心理科学进展 ›› 2022, Vol. 30 ›› Issue (11): 2497-2506.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2022.02497
收稿日期:
2021-09-08
出版日期:
2022-11-15
发布日期:
2022-11-09
通讯作者:
宋耀武
E-mail:syw@hbu.edu.cn
基金资助:
JIN Yuwei, SUN Xiao, SONG Yaowu()
Received:
2021-09-08
Online:
2022-11-15
Published:
2022-11-09
Contact:
SONG Yaowu
E-mail:syw@hbu.edu.cn
摘要:
具身记忆研究主要探讨身体及其感觉运动过程在记忆中的作用。操纵或限制身体的物理状态、具身特征或可用性, 可以影响记忆的效率、效价和内容。其内在机制可以通过编码特异性原理、启动效应、隐喻表征理论和再激活假说进行解释, 但尚未形成统一的模型。基于现有研究与理论的共性, 提出具身记忆的感觉运动模拟模型, 强调重新激活在具身记忆中的作用, 以期在同一框架下说明身体作用于记忆的过程, 并进一步指出重新激活的条件。未来亟需在具身记忆的稳定性以及深层机制等方面进行多维度的基础和应用研究。
中图分类号:
金雨薇, 孙潇, 宋耀武. (2022). 具身记忆及其内在机制. 心理科学进展 , 30(11), 2497-2506.
JIN Yuwei, SUN Xiao, SONG Yaowu. (2022). Embodied memory and its intrinsic mechanism. Advances in Psychological Science, 30(11), 2497-2506.
[1] | 李荆广, 郭秀艳. (2009). 记忆研究的功能取向. 心理科学进展, 17(5), 923-930. |
[2] | 张博, 葛鲁嘉. (2017). 温和的具身认知: 认知科学研究新进路. 华侨大学学报(哲学社会科学版), (1), 19-28. |
[3] | Atkinson, R. C., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1968). Human memory:A proposed system and its control processes. In K. W Spence & J. T. Spence (Eds.),Psychology of Learning and Motivation (Vol. 2, pp. 89-195). New York: Academic Press. |
[4] |
Berg, J. (2019). Replication challenges. Science, 365(6457), 957-957.
doi: 10.1126/science.aaz2701 pmid: 31488663 |
[5] |
Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36(2), 129-148.
pmid: 7224324 |
[6] |
Caravà, M. (2021). An exploration into enactive forms of forgetting. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 20(4), 703-722.
doi: 10.1007/s11097-020-09670-6 URL |
[7] |
Casasanto, D. (2009). Embodiment of abstract concepts: Good and bad in right- and left-handers. Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 138(3), 351-367.
doi: 10.1037/a0015854 URL |
[8] |
Casasanto, D., & de Bruin, A. (2019). Metaphors we learn by: Directed motor action improves word learning. Cognition, 182, 177-183.
doi: S0010-0277(18)30251-8 pmid: 30296659 |
[9] |
Casasanto, D., & Dijkstra, K. (2010). Motor action and emotional memory. Cognition, 115(1), 179-185.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.11.002 pmid: 20096831 |
[10] |
Chen, Y., Yu, Y., Niu, R., & Liu, Y. (2018). Selective effects of postural control on spatial vs. nonspatial working memory: A functional near-infrared spectral imaging study. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 12, 243.
doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00243 pmid: 29950981 |
[11] |
Clark, M. S., Milberg, S., & Ross, J. (1983). Arousal cues arousal-related material in memory: Implications for understanding effects of mood on memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 22(6), 633-649.
doi: 10.1016/S0022-5371(83)90375-4 URL |
[12] |
Collins, A. M., & Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing. Psychological Review, 82(6), 407-428.
doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.82.6.407 URL |
[13] |
Cook, S. W., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2006). The role of gesture in learning: Do children use their hands to change their minds?. Journal of Cognition and Development, 7(2), 211-232.
doi: 10.1207/s15327647jcd0702_4 URL |
[14] |
Damasio, A. R. (1989). Time-locked multiregional retroactivation: A systems-level proposal for the neural substrates of recall and recognition. Cognition, 33(1-2), 25-62.
pmid: 2691184 |
[15] |
Davis, C. P., Joergensen, G. H., Boddy, P., Dowling, C., & Yee, E. (2020). Making it harder to “see” meaning: The more you see something, the more its conceptual representation is susceptible to visual interference. Psychological Science, 31(5), 505-517.
doi: 10.1177/0956797620910748 URL |
[16] | de Vega, M. (2012). Language and action:An approach to embodied cognition. In V. Gyselinck & F. Pazzaglia (Eds.),From mental imagery to spatial cognition and language: essays in honour of Michel Denis (pp. 177-199). London: Psychology Press. |
[17] | Díez-Álamo, A. M., Díez, E., Alonso, M. A., & Fernandez, A. (2019). Absence of posture-dependent and posture- congruent memory effects on the recall of action sentences. PLoS ONE, 14(12), e0226297. |
[18] |
Dijkstra, K., Kaschak, M. P., & Zwaan, R. A. (2007). Body posture facilitates retrieval of autobiographical memories. Cognition, 102(1), 139-149.
pmid: 16472550 |
[19] |
Dijkstra, K., & Post, L. (2015). Mechanisms of embodiment. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1525.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01525 pmid: 26528203 |
[20] | Dutriaux, L., & Gyselinck, V. (2016). Learning is better with the hands free: The role of posture in the memory of manipulable objects. PLoS ONE, 11(7), e0159108. |
[21] |
Dutriaux, L., Dahiez, X., & Gyselinck, V. (2019). How to change your memory of an object with a posture and a verb. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 72(5), 1112-1118.
doi: 10.1177/1747021818785096 URL |
[22] | Dutriaux, L., Nicolas, S., & Gyselinck, V. (2021). Aging and posture in the memory of manipulable objects. Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition. 28( 1), 26-36. |
[23] |
Glenberg, A. M. (1997). What memory is for. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 20(1), 1-55.
pmid: 10096994 |
[24] | Halvorson, K. M., Bushinski, A., & Hilverman, C. (2019). The role of motor context in the beneficial effects of hand gesture on memory. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 81( 7), 2354-2364. |
[25] |
Hammond, A. G., Murphy, E. M., Silverman, B. M., Bernas, R. S., & Nardi, D. (2019). No environmental context- dependent effect, but interference, of physical activity on object location memory. Cognitive Processing, 20(1), 31-43.
doi: 10.1007/s10339-018-0875-4 URL |
[26] |
Harmon-Jones, E., Price, T. F., & Harmon-Jones, C. (2015). Supine body posture decreases rationalizations: Testing the action-based model of dissonance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 56, 228-234.
doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2014.10.007 URL |
[27] |
Harnad, S. (1990). The symbol grounding problem. Physica D: Nonlinear Phenomena, 42(1-3), 335-346.
doi: 10.1016/0167-2789(90)90087-6 URL |
[28] |
Huff, M., Maurer, A. E., & Merkt, M. (2018). Producing gestures establishes a motor context for procedural learning tasks. Learning and Instruction, 58, 245-254.
doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2018.07.008 URL |
[29] | Hutto, D. D., & Myin, E. (2017). Evolving enactivism: Basic minds meet content. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. |
[30] | Hutto, D. D., & Peeters, A. (2018). The roots of remembering:Radically enactive recollecting. In K. Michaelian, D. Debus, & D. Perrin (Eds.),New directions in the philosophy of memory (pp. 97-118). New York: Routledge. |
[31] |
Ianì, F. (2019). Embodied memories: Reviewing the role of the body in memory processes. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 26(6), 1747-1766.
doi: 10.3758/s13423-019-01674-x URL |
[32] |
Ianì, F., & Bucciarelli, M. (2017). Mechanisms underlying the beneficial effect of a speaker’s gestures on the listener. Journal of Memory and Language, 96, 110-121.
doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2017.05.004 URL |
[33] |
Ianì, F., & Bucciarelli, M. (2018). Relevance of the listener’s motor system in recalling phrases enacted by the speaker. Memory, 26(8), 1084-1092.
doi: 10.1080/09658211.2018.1433214 URL |
[34] |
Ianì, F., Burin, D., Salatino, A., Pia, L., Ricci, R., & Bucciarelli, M. (2018). The beneficial effect of a speaker’s gestures on the listener’s memory for action phrases: The pivotal role of the listener’s premotor cortex. Brain and Language, 180-182, 8-13.
doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2018.03.001 URL |
[35] |
Isen, A. M., Shalker, T. E., Clark, M., & Karp, L. (1978). Affect, accessibility of material in memory, and behavior: A cognitive loop?. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36(1), 1-12.
pmid: 621625 |
[36] |
Kent, C., & Lamberts, K. (2008). The encoding-retrieval relationship: Retrieval as mental simulation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(3), 92-98.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.12.004 pmid: 18262827 |
[37] |
Körner, A., Topolinski, S., & Strack, F. (2015). Routes to embodiment. Frontiers in psychology, 6, 940.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00940 pmid: 26191033 |
[38] |
Kuhnke, P., Kiefer, M., & Hartwigsen, G. (2020). Task- dependent recruitment of modality-specific and multimodal regions during conceptual processing. Cerebral Cortex, 30(7), 3938-3959.
doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhaa010 URL |
[39] | Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. |
[40] | Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1999). Philosophy in the flesh: The embodied mind and its challenge to Western thought. New York: Basic Books. |
[41] | Lautenbach, F., Jeraj, D., Loeffler, J., & Musculus, L. (2019). Give me five? Examining the psychophysiological effects of high-fives in athletes. Applied Psychophysiology & Biofeedback, 44(3), 211-219. |
[42] |
Lomoriello, A. S., Maffei, A., Brigadoi, S., & Sessa, P. (2021). Altering sensorimotor simulation impacts early stages of facial expression processing depending on individual differences in alexithymic traits. Brain and Cognition, 148, 105678.
doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2020.105678 URL |
[43] |
Lorey, B., Bischoff, M., Pilgramm, S., Stark, R., Munzert, J., & Zentgraf, K. (2009). The embodied nature of motor imagery: The influence of posture and perspective. Experimental Brain Research, 194(2), 233-243.
doi: 10.1007/s00221-008-1693-1 pmid: 19139856 |
[44] |
Mahon, B. Z., & Caramazza, A. (2008). A critical look at the embodied cognition hypothesis and a new proposal for grounding conceptual content. Journal of Physiology (Paris), 102(1-3), 59-70.
doi: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2008.03.004 URL |
[45] |
Marre, Q., Huet, N., & Labeye, E. (2021). Embodied mental imagery improves memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 74(8), 1396-1405.
doi: 10.1177/17470218211009227 URL |
[46] |
Martin, A. (2007). The representation of object concepts in the brain. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 25-45.
pmid: 16968210 |
[47] | Matheson, H. E., & Barsalou, L. W. (2018). Embodiment and grounding in cognitive neuroscience. In J. T. Wixted & S. Ghetti (Eds.),Stevens’ Handbook of experimental psychology and cognitive neuroscience (Vol. 3, pp. 1-27). New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc. |
[48] | Mehta, R. K., Shortz, A. E., & Benden, M. E. (2015). Standing up for learning: A pilot investigation on the neurocognitive benefits of stand-biased school desks. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 13(1), 59. |
[49] | Michalak, J., Mischnat, J., & Teismann, T. (2014). Sitting posture makes a difference—Embodiment effects on depressive memory bias. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 21(6), 519-524. |
[50] | Michalak, J., Rohde, K., & Troje, N. F. (2015). How we walk affects what we remember: Gait modifications through biofeedback change negative affective memory bias. Journal of Behavior Therapy & Experimental Psychiatry, 46, 121-125. |
[51] |
Miles, C., & Hardman, E. (1998). State-dependent memory produced by aerobic exercise. Ergonomics, 41(1), 20-28.
pmid: 9468804 |
[52] |
Mille, J., Brambati, S. M., Izaute, M., & Vallet, G. T. (2021). Low-resolution neurocognitive aging and cognition: An embodied perspective. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 15, 687393.
doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2021.687393 URL |
[53] |
Nyberg, L., Petersson, K. M., Nilsson, L. G., Sandblom, J., Åberg, C., & Ingvar, M. (2001). Reactivation of motor brain areas during explicit memory for actions. Neuroimage, 14(2), 521-528.
pmid: 11467924 |
[54] |
Persson, J., & Nyberg, L. (2000). Conjunction analysis of cortical activations common to encoding and retrieval. Microscopy Research and Technique, 51(1), 39-44.
pmid: 11002351 |
[55] |
Plonsky, O., & Erev, I. (2021). To predict human choice, consider the context. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 25(10), 819-820.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2021.07.007 URL |
[56] | Quettier, T., Gambarota, F., Tsuchiya, N., & Sessa, P. (2021). Blocking facial mimicry during binocular rivalry modulates visual awareness of faces with a neutral expression. Scientific Reports, 11(1), 9972. |
[57] | Rand, G., & Wapner, S. (1967). Postural status as a factor in memory. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 6(2), 268-271. |
[58] |
Reed, H. J. (1931). The influence of a change of conditions upon the amount recalled. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 14(6), 632-649.
doi: 10.1037/h0069294 URL |
[59] |
Riskind, J. H. (1983). Nonverbal expressions and the accessibility of life experience memories: A congruence hypothesis. Social Cognition, 2(1), 62-86.
doi: 10.1521/soco.1983.2.1.62 URL |
[60] |
Ross, P., & Atkinson, A. P. (2020). Expanding simulation models of emotional understanding: The case for different modalities, body-state simulation prominence, and developmental trajectories. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 309.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00309 pmid: 32194476 |
[61] | Schramke, C. J., & Bauer, R. M. (1997). State-dependent learning in older and younger adults. Psychology & Aging, 12(2), 255-262. |
[62] |
Seno, T., Kawabe, T., Ito, H., & Sunaga, S. (2013). Vection modulates emotional valence of autobiographical episodic memories. Cognition, 126(1), 115-120.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.08.009 pmid: 23063264 |
[63] |
Sinigaglia, C., & Rizzolatti, G. (2011). Through the looking glass: Self and others. Consciousness and cognition, 20(1), 64-74.
doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2010.11.012 pmid: 21220203 |
[64] |
Sirigu, A., & Duhamel, J. (2001). Motor and visual imagery as two complementary but neurally dissociable mental processes. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13(7), 910-919.
pmid: 11595094 |
[65] |
Slepian, M. L., Weisbuch, M., Rule, N. O., & Ambady, N. (2011). Tough and tender: Embodied categorization of gender. Psychological Science, 22(1), 26-28.
doi: 10.1177/0956797610390388 pmid: 21106884 |
[66] |
Slowikowski, S., & Motion, J. (2021). Unlocking meaning of embodied memories from bushfire survivors. The Oral History Review, 48(1), 83-99.
doi: 10.1080/00940798.2020.1866954 URL |
[67] |
Tucker, M., & Ellis, R. (2004). Action priming by briefly presented objects. Acta Psychologica, 116(2), 185-203.
pmid: 15158182 |
[68] |
Tulving, E., & Thomson, D. M. (1973). Encoding specificity and retrieval processes in episodic memory. Psychological Review, 80(5), 352-373.
doi: 10.1037/h0020071 URL |
[69] |
Väljamäe, A., & Seno, T. (2016). Modulation of recognition memory for emotional images by vertical vection. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 39.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00039 pmid: 26869949 |
[70] |
Vargas, C. D., Olivier, E., Craighero, L., Fadiga, L., Duhamel, J. R., & Sirigu, A. (2004). The influence of hand posture on corticospinal excitability during motor imagery: A transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Cerebral Cortex, 14(11), 1200-1206.
pmid: 15142965 |
[71] | Veenstra, L., Schneider, I. K., & Koole, S. L. (2016). Embodied mood regulation: The impact of body posture on mood recovery, negative thoughts, and mood-congruent recall. Cognition & Emotion, 31(7) 1-16. |
[72] | Villatte, J., Taconnat, L., Bidet-Ildei, C., & Toussaint, L. (2021). Short-term upper limb immobilization and the embodied view of memory: A pilot study. PLoS ONE, 16(3), e0248239. |
[73] |
Wheeler, M. E., Petersen, S. E., & Buckner, R. L. (2000). Memory’s echo: Vivid remembering reactivates sensory- specific cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 97(20), 11125-11129.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.97.20.11125 URL |
[74] |
Wilson, M. (2002). Six views of embodied cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9(4), 625-636.
doi: 10.3758/BF03196322 URL |
[1] | 杨超, 董之婕, 闻浩言, 周一舟, 王艳丽, 袁方舟. 积极身体意象的前因后果及其提升策略[J]. 心理科学进展, 2023, 31(5): 815-826. |
[2] | 张明霞, 李雨欣, 李瑾, 刘勋. 内外动机对青少年记忆的影响及其神经机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2023, 31(1): 1-9. |
[3] | 王松雪, 程思, 蒋挺, 刘勋, 张明霞. 外在奖赏对陈述性记忆的影响[J]. 心理科学进展, 2023, 31(1): 78-86. |
[4] | 赵冰洁, 张琪涵, 陈怡馨, 章鹏, 白学军. 智力运动专家领域内知觉与记忆的加工特点及其机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(9): 1993-2003. |
[5] | 王鑫麟, 邱晓悦, 翁旭初, 杨平. 工作记忆的神经振荡调控:基于神经振荡夹带现象[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(4): 802-816. |
[6] | 李俊娇, 陈伟, 石佩, 董媛媛, 郑希付. 预期错误在恐惧记忆更新中的作用与机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(4): 834-850. |
[7] | 刘俊材, 冉光明, 张琪. 不同情绪载体的神经活动及其异同——脑成像研究的ALE元分析[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(3): 536-555. |
[8] | 陈幸明, 付彤, 刘昌, 张宾, 伏云发, 李恩泽, ZHANG Jian, 陈盛强, 党彩萍. 工作记忆训练诱发的神经可塑性——基于系列fMRI实验的脑区分布递减时空模型[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(2): 255-274. |
[9] | 陈玉田, 陈睿, 李鹏. 工作记忆中“组块”概念的演化及理论模型[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(12): 2708-2717. |
[10] | 杨雪, 朱旭. 情感触摸对心理健康的影响及应用[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(12): 2789-2798. |
[11] | 顾丽佳, 宫文潇, 张静, 陈巍, 郭建友. 身体拥有感错觉对疼痛的影响及其作用机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(11): 2518-2528. |
[12] | 侯文文, 苏怡. 注意和记忆对孤独症儿童词汇发展滞后的影响[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(11): 2558-2569. |
[13] | 王紫乐, 张琪. 视觉搜索中注意模板促进搜索的内在机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(10): 2206-2218. |
[14] | 柳王娟, 定险峰, 程晓荣, 范炤. 序列依赖效应——一种全新的“历史效应”[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(10): 2228-2239. |
[15] | 郑志伟, 肖凤秋, 邵琦, 赵晓凤, 黄妍, 李娟. 情景记忆成功年老化的神经机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(10): 2254-2268. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||