心理科学进展 ›› 2024, Vol. 32 ›› Issue (8): 1233-1249.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2024.01233 cstr: 32111.14.2024.01233
收稿日期:
2023-12-29
出版日期:
2024-08-15
发布日期:
2024-06-05
基金资助:
LIANG Shujing(), YANG Guangyong
Received:
2023-12-29
Online:
2024-08-15
Published:
2024-06-05
摘要:
金钱作为可以满足人们物质与精神需求的重要资源, 与每个人密切相关。认为自己是“穷”还是“富”会对风险决策产生影响吗? 目前, 从相对层面探究金钱稀缺(富足)感知对风险决策影响的研究非常有限, 金钱稀缺(富足)感知影响风险决策的心理机制尚不明确。本研究以解构金钱稀缺感知与金钱富足感知的生理和心理维度为基础, 开发金钱稀缺与金钱富足感知量表, 界定金钱稀缺情绪和金钱富足情绪; 通过实验室和现场实验、跨时追踪的实证研究和眼动行为实验, 揭示金钱稀缺(富足)感知对个体风险决策(风险感知、预期收益、风险选择)的影响; 从心理机制层面, 构建和阐明不同金钱感知情境下金钱稀缺与富足感知影响风险决策的认知机制(认知偏差、认知反思、执行控制的中介作用)与情绪机制(特定金钱稀缺情绪与金钱富足情绪的中介作用)。
中图分类号:
梁淑静, 杨光勇. (2024). 穷则思变, 富则思安? 金钱稀缺与富足感知对个体风险决策的影响. 心理科学进展 , 32(8), 1233-1249.
LIANG Shujing, YANG Guangyong. (2024). Poverty leads to the desire to change, and wealth leads to the desire for stability: The impact of perceived money scarcity and abundance on individual risk decision-making. Advances in Psychological Science, 32(8), 1233-1249.
[1] | 陈锡友, 张湘一, 吴志辉, 丁道群. (2023). 经济稀缺对风险偏好的影响: 经济稀缺心态的作用. 中国临床心理学杂志, 31(5), 1052-1056+1041. |
[2] | 窦凯, 聂衍刚, 王玉洁, 黎建斌. (2014). 自我损耗促进冒险行为. 心理科学, 37(1), 150-155. |
[3] | 李爱梅, 罗莹, 李斌. (2016). “金钱启动”让人理性还是非理性? ——金钱启动与消费者行为决策. 外国经济与管理, 38(6), 100-112. |
[4] |
李爱梅, 谭磊, 孙海龙, 熊冠星, 潘集阳. (2016). 睡眠剥夺影响风险决策的双系统模型探讨. 心理科学进展, 24(5), 804-814.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2016.00804 |
[5] |
宋云嫱, 徐瑞珩, 刑采. (2017). 风险敏感理论:需要驱动风险决策. 心理科学进展, 25(3), 486-499.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2017.00486 |
[6] | 孙时进, 徐斐. (2019). “贫穷感”对个体生存策略和风险决策的影响. 西南民族大学学报(人文社科版), 40(1), 208-214. |
[7] | 孙彦, 李纾, 殷晓莉. (2007). 决策与推理的双系统——启发式系统和分析系统. 心理科学进展, 15(5), 721-845. |
[8] |
谢晓非, 陆静怡. (2014). 风险决策中的双参照点效应. 心理科学进展, 22(4), 571-579.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2014.00571 |
[9] |
岳灵紫, 李纾, 梁竹苑. (2018). 风险决策中的领域特异性. 心理科学进展, 26(5), 928-938.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2018.00928 |
[10] | Achtziger, A. (2022). Overspending, debt, and poverty. Current Opinion in Psychology, 46, 101342. |
[11] | Aguilar, P., Caballero, A., Sevillano, V., Fernández, I., Muñoz, D., & Carrera, P. (2020). The Relationships between economic scarcity, concrete mindset and risk behavior: A study of Nicaraguan adolescents. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17(11), 3845. |
[12] |
Allen, M. W., Bettinger, R. L., Codding, B. F., Jones, T. L., & Schwitalla, A. W. (2016). Resource scarcity drives lethal aggression among prehistoric hunter-gatherers in central California. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 113(43), 12120-12125.
pmid: 27790997 |
[13] | Aslan, A., Dinc, D., & Kutuk, B. (2017). Joint effects of anxiety and mood induction on risk taking behavior for elderly and young. In M. Gammone, M.A. Icbay, & H. Arslan (Eds.), Recent Developments in Education (pp. 471-478). E-BWN. |
[14] | Auger, V., Sommet, N., & Normand, A. (2024). The Perceived Economic Scarcity Scale: A valid tool with greater predictive utility than income. British Journal of Social Psychology, 1, 1-25. |
[15] | Batista, R. M., Sussman, A. B., & Trueblood, J. S. (2023). Self-other differences in perceptions of wealth. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 104, 104420. |
[16] |
Ben-Zur, H., & Zeidner, M. (2009). Threat to life and risk-taking behaviors: A review of empirical findings and explanatory models. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 13(2), 109-128.
doi: 10.1177/1088868308330104 pmid: 19193927 |
[17] |
Blair, R. J. R. (2016). The neurobiology of impulsive aggression. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 26(1), 4-9.
doi: 10.1089/cap.2015.0088 pmid: 26465707 |
[18] | Blais, A. R., & Weber, E. U. (2006). A Domain-Specific Risk-Taking (DOSPERT) scale for adult populations. Judgment & Decision Making, 1(1), 33-47. |
[19] |
Botvinick, M., & Braver, T. (2015). Motivation and cognitive control: From behavior to neural mechanism. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 83-113.
doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015044 pmid: 25251491 |
[20] | Burlacu, S., Kažemekaitytė, A., Ronzani, P., & Savadori, L. (2021). Blinded by worries: Sin taxes and demand for temptation under financial worries. Theory and Decision, 92(1), 141-187. |
[21] | Caraco, T., Martindale, S., & Whittam, T. S. (1980). An empirical demonstration of risk-sensitive foraging preferences. Animal Behaviour, 28(3), 820-830. |
[22] | Cook, L. A., & Sadeghein, R. (2018). Effects of perceived scarcity on financial decision making. Journal of Public Policy & Marketing, 37(1), 68-87. |
[23] | Daoud, A. (2018). Unifying studies of scarcity, abundance, and sufficiency. Ecological Economics, 147, 208-217. |
[24] | de Bruijn, E. J., & Antonides, G. (2021). Poverty and economic decision making: A review of scarcity theory. Theory and Decision, 92(1), 5-37. |
[25] | Del Missier, F., Mäntylä, T., & Bruine de Bruin, W. (2010). Executive functions in decision making: An individual differences approach. Thinking & Reasoning, 16(2), 69-97. |
[26] | Del Missier, F., Mäntylä, T., & De Bruin, W. B. (2012). Decision‐making competence, executive functioning, and general cognitive abilities. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 25(4), 331-351. |
[27] |
DeSousa, M., Reeve, C. L., & Peterman, A. H. (2020). Development and initial validation of the Perceived Scarcity Scale. Stress Health, 36(2), 131-146.
doi: 10.1002/smi.2908 pmid: 31692256 |
[28] | Devlin, H. C., Johnson, S. L., & Gruber, J. (2015). Feeling good and taking a chance? Associations of hypomania risk with cognitive and behavioral risk taking. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 39(4), 473-479. |
[29] | Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64(1), 135-168. |
[30] |
Fagerström, L., Gustafson, Y., Jakobsson, G., Johansson, S., & Vartiainen, P. (2011). Sense of security among people aged 65 and 75: External and inner sources of security. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 67(6), 1305-1316.
doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2648.2010.05562.x pmid: 21231954 |
[31] | Ferrer, R. A., Maclay, A., Litvak, P. M., & Lerner, J. S. (2017). Revisiting the effects of anger on risk‐taking: Empirical and meta-analytic evidence for differences between males and females. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 30(2), 516-526. |
[32] | Fessler, D. M. T., Pillsworth, E. G., & Flamson, T. J. (2004). Angry men and disgusted women: An evolutionary approach to the influence of emotions on risk taking. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 95(1), 107-123. |
[33] | Festinger, L. A. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations, 7(2), 117-140. |
[34] |
Finucane, M. L., & Gullion, C. M. (2010). Developing a tool for measuring the decision-making competence of older adults. Psychology and Aging, 25(2), 271-288.
doi: 10.1037/a0019106 pmid: 20545413 |
[35] |
Fischer, P., Greitemeyer, T., Kastenmüller, A., Vogrincic, C., & Sauer, A. (2011). The effects of risk-glorifying media exposure on risk-positive cognitions, emotions, and behaviors: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 137(3), 367-390.
doi: 10.1037/a0022267 pmid: 21341887 |
[36] | Frederick, S. (2005). Cognitive reflection and decision making. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 19(4), 25-42. |
[37] | Gamba, A., Manzoni, E., & Stanca, L. (2017). Social comparison and risk taking behavior. Theory and Decision, 82, 221-248. |
[38] |
Glickman, M., Tsetsos, K., & Usher, M. (2018). Attentional selection mediates framing and risk-bias effects. Psychological Science, 29(12), 2010-2019.
doi: 10.1177/0956797618803643 pmid: 30403368 |
[39] | Gonzales, J., Mishra, S., & Camp, R. D. Ⅱ. (2017). For the win: Risk-sensitive decision-making in teams. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 30(2), 462-472. |
[40] | Gonzalez, C., Dana, J., Koshino, H., & Just, M. (2005). The framing effect and risky decisions: Examining cognitive functions with fMRI. Journal of Economic Psychology, 26(1), 1-20. |
[41] | Gonzalez, C., & Mehlhorn, K. (2015). Framing from experience: Cognitive processes and predictions of risky choice. Cognitive Science, 40(5), 1163-1191. |
[42] | Grant, A. M., & Ashford, S. J. (2008). The dynamics of proactivity at work. Research in Organizational Behavior, 2008, 28, 3-34. |
[43] |
Grant, A. M., & Gino, F. (2010). A little thanks goes a long way: Explaining why gratitude expressions motivate prosocial behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98(6), 946-955.
doi: 10.1037/a0017935 pmid: 20515249 |
[44] | Guillemette, M., Browning, C., & Payne, P. (2015). Don't like the picture? Change the frame: The impact of cognitive ability and framing on risky choice. Applied Economics Letters, 22(18), 1515-1518. |
[45] | Hansen, J., Kutzner, F., & Wänke, M. (2013). Money and thinking: Reminders of money trigger abstract construal and shape consumer judgments. Journal of Consumer Research, 39(6), 1154-1166. |
[46] |
Haushofer, J., & Fehr, E. (2014). On the psychology of poverty. Science, 344(6186), 862-867.
doi: 10.1126/science.1232491 pmid: 24855262 |
[47] | Huang, L., Li, X., Xu, F., & Li, F. (2023). Consequences of scarcity: The impact of perceived scarcity on executive functioning and its neural basis. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 17, 1158544. |
[48] |
Huijsmans, I., Ma, I., Micheli, L., Civai, C., Stallen, M., & Sanfey, A. G. (2019). A scarcity mindset alters neural processing underlying consumer decision making. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 116(24), 11699-11704.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1818572116 pmid: 31123150 |
[49] | Iotzov, V., Weiß, M., Windmann, S., & Hein, G. (2022). Valence framing induces cognitive bias. Current Psychology, 42(34), 30381-30392. |
[50] |
Juergensen, J., Weaver, J. S., May, C. N., & Demaree, H. A. (2018). More than money: Experienced positive affect reduces risk-taking behavior on a real-world gambling task. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 2116.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02116 pmid: 30455658 |
[51] |
Kahneman, D. (2003). A perspective on judgment and choice: Mapping bounded rationality. American Psychologist, 58(9), 697-720.
doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.58.9.697 pmid: 14584987 |
[52] |
Kahneman, D., & Frederick, A. S. (2007). Frames and brains: Elicitation and control of response tendencies. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(2), 45-46.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.11.007 pmid: 17188556 |
[53] | Kaplan, H. S, & Gangestad, S. W. (2005). Life history theory and evolutionary psychology. In D. M. Buss (Ed.), Handbook of evolutionary psychology (pp. 68-95). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. |
[54] | Lakshminarayanan, V. R., Chen, M. K., & Santos, L. R. (2011). The evolution of decision-making under risk: Framing effects in monkey risk preferences. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47(3), 689-693. |
[55] |
Li, J., Liu, L., Sun, Y., Fan, W., Li, M., & Zhong, Y. P. (2020). Exposure to money modulates neural responses to outcome evaluations involving social reward. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 15(1), 111-121.
doi: 10.1093/scan/nsaa019 pmid: 32064532 |
[56] | Liang, S., Fan, P., & Yang, G. (2023). To take a risk or not? The effect of perceived scarcity on risky choices. Behavioral Sciences, 13(9), 1-8. |
[57] | Liang, S., Ye, D., & Liu, Y. (2021). The effect of perceived scarcity: Experiencing scarcity increases risk taking. The Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, 155(1), 59-89. |
[58] | Liang, S., & Zou, Y. (2018). Validation of adult decision- making competence in Chinese college students. Judgment and Decision Making, 13(4), 393-400. |
[59] |
Mani, A., Mullainathan, S., Shafir, E., & Zhao, J. (2013). Poverty impedes cognitive function. Science, 341(6149), 976-980.
doi: 10.1126/science.1238041 pmid: 23990553 |
[60] | Mehta, R., & Zhu, M. (2016). Creating when you have less: The impact of resource scarcity on product use creativity. Journal of Consumer Research, 42(5), 767-782. |
[61] | Mena, L., Crosby, R. A., & Geter, A. (2017). A novel measure of poverty and its association with elevated sexual risk behavior among young black MSM. International Journal of Std & Aids, 28(6), 602-607. |
[62] | Meuris, J., & Leana, C. R. (2015). The high cost of low wages: Economic scarcity effects in organizations. Research in Organizational Behavior, 35, 143-158. |
[63] | Mishra, S., Barclay, P., & Sparks, A. (2017). The relative state model integrating need-based and ability-based pathways to risk-taking. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 21(2), 176-198. |
[64] |
Mishra, S., & Fiddick, L. (2012). Beyond gains and losses: The effect of need on risky choice in framed decisions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102(6), 1136-1147.
doi: 10.1037/a0027855 pmid: 22486678 |
[65] | Mishra, S., Hing, L. S., & Lalumiere, M. L. (2015). Inequality and risk-taking. Evolutionary Psychology, 13(3), 1-11. |
[66] |
Mittal, C., & Griskevicius, V. (2014). Sense of control under uncertainty depends on people's childhood environment: A life history theory approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 107(4), 621-637.
doi: 10.1037/a0037398 pmid: 25133717 |
[67] | Mullainathan, S., & Shafir, E. (2013). Scarcity: Why having too little means so much. Macmillan. |
[68] |
Obeso, I., Herrero, M. T., Ligneul, R., Rothwell, J. C., & Jahanshahi, M. A. (2021). A Causal role for the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in avoidance of risky choices and making advantageous selections. Neuroscience, 458, 166-179.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2020.12.035 pmid: 33476698 |
[69] | Parker, A. M., & Fischhoff, B. (2005). Decision-making competence: External validation through an individual- differences approach. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 18(1), 1-27. |
[70] | Parker, A. M., & Weller, J. A. (2015). Greater decision- making competence is associated with greater expected- value sensitivity, but not overall risk taking: An examination of concurrent validity. Frontier in Psychology, 6, 717. |
[71] |
Payne, B. K., Brown-lannuzzi, J. L., & Hannay, J. W. (2017). Economic inequality increases risk taking. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 114(18), 4643-4648.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1616453114 pmid: 28416655 |
[72] | Pitesa, M., & Thau, S. (2017). Resource scarcity, effort, and performance in physically demanding jobs: An evolutionary explanation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 103(3), 237-248. |
[73] | Prediger, S., Vollan, B. & Herrmann, B. (2014). Resource scarcity and anti-social behavior. Journal of Public Economics, 119, 1-9. |
[74] | Qi, W., Guo, X., Wu, X., Marinova, D., & Fan, J. (2018). Do the sunk cost effect and cognitive dissonance increase risk perception? An empirical study in the context of city smog. Quality & Quantity, 52(5), 2269-2289. |
[75] | Ridley, M., Rao, G., Schilbach, F., & Patel, V. (2020). Poverty, depression, and anxiety: Causal evidence and mechanisms. Science, 370(6522), eaay0214. |
[76] | Romm, K. F., Barry, C. M., & Alvis, L. M. (2020). How the rich get riskier: Parenting and higher-SES emerging adults' risk behaviors. Journal of Adult Development, 27(4), 281-293. |
[77] | Roux, C., Goldsmith, K., Bonezzi, A., & Bonezzi, A. (2015). On the psychology of scarcity: When reminders of resource scarcity promote selfish (and generous) behavior. Journal of Consumer Research, 42(4), ucv048. |
[78] | Rovelli, K., & Allegretta, R. A. (2023). Framing decision- making: The role of executive functions, cognitive bias and reward. Neuropsychological Trends, 33, 37-50. |
[79] |
Sacks, D. W., Stevenson, B., & Wolfers, J. (2012). The new stylized facts about income and subjective well-being. Emotion, 12(6), 1181-1187.
doi: 10.1037/a0029873 pmid: 23231724 |
[80] |
Shah, A. K., Mullainathan, S., & Shafir, E. (2012). Some consequences of having too little. Science, 338(6107), 682-685.
doi: 10.1126/science.1222426 pmid: 23118192 |
[81] |
Shah, A. K., Shafir, E., & Mullainathan, S. (2015). Scarcity frames value. Psychological Science, 26(4), 402-412.
doi: 10.1177/0956797614563958 pmid: 25676256 |
[82] | Sheehy-Skeffington, J., & Rea, J. (2017). How poverty affects people's decision-making processes (pp. 1-73). York: Joseph Rowntree Foundation. |
[83] |
Simpson, J. A., Griskevicius, V., Kuo, S. I. C., Sung, S., & Collins, W. A. (2012). Evolution, stress, and sensitive periods: The influence of unpredictability in early versus late childhood on sex and risky behavior. Developmental Psychology, 48(3), 674-686.
doi: 10.1037/a0027293 pmid: 22329381 |
[84] |
Sirota, M., & Juanchich, M. (2018). Effect of response format on cognitive reflection: Validating a two- and four-option multiple choice question version of the cognitive reflection test. Behavior Research Methods, 50(6), 2511-2522.
doi: 10.3758/s13428-018-1029-4 pmid: 29589333 |
[85] | van Dijk, W. W., van der Werf, M. M. B., & van Dillen, L. F. (2022). The Psychological Inventory of Financial Scarcity (PIFS): A psychometric evaluation. Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Economics, 101, 101939. |
[86] | Venn, D., & Strazdins, L. (2017). Your money or your time? How both types of scarcity matter to physical activity and healthy eating? Social Science & Medicine, 172, 98-106. |
[87] |
Vohs, K. D., Baumeister, R. F., Schmeichel, B. J., Twenge, J. M., Nelson, N. M., & Tice, D. M. (2008). Making choices impairs subsequent self-control: A limited-resource account of decision making, self-regulation, and active initiative. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94(5), 883-898.
doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.94.5.883 pmid: 18444745 |
[88] |
Vohs, K. D., Mead, N. L., & Goode, M. R. (2006). The psychological consequences of money. Science, 314(5802), 1154-1156.
pmid: 17110581 |
[89] | Wang, X. T., Zheng, R., Xuan, Y. H., Chen, J., & Li, S. (2016). Not all risks are created equal: A twin study and meta-analyses of risk taking across seven domains. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(11), 1548-1560. |
[90] | Wang, Z., Jetten, J., & Steffens, N. K. (2019). The more you have, the more you want? Higher social class predicts a greater desire for wealth and status. European Journal of Social Psychology, 50(2), 360-375. |
[91] | Ward, S., & Kim, J. (2022). How does money make life meaningful? Socioeconomic status, financial self-efficacy, and meaning in life. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 18(6), 906-923. |
[92] | Ward, S. J., & King, L. A. (2019). Exploring the place of financial status in the good life: Income and meaning in life. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 14(3), 312-323. |
[93] | Weber, E. U., Blais, A. R., & Betz, N. E. (2002). A Domain-Specific Risk-Attitude Scale: Measuring risk perceptions and risk behaviors. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 15(4), 263-290. |
[94] | Weller, J. A., Ceschi, A., & Randolph, C. (2015). Decision- making competence predicts domain-specific risk attitudes. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 139420. |
[95] | Whillans, A. V., Caruso, E. M., & Dunn, E. W. (2017). Both selfishness and selflessness start with the self: How wealth shapes responses to charitable appeals. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 70, 242-250. |
[96] |
Williams, C. C., Saffer, B. Y., Mcculloch, R. B., & Krigolson, O. E. (2016). The scarcity heuristic impacts reward processing within the medial-frontal cortex. Neuroreport, 27(7), 522-526.
doi: 10.1097/WNR.0000000000000575 pmid: 27031875 |
[97] | Wohl, M. J., Branscombe, N. R., & Lister, J. J. (2014). When the going gets tough economic threat increases financial risk taking in games of chance. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 5(2), 211-217. |
[98] | Yu, L., Gao, J., Kong, Y., & Huang, L. (2023). Impact of perceived scarcity on delay of gratification: Meditation effects of self-efficacy and self-control. Current Psychology, 43, 2899-2907. |
[99] | Zhang, K., Li, Y., Chen, X., Xu, Y., Zhang, C., Wen, J., & Xu, S. (2022). Money priming enhances sensitivity to the outcome feedback of decision-making under uncertainty: Evidence from an ERP study. Neuropsychologia, 176, 108390. |
[100] | Zhao, J., & Tomm, B. M. (2018). Psychological responses to scarcity. In O. Braddick (Ed.), Oxford research encyclopedia of psychology (pp. 1-21). NY: Oxford University Press. |
[101] | Zotova, O. Y., & Karapetyan, L. V. (2018). Psychological security as the foundation of personal psychological wellbeing (analytical review). Psychology in Russia: State of the Art, 11(2), 100-113. |
[1] | 张顺民, 李柯蒨. 自我控制减少拖延行为的认知神经机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2023, 31(9): 1560-1568. |
[2] | 陈静, 章曼露, 李雨阳. 管住嘴迈开腿:身体活动当量标签促进健康行为及其认知机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2023, 31(7): 1228-1238. |
[3] | 邹迪, 李红, 王福顺. 唤醒定义探析及其认知神经生理基础[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(9): 2020-2033. |
[4] | 邓尧, 王梦梦, 饶恒毅. 风险决策研究中的仿真气球冒险任务[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(6): 1377-1392. |
[5] | 张航, 孟乐, 张积家. 为什么和声能诱发音乐情感?——音乐协和性的作用及其认知机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(4): 817-833. |
[6] | 张琳琳, 魏坤琳, 李晶. 儿童的人际运动同步[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(3): 623-634. |
[7] | 王晓田, 王娜, 何金波. 前瞻性情绪作为社会风险的信息源假说:公共场景下风险决策的情绪及文化机制探讨[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(6): 959-966. |
[8] | 隋雪, 史汉文, 李雨桐. 语言加工过程中的观点采择及其认知机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(6): 990-999. |
[9] | 张晶晶, 梁啸岳, 陈伊笛, 陈庆荣. 音乐句法加工的认知机制与音乐结构的影响模式[J]. 心理科学进展, 2020, 28(6): 883-892. |
[10] | 彭嘉熙, 赵鹿鸣, 方鹏, 曹云飞, 苗丹民, 肖玮. 睡眠剥夺对风险决策的影响机制探讨[J]. 心理科学进展, 2020, 28(11): 1789-1799. |
[11] | 孟彧琦, 邢采, 刘新辉. 负期望条件下的末期效应:情绪动机诱发风险寻求[J]. 心理科学进展, 2019, 27(5): 789-795. |
[12] | 陈琳, 田晓明, 段锦云. 建议采纳的认知机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2019, 27(1): 149-159. |
[13] | 黄欢, 刘博, 周晨琛, 姬鸣. 前瞻记忆意图后效中执行错误的机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2018, 26(9): 1600-1607. |
[14] | 杜玮玮, 宋婷, 李富洪. 双价效应及其认知机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2018, 26(11): 1969-1975. |
[15] | 范伟, 杨博, 刘娟, 傅小兰. 自我欺骗:为了调节个体心理状态[J]. 心理科学进展, 2017, 25(8): 1349-1359. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||