心理科学进展 ›› 2022, Vol. 30 ›› Issue (11): 2507-2517.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2022.02507
收稿日期:
2021-11-15
出版日期:
2022-11-15
发布日期:
2022-11-09
通讯作者:
陈群林,丁珂
E-mail:chenqunlin@swu.edu.cn;dingke2022@gmail.com
基金资助:
Received:
2021-11-15
Online:
2022-11-15
Published:
2022-11-09
Contact:
CHEN Qunlin,DING Ke
E-mail:chenqunlin@swu.edu.cn;dingke2022@gmail.com
摘要:
发散思维的序列位置效应是指随着时间的推移, 单位时间内想法生成的数量逐渐降低, 但想法的独创性逐渐升高的现象。联想理论和执行控制假说分别侧重对序列位置效应的个体差异和认知加工过程进行解释。神经影像学研究表明, 在想法产生的整个阶段序列位置效应依赖于默认网络后部和突显网络的持续增强激活和协同合作; 在想法产生的后期阶段, 颞-顶区域alpha能量值以及执行控制网络与默认网络的协同模式增强。未来研究需要结合语义和神经动态分析等技术, 对序列位置效应的特异性机制、动态加工机制以及影响因素进行深入探究, 从而为解构创新思维的动态过程提供新的视角。
中图分类号:
陈群林, 丁珂. (2022). 发散思维的序列位置效应:创新想法动态产生机制的新视角. 心理科学进展 , 30(11), 2507-2517.
CHEN Qunlin, DING Ke. (2022). Serial order effect during divergent thinking: A new perspective on the dynamic mechanism of creative thought processes. Advances in Psychological Science, 30(11), 2507-2517.
[1] | 家晓余. (2018). 个体能力观对创造性思维的影响 (博士学位论文). 浙江大学, 杭州. |
[2] | 叶超群, 林郁泓, 刘春雷. (2021). 创造力产生过程中的神经振荡机制. 心理科学进展, 29(4), 697-706 |
[3] | 赵庆柏, 李松清, 陈石, 周治金, 成良. (2015). 创造性问题解决的动态神经加工模式. 心理科学进展, 23(3), 375-384. |
[4] |
Acar, S., Abdulla Alabbasi, A. M., Runco, M. A., & Beketayev, K. (2019). Latency as a predictor of originality in divergent thinking. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 33, 100574.
doi: 10.1016/j.tsc.2019.100574 URL |
[5] | Acar, S., & Runco, M. A. (2017). Latency predicts category switch in divergent thinking. Psychology of Aesthetics Creativity & the Arts, 11(1), 43-51. |
[6] |
Acar, S., Runco, M. A., & Ogurlu, U. (2019). The moderating influence of idea sequence: A re-analysis of the relationship between category switch and latency. Personality and Individual Differences, 142, 214-217.
doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2018.06.013 URL |
[7] |
Agnoli, S., Zanon, M., Mastria, S., Avenanti, A., & Corazza, G. E. (2020). Predicting response originality through brain activity: An analysis of changes in EEG alpha power during the generation of alternative ideas. Neuroimage, 207, 116385.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116385 URL |
[8] |
Anderson, J. R., Pyke, A. A., & Fincham, J. M. (2016). Hidden stages of cognition revealed in patterns of brain activation. Psychological Science, 27(9), 1215-1226.
doi: 10.1177/0956797616654912 pmid: 27440808 |
[9] | Bai, H., Leseman, P. P. M., Moerbeek, M., Kroesbergen, E. H., & Mulder, H. (2021). Serial order effect in divergent thinking in five- to six-year-olds: Individual differences as related to executive functions. Journal of Intelligence, 9(2), 20. |
[10] |
Beaty, R. E., Benedek, M., Kaufman, S. B., & Silvia, P. J. (2015). Default and executive network coupling supports creative idea production. Scientific Reports, 5, 10964.
doi: 10.1038/srep10964 pmid: 26084037 |
[11] |
Beaty, R. E., Benedek, M., Silvia, P. J., & Schacter, D. L. (2016). Creative cognition and brain network dynamics. Trends in cognitive sciences, 20(2), 87-95.
doi: S1364-6613(15)00254-5 pmid: 26553223 |
[12] |
Beaty, R. E., & Johnson, D. R. (2020). Automating creativity assessment with SemDis: An open platform for computing semantic distance. Behavior research methods, 53(2), 757-780.
doi: 10.3758/s13428-020-01453-w URL |
[13] | Beaty, R. E., Kenett, Y. N., Hass, R. W., & Schacter, D. L. (2019). A fan effect for creative thought: Semantic richness facilitates idea quantity but constrains idea quality. PsyArXiv. |
[14] |
Beaty, R. E., & Silvia, P. J. (2012). Why do ideas get more creative across time? An executive interpretation of the serial order effect in divergent thinking tasks. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 6(4), 309-319.
doi: 10.1037/a0029171 URL |
[15] |
Beaty, R. E., Silvia, P. J., Nusbaum, E. C., Jauk, E., & Benedek, M. (2014). The roles of associative and executive processes in creative cognition. Memory and Cognition, 42(7), 1186-1197.
doi: 10.3758/s13421-014-0428-8 pmid: 24898118 |
[16] | Belden, A., Zeng, T., Przysinda, E., Anteraper, S. A., Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., & Loui, P. (2019). Improvising at rest: Differentiating jazz and classical music training with resting state functional connectivity. Neuroimage, 207(2), 116384. |
[17] |
Benedek, M., Jauk, E., Fink, A., Koschutnig, K., Reishofer, G., Ebner, F., & Neubauer, A. C. (2014). To create or to recall? Neural mechanisms underlying the generation of creative new ideas. Neuroimage, 88, 125-133.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.11.021 pmid: 24269573 |
[18] |
Benedek, M., & Neubauer, A. C. (2013). Revisiting mednick’s model on creativity-related differences in associative hierarchies. evidence for a common path to uncommon thought. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 47(4), 273-289.
doi: 10.1002/jocb.35 URL |
[19] |
Benedek, M., Schües, T., Beaty, R. E., Jauk, E., Koschutnig, K., Fink, A., & Neubauer, A. C. (2018). To create or to recall original ideas: Brain processes associated with the imagination of novel object uses. Cortex, 99, 93-102.
doi: S0010-9452(17)30372-6 pmid: 29197665 |
[20] |
Camarda, A., Borst, G., Agogué, M., Habib, M., Weil, B., Houdé, O., & Cassotti, M. (2018). Do we need inhibitory control to be creative? Evidence from a dual-task paradigm. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 12(3), 351-358.
doi: 10.1037/aca0000140 URL |
[21] |
Christensen, A. P., Kenett, Y. N., Cotter, K. N., Beaty, R. E., & Silvia, P. J. (2018). Remotely close associations: Openness to experience and semantic memory structure. European Journal of Personality, 32(4), 480-492.
doi: 10.1002/per.2157 URL |
[22] |
Christensen, P. R., Guilford, J. P., & Wilson, R. C. (1957). Relations of creative responses to working time and instructions. Journal of experimental psychology, 53(2), 82-88.
pmid: 13406185 |
[23] |
Chrysikou, E. G. (2019). Creativity in and out of (cognitive) control. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 27, 94-99.
doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.09.014 |
[24] |
Collins, A. M., & Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading- activation theory of semantic processing. Psychological Review, 82(6), 407-428.
doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.82.6.407 URL |
[25] |
Conrad, R. (1964). Acoustic confusions in immediate memory. British Journal of Psychology, 55(1), 75-84.
doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1964.tb00899.x URL |
[26] |
Desiderato, O., & Sigal, S. (1970). Associative productivity as a function of creativity level and type of verbal stimulus. Psychonomic Science, 18, 357-358.
doi: 10.3758/BF03332398 URL |
[27] | Ding, K., Chen, Q., Yang, W., Wang, X., Yang, D., Ding, C., & Qiu, J. (2021). Recognizing ideas generated in a creative thinking task: Effect of the subjective novelty. Current Psychology, 1-13. |
[28] |
Duan, H., Yang, T., Wang, X., Kan, Y., Zhao, H., Li, Y., & Hu, W. (2020). Is the creativity of lovers better? A behavioral and functional near-infrared spectroscopy hyperscanning study. Current Psychology, 41, 41-54.
doi: 10.1007/s12144-020-01093-5 URL |
[29] |
Fink, A., & Benedek, M. (2014). EEG alpha power and creative ideation. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 44(100), 111-123.
doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.12.002 URL |
[30] | Finke, R. A., Ward, T. B., & Smith, S. M. (1992). Creative cognition: Theory, research, and applications. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. |
[31] |
Friston, K. J., Harrison, L., & Penny, W. (2003). Dynamic causal modelling. Neuroimage, 19(4), 1273-1302.
pmid: 12948688 |
[32] |
Gilhooly, K. J., Fioratou, E., Anthony, S. H., & Wynn, V. (2007). Divergent thinking: Strategies for generating alternative uses for familiar objects. British Journal of Psychology, 98(4), 611-625.
doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.2007.tb00467.x URL |
[33] |
Gray, K., Anderson, S., Chen, E. E., Kelly, J. M., Christian, M. S., Patrick, J., Huang, L., Kenett, Y. N., & Lewis, K. (2019). “Forward flow”: A new measure to quantify free thought and predict creativity. American Psychologist, 74(5), 539-554.
doi: 10.1037/amp0000391 URL |
[34] | Grossberg, S. (1978). A theory of human memory:Self-organization and performance of sensory-motor codes, maps, and plans. In R. Rosen & F. Snell (Eds.), Progress in theoretical biology(Vol. 5, pp. 233-374). NY: Academic Press. |
[35] |
Hass, R. W. (2017). Semantic search during divergent thinking. Cognition, 166, 344-357.
doi: S0010-0277(17)30162-2 pmid: 28601001 |
[36] |
Heinen, D. J. P., & Johnson, D. R. (2018). Semantic distance: An automated measure of creativity that is novel and appropriate. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 12(2), 144-156.
doi: 10.1037/aca0000125 URL |
[37] | Heinonen, J., Numminen, J., Hlushchuk, Y., Antell, H., Taatila, V., & Suomala, J. (2016). Default mode and executive networks areas: Association with the serial order in divergent thinking. PLoS One, 11(9), e0162234. |
[38] |
Jaarsveld, S., Fink, A., Rinner, M., Schwab, D., Benedek, M., & Lachmann, T. (2015). Intelligence in creative processes: An EEG study. Intelligence, 49, 171-178.
doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2015.01.012 URL |
[39] |
Jia, X., Li, W., & Cao, L. (2019). The role of metacognitive components in creative thinking. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 2404.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02404 pmid: 31708842 |
[40] |
Kenett, Y. N., Anaki, D., & Faust, M. (2014). Investigating the structure of semantic networks in low and high creative persons. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 407.
doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00407 pmid: 24959129 |
[41] |
Kenett, Y. N., & Faust, M. (2019). A Semantic Network Cartography of the Creative Mind. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 23(4), 271-274.
doi: S1364-6613(19)30024-5 pmid: 30803872 |
[42] | Kleinkorres, R., Forthmann, B., & Holling, H. (2021). An experimental approach to investigate the involvement of cognitive load in divergent thinking. Journal of Intelligence, 9(1), 3. |
[43] |
Kleinmintz, O. M., Ivancovsky, T., & Shamay-Tsoory, S. G. (2019). The two-fold model of creativity: The neural underpinnings of the generation and evaluation of creative ideas. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 27, 131-138.
doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.11.004 |
[44] |
Kraus, B., Cadle, C., & Simon-Dack, S. (2019). EEG alpha activity is moderated by the serial order effect during divergent thinking. Biological psychology, 145, 84-95.
doi: S0301-0511(18)30717-8 pmid: 30999009 |
[45] |
Levin, I. (1978). Creativity and two modes of associative fluency: Chains and stars. Journal of Personality, 46(3), 426-437.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1978.tb01010.x URL |
[46] | Lewandowsky, S., & Farrell, S. (2008). Short-term memory: New data and a model. Psychology of Learning & Motivation, 49, 1-48. |
[47] |
Logan, G. D. (2018). Automatic control: How experts act without thinking. Psychological Review, 125(4), 453-485.
doi: 10.1037/rev0000100 pmid: 29952620 |
[48] |
Martindale, C., & Hasenfus, N. (1978). EEG differences as a function of creativity, stage of the creative process, and effort to be original. Biological Psychology, 6(3), 157-167.
pmid: 667239 |
[49] |
Mednick, M. T. S., Mednick, S. A., & Jung, C. C. (1964). Continual association as a function of level of creativity and type of verbal stimulus. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 69, 511-515.
pmid: 14212021 |
[50] |
Mednick, S. A. (1962). The associative basis of the creative process. Psychological Review, 69(3), 220-232.
doi: 10.1037/h0048850 URL |
[51] |
Milgram, R. M., & Rabkin, L. (1980). Developmental test of Mednick's associative hierarchies of original thinking. Developmental Psychology, 16(2), 157-158.
doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.16.2.157 URL |
[52] |
Miroshnik, K. G., & Shcherbakova, O. V. (2019). The proportion and creativity of "old" and "new" ideas: Are they related to fluid intelligence? Intelligence, 76, 101384.
doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2019.101384 URL |
[53] | Mota P. A. D., Fernandes H. M., Stark E. A., Cabral J., Heggli O. A., Sousa N., ... Vuust P. (2020). The dynamics of the improvising brain: A study of musical creativity using jazz improvisation. bioRxiv, 2020.2001. 2029.924415. |
[54] |
Olczak, P., & Kaplan, M. F. (1969). Originality and rate of response in association as a function of associative gradient. The American Journal of Psychology, 82(2), 157-167.
doi: 10.2307/1421239 URL |
[55] |
Paek, S. H., Alabbasi, A. M. A., Acar, S., & Runco, M. A. (2021). Is more time better for divergent thinking? A meta-analysis of the time-on-task effect on divergent thinking. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 41, 100894.
doi: 10.1016/j.tsc.2021.100894 URL |
[56] |
Parnes, S. J., & Meadow, A. (1959). Effects of "brainstorming" instructions on creative problem solving by trained and untrained subjects. Journal of Educational Psychology, 50(4), 171-176.
doi: 10.1037/h0047223 URL |
[57] |
Phillips, V. K., & Torrance, E. P. (1977). Levels of originality at earlier and later stages of creativity test tasks. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 11(2), 147-147.
doi: 10.1002/j.2162-6057.1977.tb00602.x URL |
[58] |
Piers, E., & Kirchner, E. (1971). Productivity and uniqueness in continued word association as a function of subject creativity and stimulus properties. Journal of Personality, 39(2), 264-276.
pmid: 5581827 |
[59] |
Rabinovich, M. I., Zaks, M. A., & Varona, P. (2020). Sequential dynamics of complex networks in mind: Consciousness and creativity. Physics Reports, 883, 1-32.
doi: 10.1016/j.physrep.2020.08.003 URL |
[60] |
Riegel, K. F., Riegel, R. M., & Levine, R. S. (1966). An analysis of associative behavior and creativity. Journal of personality and social psychology, 4(1), 50-56.
pmid: 5966182 |
[61] | Rominger, C., Papousek, I., Perchtold, C. M., Benedek, M., Weiss, E. M., Schwerdtfeger, A., & Fink, A. (2019). Creativity is associated with a characteristic U-shaped function of alpha power changes accompanied by an early increase in functional coupling. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 19(4), 1012-1021. |
[62] |
Rumelhart, D. E., & Norman, D. A. (1982). Simulating a skilled typist: A study of skilled cognitive-motor performance. Cognitive Science, 6(1), 1-36.
doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog0601_1 URL |
[63] |
Runco, M. A. (1986). Flexibility and Originality in Children's Divergent Thinking. The Journal of Psychology, 120(4), 345-352.
doi: 10.1080/00223980.1986.9712632 URL |
[64] |
Schwab, D., Benedek, M., Papousek, I., Weiss, E. M., & Fink, A. (2014). The time-course of EEG alpha power changes in creative ideation. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 310.
doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00310 pmid: 24860485 |
[65] |
Seth, A. K., Barrett, A. B., & Barnett, L. (2015). Granger causality analysis in neuroscience and neuroimaging. Journal of Neuroscience, 35(8), 3293-3297.
doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4399-14.2015 pmid: 25716830 |
[66] |
Silvia, P. J., Nusbaum, E. C., & Beaty, R. E. (2017). Old or New? Evaluating the old/new scoring method for divergent thinking tasks. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 51(3), 216-224.
doi: 10.1002/jocb.101 URL |
[67] | Sperling, G. (1960). The Information Available in Brief Visual Presentations. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 74(11), 1-29. |
[68] |
Storm, B. C., & Angello, G. (2010). Overcoming fixation: Creative problem solving and retrieval-induced forgetting. Psychological Science, 21(9), 1263-1265.
doi: 10.1177/0956797610379864 URL |
[69] | Storm, B. C., & Patel, T. N. (2014). Forgetting as a consequence and enabler of creative thinking. Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning Memory & Cognition, 40(6), 1594-1609. |
[70] |
Sun, J., Liu, Z., Rolls, E. T., Chen, Q., Yao, Y., Yang, W., ... Qiu, J. (2019). Verbal creativity correlates with the temporal variability of brain networks during the resting state. Cerebral Cortex, 29(3), 1047-1058.
doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhy010 URL |
[71] |
Tik, M., Sladky, R., Luft, C. D. B., Willinger, D., Hoffmann, A., Banissy, M. J., ... Windischberger, C. (2018). Ultra‐high‐field fMRI insights on insight: Neural correlates of the Aha!‐moment. Human Brain Mapping, 39(8), 3241-3252.
doi: 10.1002/hbm.24073 URL |
[72] |
Unsworth, N., Spillers, G. J., & Brewer, G. A. (2011). Variation in verbal fluency: A latent variable analysis of clustering, switching, and overall performance. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64(3), 447-466.
doi: 10.1080/17470218.2010.505292 URL |
[73] |
Vartanian, O., Beatty, E. L., Smith, I., Blackler, K., Lam, Q., & Forbes, S. (2018). One-way traffic: The inferior frontal gyrus controls brain activation in the middle temporal gyrus and inferior parietal lobule during divergent thinking. Neuropsychologia, 118, 68-78.
doi: S0028-3932(18)30082-4 pmid: 29477840 |
[74] | Volle, E. (2018). Associative and controlled cognition in divergent thinking:Theoretical, experimental, neuroimaging evidence, and new directions. In R. E.Jung & O.Vartanian (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of the Neuroscience of Creativity (pp.333-360). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. |
[75] |
Wang, M., Hao, N., Ku, Y., Grabner, R. H., & Fink, A. (2017). Neural correlates of serial order effect in verbal divergent thinking. Neuropsychologia, 99, 92-100.
doi: S0028-3932(17)30081-7 pmid: 28259772 |
[76] |
Ward, W. C. (1969). Rate and uniqueness in children's creative responding. Child Development, 40(3), 869-878.
pmid: 5810532 |
[77] |
Wise, R. J., & Braga, R. M. (2014). Default mode network: The seat of literary creativity? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(3), 116-117.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.11.001 pmid: 24300116 |
[1] | 王勇丽, 葛胜男, Lancy Lantin Huang, 万勤, 卢海丹. 言语想象的神经机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2023, 31(4): 608-621. |
[2] | 孔祥祯, 张凤翔, 蒲艺. 空间导航的脑网络基础和调控机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2023, 31(3): 330-337. |
[3] | 李清扬, 尹俊婷, 罗俊龙. 才思泉涌“举步”间:体育运动对创造性思维的影响[J]. 心理科学进展, 2023, 31(3): 455-466. |
[4] | 张明霞, 李雨欣, 李瑾, 刘勋. 内外动机对青少年记忆的影响及其神经机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2023, 31(1): 1-9. |
[5] | 王松雪, 程思, 蒋挺, 刘勋, 张明霞. 外在奖赏对陈述性记忆的影响[J]. 心理科学进展, 2023, 31(1): 78-86. |
[6] | 邓尧, 王梦梦, 饶恒毅. 风险决策研究中的仿真气球冒险任务[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(6): 1377-1392. |
[7] | 李亮, 李红. 人们为什么会羞怯:认知机制及神经基础[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(5): 1038-1049. |
[8] | 武晓菲, 肖风, 罗劲. 创造性认知重评在情绪调节中的迁移效应及其神经基础[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(3): 477-485. |
[9] | 章丽娜, 宣宾. 语言产生中词频效应老化的神经基础与时间进程[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(2): 333-342. |
[10] | 李何慧, 黄慧雅, 董琳, 罗跃嘉, 陶伍海. 发展性阅读障碍与小脑异常:小脑的功能和两者的因果关系[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(2): 343-353. |
[11] | 胡佳宝, 雷扬, 定险峰, 程晓荣, 范炤. 大众与个人审美品位的认知与神经机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(2): 354-364. |
[12] | 黄建平, 许婧娴, 宛小昂. 联想学习对消费行为的影响:基于产品搜索经验的视角[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(11): 2414-2423. |
[13] | 王紫乐, 张琪. 视觉搜索中注意模板促进搜索的内在机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(10): 2206-2218. |
[14] | 柳王娟, 定险峰, 程晓荣, 范炤. 序列依赖效应——一种全新的“历史效应”[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(10): 2228-2239. |
[15] | 胡小勇, 杜棠艳, 李兰玉, 王甜甜. 低社会经济地位影响自我调节的神经机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(10): 2278-2290. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||