[1] Allom V.,& Mullan, B.(2015). Two inhibitory control training interventions designed to improve eating behaviour and determine mechanisms of change. Appetite, 892015.02.022
[2] Aulbach, M. B., Knittle, K., van Beurden, S. B., Haukkala, A., & Lawrence, N. S.(2021). App-based food Go/No-Go training: User engagement and dietary intake in an opportunistic observational study. Appetite, 1652021.105315
[3] Best M., Lawrence N. S., Logan G. D., McLaren I. P., & Verbruggen F. (2016). Should I stop or should I go? The role of associations and expectancies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42(1), 115-137. https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000116
[4] Chen Z., Holland R. W., Quandt J., Dijksterhuis A., & Veling H. (2019). When Mere Action Versus Inaction Leads to Robust Preference Change. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 117(4), 721-740. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa0000158
[5] Chen Z., Holland R. W., Quandt J., Dijksterhuis A., & Veling H. (2021). How preference change induced by mere action versus inaction persists over time. Judgment and Decision Making, 16(1), 201-237. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1930297500008366
[6] Chen, Z., & Van Dessel, P. (2024). Action interpretation determines the effects of go/no-go and approach/ avoidance actions on stimulus evaluation. Open Mind, 8, 898-923. https://doi.org/10.1162/opmi_a_00151
[7] Chen Z., Veling H., De Vries S. P., Bijvank B. O., Janssen I. M., Dijksterhuis A., & Holland R. W. (2018). Go/No-Go Training Changes Food Evaluation in Both Morbidly Obese and Normal-Weight Individuals. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 86(12), 980-990. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ccp0000320
[8] Chen Z., Veling H., Dijksterhuis A., & Holland R. W. (2016). How does not responding to appetitive stimuli cause devaluation: Evaluative conditioning or response inhibition? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(12), 1687-1701. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000236
[9] Guerrieri R., Nederkoorn C.,& Jansen, A.(2012). Disinhibition is easier learned than inhibition. The effects of (dis)inhibition training on food intake. Appetite, 592012.04.006
[10] Guitart-Masip,M., Duzel, E., Dolan, R., & Dayan, P.(2014). Action versus valence in decision making. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 182014.01.003
[11] Guitart-Masip,M., Huys, Q. J., Fuentemilla, L., Dayan, P., Duzel, E., & Dolan, R. J.(2012). Go and no-go learning in reward and punishment: Interactions between affect and effect. Neuroimage, 622012.04.024
[12] Jones A., Di Lemma L. C., Robinson E., Christiansen P., Nolan S., Tudur-Smith C., & Field M. (2016). Inhibitory control training for appetitive behaviour change: A meta-analytic investigation of mechanisms of action and moderators of effectiveness. Appetite, 97, 16-28. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2015.11.013
[13] Jones A., Hardman C. A., Lawrence N.,& Field, M.(2018). Cognitive training as a potential treatment for overweight and obesity: A critical review of the evidence. Appetite, 124, 50-67. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2017.05.032
[14] Lawrence N. S., Verbruggen F., Morrison S., Adams R. C.,& Chambers, C. D.(2015). Stopping to food can reduce intake. Effects of stimulus-specificity and individual differences in dietary restraint. Appetite, 85, 91-103. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2014.11.006
[15] Liu H., Holland R. W., Blechert J., Quandt J., & Veling H. (2022). Devaluation of NoGo stimuli is both robust and fragile. Cognition and Emotion, 36(5), 876-893. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2022.2067132
[16] Lowe C. J., Reichelt A. C.,& Hall, P. A.(2019). The Prefrontal Cortex and Obesity: A Health Neuroscience Perspective. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 232019.01.005
[17] Nakamura Y., Okemoto M., & Ikuta T. (2024). Food go/no-go training alters neural circuits for food evaluation for appetite reduction. Appetite, 192, 107099. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2023.107099
[18] Oomen D., Grol M., Spronk D., Booth C.,& Fox, E.(2018). Beating uncontrolled eating: Training inhibitory control to reduce food intake and food cue sensitivity. Appetite, 131, 73-83. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2018.09.007
[19] Schonberg T., Bakkour A., Hover A. M., Mumford J. A., Nagar L., Perez J., & Poldrack R. A. (2014). Changing value through cued approach: An automatic mechanism of behavior change. Nature Neuroscience, 17(4), 625-630. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3673
[20] Song S., Zilverstand A., Gui W., Pan X., & Zhou X. (2022). Reducing craving and consumption in individuals with drug addiction, obesity or overeating through neuromodulation intervention: A systematic review and meta‐analysis of its follow-up effects. Addiction, 117(5), 1242-1255. https://doi.org/10.1111/add.15686
[21] Van Dessel P., Hughes S., & De Houwer J. (2019). How do actions influence attitudes? An inferential account of the impact of action performance on stimulus evaluation. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 23(3), 267-284. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868318795730
[22] Veling H., Becker D., Liu H., Quandt J.,& Holland, R. W.(2022). How go/no-go training changes behavior: A value- based decision-making perspective. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 47, 101206. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2022.101206
[23] Veling H., Holland R. W.,& van Knippenberg, A.(2008). When approach motivation and behavioral inhibition collide: Behavior regulation through stimulus devaluation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 442008.03.004
[24] Veling H., Lawrence N. S., Chen Z., van Koningsbruggen G. M., & Holland R. W. (2017). What is trained during food go/no-go training? A review focusing on mechanisms and a research agenda. Current Addiction Reports, 4(1), 35-41. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40429-017-0131-5
[25] Verbruggen F., Best M., Bowditch W. A., Stevens T.,& McLaren, I. P.(2014). The inhibitory control reflex. Neuropsychologia, 65, 263-278. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.08.014
[26] Verbruggen, F., & Logan, G. D. (2008). Automatic and controlled response inhibition: Associative learning in the go/no-go and stop-signal paradigms. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137(4), 649-672. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013170
[27] Wu Q., Xia H., Shields G. S., Nie H., Li J., Chen H.,& Yang, Y.(2023). Neural correlates underlying preference changes induced by food Go/No-Go training. Appetite, 186, 106578. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2023.106578
[28] Xia H., Wu Q., Shields G. S., Nie H., Hu X., Liu S., … Yang Y. (2024). Neural activity and connectivity are related to food preference changes induced by food go/no-go training. Neuropsychologia, 201, 108919. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2024.108919
[29] Yang Y., Morys F., Wu Q., Li J., & Chen H. (2023). Pilot study of food-specific go/no-go training for overweight individuals: Brain imaging data suggest inhibition shapes food evaluation. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 18(1), nsab137. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsab137
[30] Yang Y., Qi L., Morys F., Wu Q., & Chen H. (2022). Food-specific inhibition training for food devaluation: A meta-analysis. Nutrients, 14(7), 1363. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu14071363
[31] Yang Y., Shields G. S., Wu Q., Liu Y., Chen H., & Guo C. (2019). Cognitive training on eating behaviour and weight loss: A meta-analysis and systematic review. Obesity Reviews, 20(11), 1628-1641. https://doi.org/10.1111/obr.12916
[32] Zhou M., Wang H., Zeng X., Yin P., Zhu J., Chen W., .. Liang X. (2019). Mortality, morbidity, and risk factors in China and its provinces, 1990-2017: A systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2017. Lancet, 394(10204), 1145-1158. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(19)30427-1 |