relationship between empathy and estimates of observed pain. Pain Medicine, 10, 381-392.
doi: 10.1111/j.1526-4637.2009.00563.x pmid: 19254339 |
[20] | Greenberg, D. M., Müllensiefen, D., Lamb, M. E., & Rentfrow, P. J. (2015). Personality predicts musical sophistication. Journal of Research in Personality, 58, 154-158. |
[21] | Greenberg, D. M., Rentfrow, P. J., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2015). Can music increase empathy? Interpreting musical experience through the Empathizing-Systemizing (E-S) Theory: Implications for Autism. Empirical Musicology Review, 10(1-2), 80-95. |
[22] | Han, S., Fan, Y., Xu, X., Qin, J., Wu, B., Wang, X.,... Mao, L. (2009). Empathic neural responses to others’ pain are modulated by emotional contexts. Human Brain Mapping, 30(10), 3227-3237. |
[23] | Hekmat, H., Khajavi, F., & Mehryar, A. H. (1975). Some personality correlates of empathy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 43(1), 89. |
[24] | Huang, H. Q., & Su, Y. J. (2010). Interaction between the cognitive and emotional components of empathy. Journal of Southwest University (Social Sciences Edition), 36(6), 13-19. |
[黄翯青, 苏彦捷. (2010). 共情中的认知调节和情绪分享过程及其关系. 西南大学学报(社会科学版), 36(6), 13-19.] | |
[25] |
Jackendoff, R., & Lerdahl, F. (2006). The capacity for music: What is it, and what’s special about it? Cognition, 100(1), 33-72.
pmid: 16384553 |
[26] | Kalliopuska, M., & Ruókonen, I. (1986). Effects of music education on development of holistic empathy. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 62(1), 187-191. |
[27] |
Kalliopuska, M., & Ruókonen, I. (1993). A study with a follow-up of the effects of music education on holistic development of empathy. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 76(1), 131-137.
pmid: 8451117 |
[28] | Kalliopuska, M., & Tiitinen, U. (1991). Influence of two developmental programmes on the empathy and prosociability of preschool children. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 72(1), 323-328. |
[29] | Kawakami, A., & Katahira, K. (2015). Influence of trait empathy on the emotion evoked by sad music and on the preference for it. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, Article 1541. |
[30] | Klarlund, M., Brattico, E., Pearce, M., Wu, Y., Vuust, P., Overgaard, M., & Du, Y. (2023). Worlds apart? Testing the cultural distance hypothesis in music perception of Chinese and Western listeners. Cognition, 235, Article 105405. |
[31] | Koivisto, M., Virkkala, M., Puustinen, M., & Aarnio, J. (2023). Open and empathic personalities see two things at the same time: The relationship of big-five personality traits and cognitive empathy with mixed percepts during binocular rivalry. Current Psychology, 42(11), 9552-9562. |
[32] | Kraus, N., & Chandrasekaran, B. (2010). Music training for the development of auditory skills. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11(8), Article 8. |
[33] | Li. J., D. (2013). Psychometric properties of Ten-Item Personality Inventory in China. China Journal of Health Psychology, 21(11), 1688-1692. |
[李金德. (2013). 中国版10项目大五人格量表(TIPI-C)的信效度检验. 中国健康心理学杂志, 21(11), 1688-1692.] | |
[34] | Lin, H. R., Kopiez, R., Müllensiefen, D., & Wolf, A. (2021). The Chinese version of the Gold-MSI: Adaptation and validation of an inventory for the measurement of musical sophistication in a Taiwanese sample. Musicae Scientiae, 25(2), 226-251. |
[35] | Meng. J., Chen. Y. G., & Huang. X. T. (2010). Influencing factors and the mechanism of empathy for pain. Advances in Psychological Science, 18(3), 432-440. |
[孟景, 陈有国, 黄希庭. (2010). 疼痛共情的影响因素及其认知机制. 心理科学进展, 18(3), 432-440.] | |
[36] | Müllensiefen, D., Gingras, B., Musil, J., & Stewart, L. (2014). The musicality of non-musicians: An index for assessing musical sophistication in the general population. PloS One, 9(2), Article e89642. |
[37] | Olszewska, A. M., Gaca, M., Herman, A. M., Jednoróg, K., & Marchewka, A. (2021). How musical training shapes the adult brain: Predispositions and neuroplasticity. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 15, Article 630829. |
[38] | Parsons, C. E., Young, K. S., Jegindø, E.-M. E., Vuust, P., Stein, A., & Kringelbach, M. L. (2014). Music training and empathy positively impact adults’ sensitivity to infant distress. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, Article 1440. |
[39] | Perlovsky, L. (2012). Cognitive function, origin, and evolution of musical emotions. Musicae Scientiae, 16(2), 185-199. |
[40] | Picerni, E., Laricchiuta, D., Piras, F., Vecchio, D., Petrosini, L., Cutuli, D., & Spalletta, G. (2021). Macro- and micro- structural cerebellar and cortical characteristics of cognitive empathy towards fictional characters in healthy individuals. Scientific Reports, 11(1), Article 8804. |
[41] |
Preston, S. D., & Waal, F. B. M. de. (2002). Empathy: Its ultimate and proximate bases. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25(1), 1-20.
doi: 10.1017/s0140525x02000018 pmid: 12625087 |
[42] | Rabinowitch, T.-C., Cross, I., & Burnard, P. (2013). Long-term musical group interaction has a positive influence on empathy in children. Psychology of Music, 41(4), 484-498. |
[43] | Rabinowitch, T.-C., & Knafo-Noam, A. (2015). Synchronous rhythmic interaction enhances children’s perceived similarity and closeness towards each other. PloS One, 10(4), Article e0120878. |
[44] | Rabinowitch, T.-C., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2017). Joint rhythmic movement increases 4-year-old children’s prosocial sharing and fairness toward peers. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, Article 1050. |
[45] | Ren, Q. Y., Sun, Y. M., Lu, X. J., Huang, C., & Hu, L.. (2019). Empathy: Methodologies and characteristics from a psychophysiological perspective. Chinese Science Bulletin, 64(22), 2292-2304. |
[任巧悦, 孙元淼, 吕雪靖, 黄超, 胡理. (2019). 基于心理生理学视角的共情研究:方法与特点. 科学通报, 64(22), 2292-2304.] | |
[46] | Roberts, W., & Strayer, J. (1996). Empathy, emotional expressiveness, and prosocial behavior. Child Development, 67(2), 449-470. |
[47] | Rosseel, Y. (2012). lavaan: An R package for structural equation modeling. Journal of Statistical Software, 48(2), 1-36. |
[48] | Savage, P. E. (2019). Cultural evolution of music. Palgrave Communications, 5(1), Article 16. |
[49] | Savage, P. E., Loui, P., Tarr, B., Schachner, A., Glowacki, L., Mithen, S., & Fitch, W. T. (2021). Music as a coevolved system for social bonding. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 44, Article e59. |
[50] | Schellenberg, E. G., Corrigall, K. A., Dys, S. P., & Malti, T. (2015). Group music training and children’s prosocial skills. Plos One, 10(10), Article e0141449. |
[51] | Schellenberg, E. G., & Lima, C. F. (2024). Music training and nonmusical abilities. Annual Review of Psychology, 75(1), 87-128. |
[52] | Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Aharon-Peretz, J., & Perry, D. (2009). Two systems for empathy: A double dissociation between emotional and cognitive empathy in inferior frontal gyrus versus ventromedial prefrontal lesions. Brain, 132(3), 617-627. |
[53] | Spielberger, C., Gorsuch, R., Lushene, R., Vagg, P., & Jacobs, G. (1983). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Consulting Psychologists Press. |
[54] | Steele, J. R. (2019). The relationship between music aptitude, empathy, and sensitivity to emotional prosody: An ERP investigation [Unpublished master’s thesis]. Middle Tennessee State University. |
[55] | Stotland, E. (1978). Empathy, fantasy, and helping. Sage Publications. |
[56] |
Stupacher, J., Mikkelsen, J., & Vuust, P. (2022). Higher empathy is associated with stronger social bonding when moving together with music. Psychology of Music, 50(5), 1511-1526.
doi: 10.1177/03057356211050681 pmid: 36097608 |
[57] | The jamovi project. (2023). jamovi. (Version 2.4) [Computer Software]. Retrieved from https://www.jamovi.org. |
[58] | Wallentin, M., Højlund, A., Friis-Olivarius, M., Vuust, C., & Vuust, P. (2010). The Musical Ear Test, a new reliable test for measuring musical competence. Learning and Individual Differences, 20(3), 188-196. |
[59] | Walter, H. (2012). Social cognitive neuroscience of empathy: Concepts, circuits, and genes. Emotion Review, 4(1), 9-17. |
[60] | Wan, Y., & Zhu, L. (2021). Effects of rhythmic turn-taking coordination on five-year-old children’s prosocial behaviors. Developmental Psychology, 57(11), 1787-1795. |
[61] | Wang, X. D., Wang, X. L., & Ma, H. (1999). Rating scales for mental health. Chinese Mental Health Journal Publisher. |
[汪向东, 王希林, 马弘. (1999). 心理卫生评定量表手册(增订版). 北京: 中国心理卫生杂志社.] | |
[62] |
Watanabe, D., Savion-Lemieux, T., & Penhune, V. B. (2007). The effect of early musical training on adult motor performance: Evidence for a sensitive period in motor learning. Experimental Brain Research, 176(2), 332-340.
doi: 10.1007/s00221-006-0619-z pmid: 16896980 |
[63] | Wei, B., Zhang, X., Cui, D., & Li, Y. (2023). Linking objective and subjective social status to altruistic sharing in China: The role of empathy. Current Psychology, 42, 27401-27414. |
[64] | Wöllner, C. (2012). Is empathy related to the perception of emotional expression in music? A multimodal time-series analysis. Psychology of Aesthetics Creativity and the Arts, 6(3), 214-223. |
[65] | Wu, X., & Lu, X. (2021). Musical training in the development of empathy and prosocial behaviors. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, Article 661769. |
[66] | Yan, Z., Zeng, X., Su, J., & Zhang, X. (2021). The dark side of empathy: Meta-analysis evidence of the relationship between empathy and depression. PsyCh Journal, 10(5), 794-804. |
[67] | Yu, C.-L., & Chou, T.-L. (2018). A dual route model of empathy: A neurobiological prospective. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, Article 2212. |
[68] | Zhang, F. F., Dong, Y., Wang, K., Zhan, Z. Y., & Xie, L. F. (2010). Reliability and validity of the Chinese version of the Interpersonal Reactivity Index-C. Chinese Journal of Clinical Psychology, 18(2), 155-157. |
[张凤凤, 董毅, 汪凯, 詹志禹, 谢伦芳. (2010). 中文版人际反应指针量表(IRI-C)的信度及效度研究. 中国临床心理学杂志, 18(2), 155-157.] | |
[69] |
Zhang, W. X., Li, X., Chen, G. H., & Cao, Y. M. (2021). The relationship between positive parenting and adolescent prosocial behaviour: The mediating role of empathy and the moderating role of the oxytocin receptor gene. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 53(9), 976-991.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00976 |
[张文新, 李曦, 陈光辉, 曹衍淼. (2021). 母亲积极教养与青少年亲社会行为:共情的中介作用与OXTR基因的调节作用. 心理学报, 53(9), 976-991.]
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00976 |
|
[70] | Zhong, Y. P., Yang, Z. L., & Fan, W. (2015). The effects of self-other overlap on helping behavior: Moderating of perspective taking. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 47(8), 1050-1057. |
[钟毅平, 杨子鹿, 范伟. (2015). 自我-他人重叠对助人行为的影响:观点采择的调节作用. 心理学报, 47(8), 1050-1057.] | |
[71] | Zhou, H., & Long, L. R. (2004). Statistical remedies for common method biases. Advances in Psychological Science, 12(6), 942-950. |
[周浩, 龙立荣. (2004). 共同方法偏差的统计检验与控制方法. 心理科学进展, 12(6), 942-950.] |
[1] | 张文芸, 卓诗维, 郑倩倩, 关颖琳, 彭微微. 自闭特质对疼痛共情的影响:疼痛负性情绪和认知的中介作用[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(9): 1501-1517. |
[2] | 陈杰, 伍可, 史宇鹏, 艾小青. 特质性自我构念与内外群体疼痛共情的关系: 来自事件相关电位的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(6): 629-638. |
[3] | 张政华, 韩梅, 张放, 李卫君. 音乐训练促进诗句韵律整合加工的神经过程[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(7): 847-860. |
[4] | 姚尧, 陈晓湘. 音乐训练对4~5岁幼儿普通话声调范畴感知能力的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(4): 456-468. |
[5] | 李雄, 李祚山, 向滨洋, 孟景. 注意线索对自闭特质个体疼痛共情的影响:来自事件相关电位的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(3): 294-306. |
[6] | 陈洁佳, 周翊, 陈杰. 音乐训练与抑制控制的关系:来自ERPs的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(12): 1365-1376. |
[7] | 程家萍;罗跃嘉;崔芳. 认知负荷对疼痛共情的影响:来自ERP研究的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(5): 622-630. |
[8] | 高雪梅;翁蕾;周群;赵偲;李芳. 暴力犯的疼痛共情更低:来自ERP的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2015, 47(4): 478-487. |
[9] | 孟景;沈林;吕振勇;杨周;陈红;Todd Jackson. 疼痛表征在自我和他人间的一致性效应[J]. 心理学报, 2012, 44(11): 1515-1522. |
[10] | 梅磊磊,李燕芳,龙柚杉,陈传升,董奇. 材料呈现方式对不同音乐训练经验的汉语儿童英语言语记忆的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2008, 40(08): 883-889. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||