[1] |
Abrahamse, E., van Dijck, J. P., & Fias, W. (2016). How does working memory enable number-induced spatial biases? Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 977. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00977.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00977
pmid: 27445937
|
[2] |
Abrahamse, E., van Dijck, J. P., Majerus, S., & Fias, W. (2014). Finding the answer in space: the mental whiteboard hypothesis on serial order in working memory. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 932. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00932.
doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00932
pmid: 25505394
|
[3] |
Bulf, H., Capparini, C., Nava, E., de Hevia, M. D., & Cassia, V. M. (2022). Space modulates cross-domain transfer of abstract rules in infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 213, 105270. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2021.105270.
doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2021.105270
URL
|
[4] |
Bulf, H., de Hevia, M. D., & Cassia, M. V. (2016). Small on the left, large on the right: numbers orient visual attention onto space in preverbal infants. Developmental Science, 19(3), 394-401.
doi: 10.1111/desc.12315
URL
|
[5] |
Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122(3), 371-396.
doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.122.3.371
URL
|
[6] |
Dehaene, S., Dupoux, E., & Mehler, J. (1990). Is numerical comparison digital? analogical and symbolic effects in two-digit number comparison. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16(3), 626-641.
doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.16.3.626
URL
|
[7] |
Fumarola, A., Prpic, V., Fornasier, D., Sartoretto, F., Agostini, T., & Umilta, C. (2016). The spatial representation of angles. Perception, 45(11), 1320-1330.
doi: 10.1177/0301006616661915
pmid: 27495184
|
[8] |
Fumarola, A., Prpic, V., Pos, O. D., Murgia, M., Umiltà, C., & Agostini, T. (2014). Automatic spatial association for luminance. Attention Perception & Psychophysics, 76(3), 759-765.
doi: 10.3758/s13414-013-0614-y
URL
|
[9] |
Gevers, W., Reynvoet, B., & Fias, W. (2003). The mental representation of ordinal sequences is spatially organized. Cognition, 87(3), B87-B95.
|
[10] |
Guida, A., & Campitelli, G. (2019). Explaining the SPoARC and SNARC effects with knowledge structures: An expertise account. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 26(2), 434-451.
doi: 10.3758/s13423-019-01582-0
URL
|
[11] |
Guida, A., & Lavielle-Guida, M. (2014). 2011 space odyssey: Spatialization as a mechanism to code order allows a close encounter between memory expertise and classic immediate memory studies. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 573. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00573.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00573
|
[12] |
Guida, A., & Maherault, M. (2021). Bootstrapping the visuospatial bootstrapping effect and testing its spatialisation. Memory, 29(4), 456-470.
doi: 10.1080/09658211.2021.1903039
URL
|
[13] |
Guida, A., Megreya, A. M., Lavielle-Guida, M., Noël, Y., Mathy, F., van Dijck, J. P., & Abrahamse, E. (2018). Spatialization in working memory is related to literacy and reading direction: culture “literarily” directs our thoughts. Cognition, 175, 96-100.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.02.013
URL
|
[14] |
Kopiske, K. K., Löwenkamp, C., Eloka, O., Schiller, F., Kao, C. S., Wu, C., Gao, X., & Franz, V. H. (2016). The SNARC effect in chinese numerals: do visual properties of characters and hand signs influence number processing? Plos One, 11(9), e0163897. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone. 0163897.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone. 0163897
URL
|
[15] |
Nuerk, H. C., Wood, G., & Willmes, K. (2005). The universal SNARC effect: The association between number magnitude and space is amodal. Experimental Psychology, 52(3), 187-194.
doi: 10.1027/1618-3169.52.3.187
URL
|
[16] |
Prete, G. (2020). Spatializing emotions besides magnitudes: is there a left-to-right valence or intensity mapping? Symmetry, 12(5), 775. doi: 10.3390/sym12050775.
doi: 10.3390/sym12050775
URL
|
[17] |
Previtali, P., de Hevia, M. D., & Girelli, L. (2010). Placing order in space: The SNARC effect in serial learning. Experimental Brain Research, 201(3), 599-605.
doi: 10.1007/s00221-009-2063-3
pmid: 19888566
|
[18] |
Shaki, S., Fischer, M. H., & Petrusic, W. M. (2009). Reading habits for both words and numbers contribute to the SNARC effect. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16(2), 328-331.
doi: 10.3758/PBR.16.2.328
URL
|
[19] |
Shi, W., Wang, Q., Deng, M., & Xu, D. (2020). The influence of the location of ordered symbols on the ordinal position effect: The involvement of the task performed. Acta Psychologica, 202, 102978. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2019. 102978.
doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2019. 102978
URL
|
[20] |
van Dijck, J. P., & Fias, W. (2011). A working memory account for spatial-numerical associations. Cognition, 119(1), 114-119.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2010.12.013
URL
|
[21] |
Wang, Q. Q. (2015). The research on the cause of SNARC effect—Induced by size or order information?(Unpublished master’s thesis). Northeast Normal University, Changchun, China.
|
|
[王强强. (2015). SNARC效应的起因探究——大小还是顺序信息引起?(硕士学位论文). 东北师范大学, 长春.]
|
[22] |
Wang, Q. Q., An, B. X., Yue, H. L., Tao, W. D., & Shi, W. D. (2021). Interaction mechanism between location and sequence in letter cognition. Acta Psychologica, 217, 103329. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2021.103329.
doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2021.103329
URL
|
[23] |
Wang, Q. Q., Nie, T. T., Zhang, W. X., & Shi, W. D. (2019). The mechanism of the ordinal position effect: Stability across sense modalities and the hands crossed context. I-perception, 10(2). doi: 10.1177/2041669519841071.
doi: 10.1177/2041669519841071
|
[24] |
Wang, Q. Q., Shi, W. D., & Ye, J. (2018). The impact effect of the task switch on the SNARC effect. Chinese Journal of Applied Psychology, 24(3), 271-279.
|
|
[王强强, 石文典, 叶晶. (2018). 任务转换对SNARC效应的抑制. 应用心理学, 24(3), 271-279.]
|
[25] |
Wang, Z., Zhu, X., & Jiang, Y. (2020). Influence of number location on the SNARC effect: evidence from the processing of rotated traditional chinese numerical words. I-perception, 11(2). doi: 10.1177/2041669520917169.
doi: 10.1177/2041669520917169
|
[26] |
Yan, L. Z., Chen, Y. X., Liu, X., Fu, S. M., & Nan, W. Z. (2022). The flexibility of spatial-numerical associations and its internal mechanism. Advances in Psychological Science, 30(1), 51-64.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2022.00051
URL
|
|
[颜丽珠, 陈妍秀, 刘勋, 傅世敏, 南威治. (2022). 数字空间联结的灵活性及其内在机制. 心理科学进展, 30(1), 51-64.]
|
[27] |
Zhang, M., Gao, X., Li, B., Yu, S., Gong, T., Jiang, T., Hu, Q., & Chen, Y. (2016). Spatial representation of ordinal information. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 505. doi: 10.3389/ fpsyg.2016.00505.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00505
pmid: 27092100
|