心理科学进展 ›› 2019, Vol. 27 ›› Issue (5): 773-788.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2019.00773
收稿日期:
2018-05-18
出版日期:
2019-05-15
发布日期:
2019-03-20
通讯作者:
袁博
E-mail:yuanbopsy@gmail.com
基金资助:
TANG Ming, LI Weiqiang, LIU Fuhui, YUAN Bo()
Received:
2018-05-18
Online:
2019-05-15
Published:
2019-03-20
Contact:
YUAN Bo
E-mail:yuanbopsy@gmail.com
摘要:
内疚作为一种典型的道德情绪, 被认为具有亲社会作用, 但很多研究却发现内疚并不总能促进亲社会行为。为了明确内疚对亲社会行为的作用, 分析造成结论分歧的可能原因, 本研究采用元分析方法探讨了特质内疚与亲社会行为的关系以及状态内疚对亲社会行为的影响。共有46篇文献92个独立样本纳入元分析(N = 17248)。元分析结果表明:(1)特质内疚与亲社会行为之间存在中等程度的正相关, 二者之间的关系受到亲社会行为类型的调节, 相比较捐赠、助人、环保行为等, 特质内疚与补偿之间的相关更强; (2)启动内疚状态能显著提升个体的亲社会行为, 但两者之间的关联呈较小的效应量, 亲社会行为对象在其中起到调节作用, 感到内疚的个体更愿意对受害方做出亲社会行为; (3) p曲线(p-curve)分析发现, 两个元分析研究的p曲线均呈显著右偏态, 表明特质内疚与亲社会行为的关系以及状态内疚对亲社会行为的影响均存在真实的效应, 而不是出版偏倚或者p hacking导致。
中图分类号:
汤明, 李伟强, 刘福会, 袁博. (2019). 内疚与亲社会行为的关系:来自元分析的证据. 心理科学进展 , 27(5), 773-788.
TANG Ming, LI Weiqiang, LIU Fuhui, YUAN Bo. (2019). The association between guilt and prosocial behavior: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Advances in Psychological Science, 27(5), 773-788.
作者(发表时间) | 样本 数目 | 年龄 | 内疚 测量方法 | 内疚 性质 | 内疚 类型 | 亲社会 行为类型 | 亲社会 行为对象 | 出版 与否 | 文化 背景 | 相关 系数 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
丁菀, 2015 | 491 | Ad | UQ | S | IBG | T | UV | UP | E | 0.32 |
方圆, 2017 | 388 | Ad | Q | T | IBG | T | UV | P | E | 0.50 |
黄晓娇, 2016-a | 577 | A | UQ | S | IBG | R | NV | UP | E | 0.65 |
黄晓娇, 2016-c | 25 | A | UQ | S | IBG | C | NV | UP | E | -0.38 |
康家慧, 2014 | 1859 | Ad | Q | T | IBG | T | UV | UP | E | 0.46 |
李磊, 2012-a | 413 | Ad | Q | T | IBG | T | UV | UP | E | 0.18 |
李磊, 2012-b | 34 | Ad | Q | T | IBG | O | NV | UP | E | 0.05 |
李娟, 2008 | 261 | A | UQ | S | IBG | O | UV | UP | E | 0.35 |
刘金梅, 2009-a | 655 | A | UQ | S | IBG | R | V | UP | E | 0.49 |
刘谞, 2011-a | 157 | Ad | UQ | S | IBG | R | V | UP | E | 0.31 |
吕丽, 2017 | 358 | Ad | UQ | S | GBG | R | NV | UP | E | 0.51 |
毛静思, 2012 | 667 | A | Q | T | IBG | T | UV | UP | E | 0.36 |
Brown & Cehajic, 2008-a | 173 | Ad | UQ | S | GBG | R | V | P | W | 0.46 |
Brown & Cehajic, 2008-b | 257 | A | UQ | S | GBG | R | V | P | W | 0.49 |
Cehajic-Clancy et al., 2011-a | 139 | Ad | UQ | S | GBG | R | V | P | W | 0.66 |
Cehajic-Clancy et al., 2011-b | 97 | Ad | UQ | S | GBG | R | V | P | W | 0.42 |
Figueiredo et al., 2016 | 464 | Ad | UQ | S | GBG | O | V | P | W | 0.17 |
Halmburger et al., 2015 | 63 | Ad | UQ | S | IBG | O | NV | P | W | -0.06 |
Harth et al., 2013-a | 67 | Ad | UQ | S | GBG | R | V | P | W | 0.37 |
Harth et al., 2013-a | 67 | Ad | UQ | S | GBG | E | V | P | W | 0.17 |
Harth et al., 2013-b | 81 | Ad | UQ | S | GBG | R | V | P | W | 0.6 |
Harth et al., 2013-b | 81 | Ad | UQ | S | GBG | E | V | P | W | -0.21 |
Iyer et al., 2007-a | 194 | Ad | UQ | S | GBG | R | V | P | W | 0.39 |
Iyer et al., 2007-b | 185 | Ad | UQ | S | GBG | R | V | P | W | 0.35 |
Jordan et al., 2016-a | 101 | Ad | UQ | T | IBG | F | NV | P | W | 0.37 |
Kahn et al., 2016-a | 87 | Ad | UQ | S | IBG | C | V | P | W | 0.32 |
Kahn et al., 2016-b | 85 | Ad | UQ | S | IBG | C | V | P | W | 0.23 |
Konstam et al., 2011 | 148 | Ad | Q | T | IBG | F | NV | P | W | 0.17 |
Leach et al., 2006-a | 159 | Ad | Q | S | GBG | I | V | P | W | 0.15 |
Leach et al., 2006-b | 203 | Ad | UQ | S | GBG | R | V | P | W | 0.40 |
Leonard et al., 2015-a | 363 | Ad | Q | T | GBG | O | V | P | W | 0.17 |
Linda et al., 2016 | 568 | Ad | Q | T | IBG | T | UV | P | W | 0.21 |
作者(发表时间) | 样本 数目 | 年龄 | 内疚 测量方法 | 内疚 性质 | 内疚 类型 | 亲社会 行为类型 | 亲社会 行为对象 | 出版 与否 | 文化 背景 | 相关 系数 |
Mashuri et al., 2017-a | 200 | Ad | UQ | S | GBG | R | V | P | W | 0.43 |
Mashuri et al., 2017-b | 200 | Ad | UQ | S | GBG | R | V | P | W | 0.19 |
Mcgarty et al., 2005-a | 164 | Ad | UQ | T | GBG | O | V | P | W | 0.58 |
Mcgarty et al., 2005-b | 106 | Ad | UQ | S | GBG | O | V | P | W | 0.66 |
Ongley et al., 2014 | 160 | C | UQ | S | IBG | O | NV | P | W | 0.14 |
Roberts et al., 2014 | 93 | C | Q | T | IBG | O | NV | P | W | 0.59 |
Roberts et al., 2014 | 93 | C | Q | T | IBG | C | NV | P | W | 0.57 |
Roos et al., 2014 | 395 | C | Q | S | IBG | T | UV | P | W | 0.34 |
Rotella & Richeson, 2013-b | 39 | Ad | Q | S | IBG | R | NV | P | W | 0.79 |
Shepherd et al., 2013-a | 179 | Ad | UQ | S | GBG | R | V | P | W | 0.49 |
Shepherd et al., 2013-b | 186 | Ad | UQ | S | GBG | R | V | P | W | 0.66 |
Strelan, 2007 | 176 | Ad | Q | T | IBG | F | NV | P | W | -0.33 |
Swim & Miller, 1999-a | 102 | Ad | UQ | T | GBG | I | V | P | W | 0.51 |
Swim & Miller, 1999-b | 51 | Ad | UQ | T | GBG | I | V | P | W | 0.4 |
Swim & Miller, 1999-c | 364 | Ad | UQ | T | GBG | I | V | P | W | 0.52 |
Swim & Miller, 1999-d | 124 | Ad | UQ | T | GBG | I | V | P | W | 0.19 |
Wan et al., 2016-a | 180 | Ad | Q | S | IBG | O | NV | P | E | 0.33 |
Wan et al., 2016-b | 180 | Ad | Q | S | IBG | O | NV | P | E | 0.3 |
Wohl et al., 2013-a | 284 | Ad | UQ | S | GBG | O | V | P | W | -0.35 |
Wohl et al., 2013-b | 88 | Ad | UQ | S | GBG | O | V | P | W | 0.01 |
Zimmermann et al., 2011-a | 96 | Ad | UQ | S | GBG | R | V | P | W | 0.31 |
Zimmermann et al., 2011-b | 156 | Ad | UQ | S | GBG | O | V | P | W | 0.24 |
表1 元分析中纳入的原始研究(特质内疚与亲社会行为之间的关系)
作者(发表时间) | 样本 数目 | 年龄 | 内疚 测量方法 | 内疚 性质 | 内疚 类型 | 亲社会 行为类型 | 亲社会 行为对象 | 出版 与否 | 文化 背景 | 相关 系数 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
丁菀, 2015 | 491 | Ad | UQ | S | IBG | T | UV | UP | E | 0.32 |
方圆, 2017 | 388 | Ad | Q | T | IBG | T | UV | P | E | 0.50 |
黄晓娇, 2016-a | 577 | A | UQ | S | IBG | R | NV | UP | E | 0.65 |
黄晓娇, 2016-c | 25 | A | UQ | S | IBG | C | NV | UP | E | -0.38 |
康家慧, 2014 | 1859 | Ad | Q | T | IBG | T | UV | UP | E | 0.46 |
李磊, 2012-a | 413 | Ad | Q | T | IBG | T | UV | UP | E | 0.18 |
李磊, 2012-b | 34 | Ad | Q | T | IBG | O | NV | UP | E | 0.05 |
李娟, 2008 | 261 | A | UQ | S | IBG | O | UV | UP | E | 0.35 |
刘金梅, 2009-a | 655 | A | UQ | S | IBG | R | V | UP | E | 0.49 |
刘谞, 2011-a | 157 | Ad | UQ | S | IBG | R | V | UP | E | 0.31 |
吕丽, 2017 | 358 | Ad | UQ | S | GBG | R | NV | UP | E | 0.51 |
毛静思, 2012 | 667 | A | Q | T | IBG | T | UV | UP | E | 0.36 |
Brown & Cehajic, 2008-a | 173 | Ad | UQ | S | GBG | R | V | P | W | 0.46 |
Brown & Cehajic, 2008-b | 257 | A | UQ | S | GBG | R | V | P | W | 0.49 |
Cehajic-Clancy et al., 2011-a | 139 | Ad | UQ | S | GBG | R | V | P | W | 0.66 |
Cehajic-Clancy et al., 2011-b | 97 | Ad | UQ | S | GBG | R | V | P | W | 0.42 |
Figueiredo et al., 2016 | 464 | Ad | UQ | S | GBG | O | V | P | W | 0.17 |
Halmburger et al., 2015 | 63 | Ad | UQ | S | IBG | O | NV | P | W | -0.06 |
Harth et al., 2013-a | 67 | Ad | UQ | S | GBG | R | V | P | W | 0.37 |
Harth et al., 2013-a | 67 | Ad | UQ | S | GBG | E | V | P | W | 0.17 |
Harth et al., 2013-b | 81 | Ad | UQ | S | GBG | R | V | P | W | 0.6 |
Harth et al., 2013-b | 81 | Ad | UQ | S | GBG | E | V | P | W | -0.21 |
Iyer et al., 2007-a | 194 | Ad | UQ | S | GBG | R | V | P | W | 0.39 |
Iyer et al., 2007-b | 185 | Ad | UQ | S | GBG | R | V | P | W | 0.35 |
Jordan et al., 2016-a | 101 | Ad | UQ | T | IBG | F | NV | P | W | 0.37 |
Kahn et al., 2016-a | 87 | Ad | UQ | S | IBG | C | V | P | W | 0.32 |
Kahn et al., 2016-b | 85 | Ad | UQ | S | IBG | C | V | P | W | 0.23 |
Konstam et al., 2011 | 148 | Ad | Q | T | IBG | F | NV | P | W | 0.17 |
Leach et al., 2006-a | 159 | Ad | Q | S | GBG | I | V | P | W | 0.15 |
Leach et al., 2006-b | 203 | Ad | UQ | S | GBG | R | V | P | W | 0.40 |
Leonard et al., 2015-a | 363 | Ad | Q | T | GBG | O | V | P | W | 0.17 |
Linda et al., 2016 | 568 | Ad | Q | T | IBG | T | UV | P | W | 0.21 |
作者(发表时间) | 样本 数目 | 年龄 | 内疚 测量方法 | 内疚 性质 | 内疚 类型 | 亲社会 行为类型 | 亲社会 行为对象 | 出版 与否 | 文化 背景 | 相关 系数 |
Mashuri et al., 2017-a | 200 | Ad | UQ | S | GBG | R | V | P | W | 0.43 |
Mashuri et al., 2017-b | 200 | Ad | UQ | S | GBG | R | V | P | W | 0.19 |
Mcgarty et al., 2005-a | 164 | Ad | UQ | T | GBG | O | V | P | W | 0.58 |
Mcgarty et al., 2005-b | 106 | Ad | UQ | S | GBG | O | V | P | W | 0.66 |
Ongley et al., 2014 | 160 | C | UQ | S | IBG | O | NV | P | W | 0.14 |
Roberts et al., 2014 | 93 | C | Q | T | IBG | O | NV | P | W | 0.59 |
Roberts et al., 2014 | 93 | C | Q | T | IBG | C | NV | P | W | 0.57 |
Roos et al., 2014 | 395 | C | Q | S | IBG | T | UV | P | W | 0.34 |
Rotella & Richeson, 2013-b | 39 | Ad | Q | S | IBG | R | NV | P | W | 0.79 |
Shepherd et al., 2013-a | 179 | Ad | UQ | S | GBG | R | V | P | W | 0.49 |
Shepherd et al., 2013-b | 186 | Ad | UQ | S | GBG | R | V | P | W | 0.66 |
Strelan, 2007 | 176 | Ad | Q | T | IBG | F | NV | P | W | -0.33 |
Swim & Miller, 1999-a | 102 | Ad | UQ | T | GBG | I | V | P | W | 0.51 |
Swim & Miller, 1999-b | 51 | Ad | UQ | T | GBG | I | V | P | W | 0.4 |
Swim & Miller, 1999-c | 364 | Ad | UQ | T | GBG | I | V | P | W | 0.52 |
Swim & Miller, 1999-d | 124 | Ad | UQ | T | GBG | I | V | P | W | 0.19 |
Wan et al., 2016-a | 180 | Ad | Q | S | IBG | O | NV | P | E | 0.33 |
Wan et al., 2016-b | 180 | Ad | Q | S | IBG | O | NV | P | E | 0.3 |
Wohl et al., 2013-a | 284 | Ad | UQ | S | GBG | O | V | P | W | -0.35 |
Wohl et al., 2013-b | 88 | Ad | UQ | S | GBG | O | V | P | W | 0.01 |
Zimmermann et al., 2011-a | 96 | Ad | UQ | S | GBG | R | V | P | W | 0.31 |
Zimmermann et al., 2011-b | 156 | Ad | UQ | S | GBG | O | V | P | W | 0.24 |
作者(发表时间) | 实验组 样本量 | 控制组 样本量 | 年龄 | 内疚 类型 | 内疚诱 发方式 | 亲社会 行为类型 | 亲身会 行为对象 | 控制组 | 出版 与否 | 文化 背景 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
丁芳 等, 2014-a | 60 | 60 | A | IBG | CS | O | V | C | P | E |
丁芳 等, 2014-b | 60 | 60 | A | IBG | CS | O | NV | C | P | E |
杜灵燕, 2012-a | 407 | 360 | A | IBG | MS | H | NV | S | UP | E |
杜灵燕, 2012-b | 35 | 37 | A | IBG | MS | D | NV | C | UP | E |
杜灵燕, 2012-b | 35 | 35 | A | IBG | MS | D | NV | S | UP | E |
刘谞, 2011-b | 48 | 48 | Ad | IBG | CS | R | V | C | UP | E |
孙铭鸿, 2015 | 85 | 80 | A | IBG | MS | D | NV | C | UP | E |
张晓贤 等, 2012-a | 27 | 28 | C | IBG | CS | H | NV | C | P | E |
张晓贤 等, 2012-b | 33 | 28 | C | IBG | CS | H | NV | C | P | E |
de Hooge et al., 2007-a | 42 | 39 | Ad | IBG | GR | O | NV | C | P | W |
de Hooge et al., 2007-a | 42 | 45 | Ad | IBG | GR | O | NV | S | P | W |
de Hooge et al., 2007-b | 44 | 37 | Ad | IBG | GR | H | NV | C | P | W |
de Hooge et al., 2007-b | 44 | 48 | Ad | IBG | GR | H | NV | S | P | W |
作者(发表时间) | 实验组 样本量 | 控制组 样本量 | 年龄 | 内疚 类型 | 内疚诱 发方式 | 亲社会 行为类型 | 亲身会 行为对象 | 控制组 | 出版 与否 | 文化 背景 |
de Hooge et al., 2011-a | 16 | 17 | Ad | IBG | GR | R | V | C | P | W |
de Hooge et al., 2011-a | 16 | 17 | Ad | IBG | GR | D | NV | C | P | W |
de Hooge et al., 2011-b | 58 | 34 | Ad | IBG | GR | R | V | C | P | W |
de Hooge et al., 2011-b | 58 | 34 | Ad | IBG | GR | D | NV | C | P | W |
de Hooge et al., 2011-c | 25 | 24 | Ad | IBG | GR | R | V | C | P | W |
de Hooge et al., 2011-c | 25 | 24 | Ad | IBG | GR | D | NV | C | P | W |
de Hooge et al., 2011-d | 71 | 72 | Ad | IBG | CS | R | V | C | P | W |
de Hooge et al., 2011-d | 71 | 72 | Ad | IBG | CS | D | NV | C | P | W |
de Hooge et al., 2011-e | 22 | 22 | Ad | IBG | CS | R | V | C | P | W |
de Hooge et al., 2011-f | 23 | 23 | Ad | IBG | MS | R | V | C | P | W |
de Hooge et al., 2011-f | 23 | 23 | Ad | IBG | MS | R | V | S | P | W |
Ferguson & Branscombe, 2010 | 19 | 55 | Ad | GBG | MS | E | V | C | P | W |
Furukawa et al., 2016-a | 107 | 107 | Ad | IBG | MS | R | V | C | P | E |
Furukawa et al., 2016-b | 107 | 107 | Ad | IBG | MS | O | NV | C | P | E |
Graton et al., 2016-a | 28 | 28 | Ad | IBG | GR | E | NV | C | P | W |
Graton et al., 2016-a | 28 | 28 | Ad | IBG | GR | E | NV | S | P | W |
Graton et al., 2016-b | 45 | 45 | Ad | IBG | GR | E | NV | C | P | W |
Graton et al., 2016-c | 41 | 41 | Ad | IBG | GR | E | NV | C | P | W |
Jordan et al., 2016-b | 54 | 47 | Ad | IBG | GR | O | NV | C | P | W |
Jordan et al., 2016-c | 103 | 106 | Ad | IBG | GR | O | NV | C | P | W |
Polman & Ruttan, 2012 | 64 | 64 | Ad | IBG | GR | D | NV | C | P | W |
Solak et al., 2016-a | 30 | 30 | Ad | GBG | MS | O | V | C | P | W |
Solak et al., 2016-b | 34 | 34 | Ad | GBG | MS | O | V | C | P | W |
Solak et al., 2016-b | 34 | 34 | Ad | GBG | MS | D | V | C | P | W |
Zhang et al., 2017 | 59 | 49 | Ad | IBG | MS | D | NV | C | P | E |
表2 元分析中纳入的原始研究(状态内疚对亲社会行为的影响)
作者(发表时间) | 实验组 样本量 | 控制组 样本量 | 年龄 | 内疚 类型 | 内疚诱 发方式 | 亲社会 行为类型 | 亲身会 行为对象 | 控制组 | 出版 与否 | 文化 背景 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
丁芳 等, 2014-a | 60 | 60 | A | IBG | CS | O | V | C | P | E |
丁芳 等, 2014-b | 60 | 60 | A | IBG | CS | O | NV | C | P | E |
杜灵燕, 2012-a | 407 | 360 | A | IBG | MS | H | NV | S | UP | E |
杜灵燕, 2012-b | 35 | 37 | A | IBG | MS | D | NV | C | UP | E |
杜灵燕, 2012-b | 35 | 35 | A | IBG | MS | D | NV | S | UP | E |
刘谞, 2011-b | 48 | 48 | Ad | IBG | CS | R | V | C | UP | E |
孙铭鸿, 2015 | 85 | 80 | A | IBG | MS | D | NV | C | UP | E |
张晓贤 等, 2012-a | 27 | 28 | C | IBG | CS | H | NV | C | P | E |
张晓贤 等, 2012-b | 33 | 28 | C | IBG | CS | H | NV | C | P | E |
de Hooge et al., 2007-a | 42 | 39 | Ad | IBG | GR | O | NV | C | P | W |
de Hooge et al., 2007-a | 42 | 45 | Ad | IBG | GR | O | NV | S | P | W |
de Hooge et al., 2007-b | 44 | 37 | Ad | IBG | GR | H | NV | C | P | W |
de Hooge et al., 2007-b | 44 | 48 | Ad | IBG | GR | H | NV | S | P | W |
作者(发表时间) | 实验组 样本量 | 控制组 样本量 | 年龄 | 内疚 类型 | 内疚诱 发方式 | 亲社会 行为类型 | 亲身会 行为对象 | 控制组 | 出版 与否 | 文化 背景 |
de Hooge et al., 2011-a | 16 | 17 | Ad | IBG | GR | R | V | C | P | W |
de Hooge et al., 2011-a | 16 | 17 | Ad | IBG | GR | D | NV | C | P | W |
de Hooge et al., 2011-b | 58 | 34 | Ad | IBG | GR | R | V | C | P | W |
de Hooge et al., 2011-b | 58 | 34 | Ad | IBG | GR | D | NV | C | P | W |
de Hooge et al., 2011-c | 25 | 24 | Ad | IBG | GR | R | V | C | P | W |
de Hooge et al., 2011-c | 25 | 24 | Ad | IBG | GR | D | NV | C | P | W |
de Hooge et al., 2011-d | 71 | 72 | Ad | IBG | CS | R | V | C | P | W |
de Hooge et al., 2011-d | 71 | 72 | Ad | IBG | CS | D | NV | C | P | W |
de Hooge et al., 2011-e | 22 | 22 | Ad | IBG | CS | R | V | C | P | W |
de Hooge et al., 2011-f | 23 | 23 | Ad | IBG | MS | R | V | C | P | W |
de Hooge et al., 2011-f | 23 | 23 | Ad | IBG | MS | R | V | S | P | W |
Ferguson & Branscombe, 2010 | 19 | 55 | Ad | GBG | MS | E | V | C | P | W |
Furukawa et al., 2016-a | 107 | 107 | Ad | IBG | MS | R | V | C | P | E |
Furukawa et al., 2016-b | 107 | 107 | Ad | IBG | MS | O | NV | C | P | E |
Graton et al., 2016-a | 28 | 28 | Ad | IBG | GR | E | NV | C | P | W |
Graton et al., 2016-a | 28 | 28 | Ad | IBG | GR | E | NV | S | P | W |
Graton et al., 2016-b | 45 | 45 | Ad | IBG | GR | E | NV | C | P | W |
Graton et al., 2016-c | 41 | 41 | Ad | IBG | GR | E | NV | C | P | W |
Jordan et al., 2016-b | 54 | 47 | Ad | IBG | GR | O | NV | C | P | W |
Jordan et al., 2016-c | 103 | 106 | Ad | IBG | GR | O | NV | C | P | W |
Polman & Ruttan, 2012 | 64 | 64 | Ad | IBG | GR | D | NV | C | P | W |
Solak et al., 2016-a | 30 | 30 | Ad | GBG | MS | O | V | C | P | W |
Solak et al., 2016-b | 34 | 34 | Ad | GBG | MS | O | V | C | P | W |
Solak et al., 2016-b | 34 | 34 | Ad | GBG | MS | D | V | C | P | W |
Zhang et al., 2017 | 59 | 49 | Ad | IBG | MS | D | NV | C | P | E |
调节变量 | k | r | LL | UL | QB | p | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
年龄 | 成人 | 44 | 0.34 | 0.26 | 0.42 | 0.79 | 0.672 |
青少年 | 6 | 0.41 | 0.18 | 0.64 | |||
儿童 | 4 | 0.44 | 0.17 | 0.72 | |||
内疚测量方式 | 正式问卷 | 16 | 0.33 | 0.19 | 0.47 | 0.19 | 0.665 |
自编问卷 | 38 | 0.36 | 0.27 | 0.46 | |||
内疚类型 | 个体内疚 | 25 | 0.33 | 0.22 | 0.44 | 0.38 | 0.538 |
群体内疚 | 29 | 0.38 | 0.28 | 0.48 | |||
亲社会行为对象 | 非受害方 | 13 | 0.3 | 0.14 | 0.46 | 0.61 | 0.736 |
受害方 | 33 | 0.38 | 0.28 | 0.47 | |||
未指明对象 | 8 | 0.36 | 0.17 | 0.55 | |||
文化背景 | 西方 | 14 | 0.37 | 0.22 | 0.52 | 0.04 | 0.836 |
东方 | 40 | 0.35 | 0.26 | 0.44 | |||
亲社会行为类型 | 补偿 | 19 | 0.53 | 0.42 | 0.64 | 20.33 | 0.002 |
合作 | 4 | 0.26 | 0 | 0.52 | |||
环保 | 2 | -0.02 | -0.39 | 0.34 | |||
宽恕 | 3 | 0.07 | -0.22 | 0.35 | |||
其它 | 14 | 0.25 | 0.12 | 0.38 | |||
群际支援 | 5 | 0.38 | 0.16 | 0.6 | |||
特质 | 7 | 0.36 | 0.18 | 0.53 |
表3 特质内疚与亲社会行为关系的调节效应检验(随机效应模型)
调节变量 | k | r | LL | UL | QB | p | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
年龄 | 成人 | 44 | 0.34 | 0.26 | 0.42 | 0.79 | 0.672 |
青少年 | 6 | 0.41 | 0.18 | 0.64 | |||
儿童 | 4 | 0.44 | 0.17 | 0.72 | |||
内疚测量方式 | 正式问卷 | 16 | 0.33 | 0.19 | 0.47 | 0.19 | 0.665 |
自编问卷 | 38 | 0.36 | 0.27 | 0.46 | |||
内疚类型 | 个体内疚 | 25 | 0.33 | 0.22 | 0.44 | 0.38 | 0.538 |
群体内疚 | 29 | 0.38 | 0.28 | 0.48 | |||
亲社会行为对象 | 非受害方 | 13 | 0.3 | 0.14 | 0.46 | 0.61 | 0.736 |
受害方 | 33 | 0.38 | 0.28 | 0.47 | |||
未指明对象 | 8 | 0.36 | 0.17 | 0.55 | |||
文化背景 | 西方 | 14 | 0.37 | 0.22 | 0.52 | 0.04 | 0.836 |
东方 | 40 | 0.35 | 0.26 | 0.44 | |||
亲社会行为类型 | 补偿 | 19 | 0.53 | 0.42 | 0.64 | 20.33 | 0.002 |
合作 | 4 | 0.26 | 0 | 0.52 | |||
环保 | 2 | -0.02 | -0.39 | 0.34 | |||
宽恕 | 3 | 0.07 | -0.22 | 0.35 | |||
其它 | 14 | 0.25 | 0.12 | 0.38 | |||
群际支援 | 5 | 0.38 | 0.16 | 0.6 | |||
特质 | 7 | 0.36 | 0.18 | 0.53 |
调节变量 | k | d | LL | UL | QB | p | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
年龄 | 成人 | 30 | 0.2 | 0.05 | 0.35 | 1.54 | 0.461 |
青少年 | 2 | 0.32 | -0.29 | 0.93 | |||
儿童 | 6 | 0.42 | 0.1 | 0.73 | |||
控制组设置 | 羞耻 | 7 | 0.18 | -0.12 | 0.48 | 0.21 | 0.650 |
中性 | 31 | 0.26 | 0.11 | 0.41 | |||
内疚类型 | 个体内疚 | 34 | 0.23 | 0.09 | 0.37 | 0.22 | 0.642 |
群体内疚 | 4 | 0.34 | -0.09 | 0.77 | |||
内疚诱发方式 | 材料启动 | 13 | 0.39 | 0.18 | 0.61 | 3.25 | 0.119 |
回忆范式 | 17 | 0.09 | -0.1 | 0.29 | |||
情境启动 | 8 | 0.3 | 0.02 | 0.57 | |||
亲社会行为对象 | 非受害方 | 24 | 0.12 | -0.03 | 0.28 | 6.72 | 0.009 |
受害方 | 14 | 0.47 | 0.26 | 0.68 | |||
文化背景 | 西方 | 14 | 0.37 | 0.22 | 0.52 | 1.39 | 0.238 |
东方 | 40 | 0.35 | 0.26 | 0.44 | |||
亲社会行为类型 | 补偿 | 9 | 0.56 | 0.28 | 0.84 | 7.15 | 0.128 |
环保 | 5 | 0.22 | -0.15 | 0.59 | |||
捐款 | 10 | 0.11 | -0.15 | 0.36 | |||
其它 | 9 | 0.22 | -0.04 | 0.47 | |||
助人 | 5 | 0.07 | -0.27 | 0.42 |
表4 状态内疚与亲社会行为关系的调节效应检验(随机效应模型)
调节变量 | k | d | LL | UL | QB | p | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
年龄 | 成人 | 30 | 0.2 | 0.05 | 0.35 | 1.54 | 0.461 |
青少年 | 2 | 0.32 | -0.29 | 0.93 | |||
儿童 | 6 | 0.42 | 0.1 | 0.73 | |||
控制组设置 | 羞耻 | 7 | 0.18 | -0.12 | 0.48 | 0.21 | 0.650 |
中性 | 31 | 0.26 | 0.11 | 0.41 | |||
内疚类型 | 个体内疚 | 34 | 0.23 | 0.09 | 0.37 | 0.22 | 0.642 |
群体内疚 | 4 | 0.34 | -0.09 | 0.77 | |||
内疚诱发方式 | 材料启动 | 13 | 0.39 | 0.18 | 0.61 | 3.25 | 0.119 |
回忆范式 | 17 | 0.09 | -0.1 | 0.29 | |||
情境启动 | 8 | 0.3 | 0.02 | 0.57 | |||
亲社会行为对象 | 非受害方 | 24 | 0.12 | -0.03 | 0.28 | 6.72 | 0.009 |
受害方 | 14 | 0.47 | 0.26 | 0.68 | |||
文化背景 | 西方 | 14 | 0.37 | 0.22 | 0.52 | 1.39 | 0.238 |
东方 | 40 | 0.35 | 0.26 | 0.44 | |||
亲社会行为类型 | 补偿 | 9 | 0.56 | 0.28 | 0.84 | 7.15 | 0.128 |
环保 | 5 | 0.22 | -0.15 | 0.59 | |||
捐款 | 10 | 0.11 | -0.15 | 0.36 | |||
其它 | 9 | 0.22 | -0.04 | 0.47 | |||
助人 | 5 | 0.07 | -0.27 | 0.42 |
[1] | 曹萍 . ( 2012). 试论中西方传统文化的差异及其根源. 社科纵横, 27( 11), 128-129. |
[2] | *丁芳 , 周鋆, 胡雨 . ( 2014). 初中生内疚情绪体验的发展及其对公平行为的影响. 心理科学, 37( 5), 1154-1159. |
[3] | *丁菀 . ( 2015). 道德补偿行为的发生机制: 道德情绪和道德认同的不同作用. 硕士学位论文, 浙江师范大学. |
[4] | *杜灵燕 . ( 2012). 内疚与羞耻对道德判断、道德行为影响的差异研究 . 硕士学位论文, 中国地质大学(北京). |
[5] | *方圆 . ( 2017). 大学生内疚感与亲社会倾向关系实证研究. 南阳师范学院学报, 16( 2), 61-64. |
[6] | 胡金生 . ( 2008). 中文版内疚感问卷的信、效度及调查结果. 中国健康心理学, 16( 2), 236-238. |
[7] | *黄晓娇 . ( 2016). 初中生的内疚水平及其与公共物品困境中合作行为的关系. 硕士学位论文, 海师范大学. |
[8] | *康家慧 . ( 2014). 高职院校学生道德判断能力、内疚与亲社会行为的关系研究. 硕士学位论文, 河北师范大学. |
[9] | 冷冰冰, 王香玲, 高贺明, 李富洪 . ( 2015). 内疚的认知和情绪活动及其脑区调控. 心理科学进展, 23( 12), 2064-2071. |
[10] | *李娟 . ( 2008). 犯罪青少年的道德情绪及其与社会行为的关系. 硕士学位论文, 山东师范大学. |
[11] | *李磊 . ( 2012). 外显、内隐利他行为及其预测源研究. 硕士学位论文, 西北师范大学. |
[12] | *刘金梅 . ( 2009). 不同虚拟内疚类型下青少年亲社会行为选择研究. 硕士学位论文, 西北师范大学. |
[13] | *刘谞 . ( 2011). 社会情绪和社会规范对独裁者博弈中公平行为的影响研究. 硕士学位论文, 浙江大学. |
[14] | *吕丽 . ( 2017). 群体内疚与群体羞耻的差异研究. 硕士学位论文, 中国地质大学(北京). |
[15] | *毛静思 . ( 2012). 中学生道德判断能力、内疚与亲社会行为的关系研究. 硕士学位论文, 四川师范大学. |
[16] | 乔建中, 王蓓 . ( 2003). 霍夫曼虚拟内疚理论述评. 心理学探新, 23( 3), 25-28. |
[17] | 石伟, 闫现洋, 刘杰 . ( 2011). 对不公正历史事件的情绪反应——群体内疚. 心理科学进展, 19( 2), 224-232. |
[18] | * 孙铭鸿 . ( 2015). 初中生内疚情绪对其禁止性道德行为和指定性道德行为的影响. 硕士学位论文, 沈阳师范大学. |
[19] | 王洁, 陈健芷, 杨琳, 高爽 . ( 2013). 感觉寻求与网络成瘾关系的元分析. 心理科学进展, 21( 10), 1720-1730. |
[20] | 王小凤, 占友龙, 燕良轼 . ( 2016). 中文版内疚和羞耻倾向量表的信效度检验. 中国临床心理学杂志, 24( 5), 865-868. |
[21] | 吴鹏, 刘华山 . ( 2014). 道德推理与道德行为关系的元分析. 心理学报, 46( 8), 1192-1207. |
[22] | 张琨, 方平, 姜媛, 于悦, 欧阳恒磊 . ( 2014). 道德视野下的内疚. 心理科学进展, 22( 10), 1628-1636. |
[23] | *张晓贤 , 桑标 . ( 2012). 儿童内疚情绪对其亲社会行为的影响. 心理科学, 35( 2), 314-320. |
[24] |
Barr P. , ( 2004). Guilt- and shame-proneness and the grief of perinatal bereavement. Psychology and Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice, 77( 4), 493-510.
doi: 10.1348/1476083042555442 URL |
[25] | Berndsen M. &Mcgarty C. , ( 2010). The impact of magnitude of harm and perceived difficulty of making reparations on group-based guilt and reparation towards victims of historical harm. European Journal of Social Psychology, 40( 3), 500-513. |
[26] | Bolino,M. C., & Grant, A. M . ( 2016). The bright side of being prosocial at work, and the dark side, too: A review and agenda for research on other-oriented motives, behavior, and impact in organizations. The Academy of Management Annals, 10( 1), 599-670. |
[27] | Borenstein M., Hedges L., Higgins J., & Rothstein H. R . ( 2009). Introduction to meta-analysis . West Sussex, UK: Wiley & Sons. |
[28] |
* Brown R. &Cehajic, S.( 2008). Dealing with the past and facing the future: Mediators of the effects of collective guilt and shame in Bosnia and Herzegovina. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38( 4), 669-684.
doi: 10.1002/(ISSN)1099-0992 URL |
[29] |
*Čehajic-Clanćy S., Effron D. A., Halperin E., Liberman V., & Ross L. D . ( 2011). Affirmation, acknowledgment of in-group responsibility, group-based guilt, and support for reparative measures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101( 2), 256-270.
doi: 10.1037/a0023936 URL |
[30] |
Cohen,T. R . ( 2010). Moral emotions and unethical bargaining: The differential effects of empathy and perspective taking in deterring deceitful negotiation. Journal of Business Ethics, 94( 4), 569-579.
doi: 10.1007/s10551-009-0338-z URL |
[31] |
Cohen T. R., Wolf S. T., Panter A. T., & Insko C. A . ( 2011). Introducing the GASP scale: A new measure of guilt and shame proneness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100( 5), 947-966.
doi: 10.1037/a0022641 URL |
[32] |
Cohen T. R., Panter A. T., & Turan N . ( 2012). Guilt proneness and moral character. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21( 5), 355-359.
doi: 10.1177/0963721412454874 URL |
[33] | Doosje B., Branscombe N. R., Spears R ., & Manstead, A. S. R. ( 1998). Guilty by association: When one’s group has a negative history. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75( 4), 872-874. |
[34] | Dovidio J. F., Piliavin J. A., Schroeder D. A., & Penner L . ( 2006). The social psychology of prosocial behavior . Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum Publishers. |
[35] | Dumont, K., &Waldzus, S . ( 2014). Group-based guilt and reparation in the context of social change. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 44( 4), 331-341. |
[36] | Duval, S. &Tweedie, R. ( 2000). Trim and fill: A simple funnel-plot-based method of testing and adjusting for publication bias in meta-analysis. Biometrics, 56( 2), 455-463. |
[37] |
*de Hooge I. E., Nelissen R. M. A., Breugelmans S. M., & Zeelenberg M . ( 2011). What is moral about guilt? Acting “prosocially” at the disadvantage of others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100( 3), 462-473.
doi: 10.1037/a0021459 URL |
[38] |
*de Hooge I. E., Zeelenberg M., & Breugelmans S. M . ( 2007). Moral sentiments and cooperation: Differential influences of shame and guilt. Cognition and Emotion, 21( 5), 1025-1042.
doi: 10.1080/02699930600980874 URL |
[39] |
Egger M., Smith G. D., Schneider M., & Minder C . ( 1997). Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. British Medical Journal, 315( 7109), 629-634.
doi: 10.1136/bmj.315.7109.629 URL |
[40] | Eisenberg N., Fabes R. A., Shepard S. A., Murphy B. C., Jones S., & Guthrie I. K . ( 1998). Contemporaneous and longitudinal prediction of children’s sympathy from dispositional regulation and emotionality. Developmental Psychology, 34( 5), 910-24. |
[41] |
*Ferguson, M. A., &Branscombe, N. R . ( 2010). Collective guilt mediates the effect of beliefs about global warming on willingness to engage in mitigation behavior. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 30( 2), 135-142.
doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2009.11.010 URL |
[42] | *Figueiredo A., Doosje B., & Valentim J. P . ( 2016). Group-based compunction and anger: Their antecedents and consequences in relation to colonial conflicts. International Journal of Conflict & Violence, 9( 1), 90-105. |
[43] |
*Furukawa Y., Nakashima K., & Morinaga Y . ( 2016). Influence of social context on the relationship between guilt and prosocial behaviour. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 19( 1), 49-54.
doi: 10.1111/ajsp.v19.1 URL |
[44] | George J. M., &Brief,A. P . ( 1992). Feeling good-doing good: A conceptual analysis of the mood at work- organizational spontaneity relationship. Psychological Bulletin, 112( 2), 310-329. |
[45] |
Ghorbani M. ( 2013). Guilt, shame, and reparative behavior: The effect of psychological proximity. Journal of Business Ethics, 114( 2), 311-323.
doi: 10.1007/s10551-012-1350-2 URL |
[46] |
*Graton A., Ric F., & Gonzalez E . ( 2016). Reparation or reactance? The influence of guilt on reaction to persuasive communication. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 62, 40-49.
doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2015.09.016 URL |
[47] | Greene J. ( 2003). From neural 'is' to moral 'ought': What are the moral implications of neuroscientific moral psychology?. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4( 10), 846-849. |
[48] |
*Halmburger A., Baumert A., & Schmitt M . ( 2015). Anger as driving factor of moral courage in comparison with guilt and global mood: A multimethod approach. European Journal of Social Psychology, 45( 1), 39-51.
doi: 10.1002/ejsp.2071 URL |
[49] |
*Harth N. S., Leach C. W., & Kessler T . ( 2013). Guilt, anger, and pride about in-group environmental behaviour: Different emotions predict distinct intentions. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 34, 18-26.
doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2012.12.005 URL |
[50] |
*Iyer A., Schmader T., & Lickel B . ( 2007). Why individuals protest the perceived transgressions of their country: The role of anger, shame, and guilt. Pers Soc Psychol Bull, 33( 4), 572-587.
doi: 10.1177/0146167206297402 URL |
[51] |
*Jordan J., Flynn F. J., & Cohen T. R . ( 2016). Correction to ‘forgive them for I have sinned: The relationship between guilt and forgiveness of others' transgressions’. European Journal of Social Psychology, 46( 2), 265-266.
doi: 10.1002/ejsp.v46.2 URL |
[52] |
*Kahn D. T., Liberman V., Halperin E., & Ross L . ( 2016). Intergroup sentiments, political identity, and their influence on responses to potentially ameliorative proposals in the context of an intractable conflict. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 60( 1), 61-88.
doi: 10.1177/0022002714535250 URL |
[53] | Ketelaar T., &Au,W. T . ( 2003). The effects of feelings of guilt on the behaviour of uncooperative individuals in repeated social bargaining games: An affect-as-information interpretation of the role of emotion in social interaction. Cognition and Emotion. 17( 3), 429-453. |
[54] |
Kim S., Thibodeau R., & Jorgensen R. S . ( 2011). Shame, guilt, and depressive symptoms: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 137( 1), 68-96.
doi: 10.1037/a0021466 URL |
[55] | *Konstam V., Chernoff M., & Deveney S . ( 2011). Toward forgiveness: The role of shame, guilt anger, and empathy. Counseling and Values, 46( 1), 26-39. |
[56] | Kubany,E. S., & Watson, S. B . ( 2003). Guilt: Elaboration of a multidimensional model. The Psychological Record, 53( 1), 51-90. |
[57] |
Kugler K. &Jones, W. H . ( 1992). On conceptualizing and assessing guilt. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62( 2), 318-327.
doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.62.2.318 URL |
[58] | Kuppens S., Laurent L., Heyvaert M., & Onghena P . ( 2013). Associations between parental psychological control and relational aggression in children and adolescents: A multilevel and sequential meta-analysis. Developmental Psychology, 49( 9), 1697-1712. |
[59] |
*Leach C. W., Iyer A., & Pedersen A . ( 2006). Anger and guilt about ingroup advantage explain the willingness for political action. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32( 9), 1232-1245.
doi: 10.1177/0146167206289729 URL |
[60] |
*Leonard M. A., Yung S. M., & Cairns E . ( 2015). Predicting intergroup forgiveness from in-group identification and collective guilt in adolescent and adult affiliates of a Northern Irish cross-community organization. Peace and Conflict: Journal of Peace Psychology, 21( 2), 155-167.
doi: 10.1037/pac0000055 URL |
[61] |
*Linda T., Stefan S., & Lugo R. G . ( 2016). Empathy, guilt proneness, and gender: Relative contributions to prosocial behaviour. Europe's Journal of Psychology, 12( 2), 260-270.
doi: 10.5964/ejop.v12i2.1097 URL |
[62] |
Locke G. W., Shilkret R., Everett J. E., & Petry N. M . ( 2013). Interpersonal guilt in college student pathological gamblers. The American Journal of Drug and Alcohol abuse, 39( 1), 28-32.
doi: 10.3109/00952990.2012.694520 URL |
[63] | Martin A., &Olson, K. R . ( 2015). Beyond good and evil: What motivations underlie children’s prosocial behavior? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10(2), 159-175. |
[64] |
*Mashuri A., Zaduqisti E., & Alroy-Thiberge D . ( 2017). The role of dual categorization and relative ingroup prototypicality in reparations to a minority group: An examination of empathy and collective guilt as mediators. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 20( 1), 33-44.
doi: 10.1111/ajsp.v20.1 URL |
[65] |
*Mcgarty C., Pedersen A., Leach C. W., Mansell T., Waller J., & Bliuc A-M . ( 2005). Group‐based guilt as a predictor of commitment to apology. British Journal of Social Psychology, 44( 4), 659-680.
doi: 10.1348/014466604X18974 URL |
[66] | *Ongley S. F., Nola M., & Malti T . ( 2014). Children's giving: Moral reasoning and moral emotions in the development of donation behaviors. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 458-465. |
[67] | Penner L. A., Dovidio J. F., Piliavin J. A., & Schroeder D. A . ( 2005). Prosocial behavior: Multilevel perspectives. Annual Review of Psychology, 56( 1), 365-392. |
[68] |
* Polman E., &Ruttan, R. L . ( 2012). Effects of anger, guilt, and envy on moral hypocrisy. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38( 1), 129-139.
doi: 10.1177/0146167211422365 URL |
[69] |
*Roberts W., Strayer J., & Denham S . ( 2014). Empathy, anger, guilt: Emotions and prosocial behaviour. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 46( 4), 465-474.
doi: 10.1037/a0035057 URL |
[70] |
*Roos S., Hodges E. V., & Salmivalli C . ( 2014). Do guilt- and shame-proneness differentially predict prosocial, aggressive, and withdrawn behaviors during early adolescence? Developmental Psychology, 50( 3), 941-946.
doi: 10.1037/a0033904 URL |
[71] |
* Rotella K. N., &Richeson,J. A . ( 2013). Body of guilt: Using embodied cognition to mitigate backlash to reminders of personal & ingroup wrongdoing. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49( 4), 643-650.
doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2013.02.013 URL |
[72] | Rothstein H. R., Sutton A. J., & Borenstein M . ( 2006). Publication bias in meta-analysis: Prevention, assessment and adjustments. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. |
[73] |
Schaumberg,R. L., &Flynn,F. J . ( 2012). Uneasy lies the head that wears the crown: The link between guilt- proneness and leadership. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103( 2), 327-342.
doi: 10.1037/a0028127 URL |
[74] |
*Shepherd L., Spears R ., & Manstead, A. S. R .( 2013). ‘This will bring shame on our nation’: The role of anticipated group-based emotions on collective action. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49( 1), 42-57.
doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2012.07.011 URL |
[75] |
Simonsohn U., Nelson L. D., & Simmons J. P . ( 2014). P-curve and effect size: Correcting for publication bias using only significant results. Perspectives on Psychological Science A Journal of the Association for Psychological Science, 9( 6), 666-681.
doi: 10.1177/1745691614553988 URL |
[76] | *Solak N., Tagar M. R., Cohen-Chen S., Saguy T., & Halperin E . ( 2016). Disappointment expression evokes collective guilt and collective action in intergroup conflict: The moderating role of legitimacy perceptions. Cognition and Emotion, 31( 6), 1112-6. |
[77] |
* Strelan P. , ( 2007). Who forgives others, themselves, and situations? The roles of narcissism, guilt, self-esteem, and agreeableness. Personality and Individual Differences, 42( 2), 259-269.
doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2006.06.017 URL |
[78] |
* Swim,J. K J. K., & Miller, D. L . ( 1999). White guilt: Its antecedents and consequences for attitudes toward affirmative action. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25( 4), 500-514.
doi: 10.1177/0146167299025004008 URL |
[79] |
Tangney,J. P . ( 1990). Assessing individual differences in proneness to shame and guilt: Development of the self-conscious affect and attribution inventory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59( 1), 102-111.
doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.59.1.102 URL |
[80] | Tangney J. P. &Dearing R. L. ,( 2002) . Shame and guilt. New York, NY,USA: Guilford Press. |
[81] | Tangney J. P., Youman K. & Stuewig J. , ( 2009) . Proneness to shame and proneness to guilt. In M. R. Leary & R. H. Hoyle (Eds.), Handbook of individual differences in social behavior (pp. 192-209). New York, NY, USA: The Guilford Press. |
[82] | Tignor, S. M., &Colvin,C. R . ( 2016). The interpersonal adaptiveness of dispositional guilt and shame: A meta-analytic investigation. Journal of Personality, 85( 3), 341-363. |
[83] | Turner,J. H., &Stets,J. E . ( 2006). Moral emotions. In J. E. Stets & J. H. Turner (Eds.), Handbook of the sociology of emotions. ( pp.545-566). New York: Springer. |
[84] | Viechtbauer W. , ( 2010). Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package. Journal of Statistical Software, 36(3). |
[85] | *Wan D., Xie R. B., Sun B. H., Li W. J., Wang D., & Rui Z . ( 2016). Why does the “sinner” act prosocially? The mediating role of guilt and the moderating role of moral identity in motivating moral cleansing. Frontiers in Psychology, 7( 1878), 1317-1324. |
[86] | *Wohl M. J. A., Matheson K., Branscombe N. R., & Anisman H . ( 2013). Victim and perpetrator groups' responses to the Canadian government's apology for the head tax on Chinese immigrants and the moderating influence of collective guilt. Political Psychology, 34( 5), 713-729. |
[87] |
*Zhang H., Chen S., Wang R., Jiang J., Xu Y., & Zhao H . ( 2017). How upward moral comparison influences prosocial behavioral intention: Examining the mediating role of guilt and the moderating role of moral identity. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1554-1563.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01554 URL |
[88] |
*Zimmermann A., Abrams D., Doosje B & Manstead A. S. R. ., ( 2011). Causal and moral responsibility: Antecedents and consequences of group-based guilt. European Journal of Social Psychology, 41( 7), 825-839.
doi: 10.1002/ejsp.826 URL |
[1] | 李亚丹, 杜颖, 谢聪, 刘春宇, 杨毅隆, 李阳萍, 邱江. 语义距离与创造性思维关系的元分析[J]. 心理科学进展, 2023, 31(4): 519-534. |
[2] | 曾润喜, 李游. 自我效能感与网络健康信息搜寻关系的元分析[J]. 心理科学进展, 2023, 31(4): 535-551. |
[3] | 吴佳桧, 傅海伦, 张玉环. 感知社会支持与学生学业成就关系的元分析:学习投入的中介作用[J]. 心理科学进展, 2023, 31(4): 552-569. |
[4] | 郭英, 田鑫, 胡东, 白书琳, 周蜀溪. 羞愧对亲社会行为影响的三水平元分析[J]. 心理科学进展, 2023, 31(3): 371-385. |
[5] | 陈必忠, 孙晓军. 中国内地大学生时间管理倾向的时代变迁:1999~2020[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(9): 1968-1980. |
[6] | 杜宇飞, 欧阳辉月, 余林. 隔代抚养与老年人抑郁水平:一项基于东西方文化背景的元分析[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(9): 1981-1992. |
[7] | 方杰, 温忠麟, 欧阳劲樱, 蔡保贞. 国内调节效应的方法学研究[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(8): 1703-1714. |
[8] | 郝娜, 崔丽莹. 补偿他人还是保护自己?内疚与羞耻情绪对合作行为的影响差异[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(7): 1626-1636. |
[9] | 赵宁, 刘鑫, 李纾, 郑蕊. 默认选项设置的助推效果:来自元分析的证据[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(6): 1230-1241. |
[10] | 黄潇潇, 张亚利, 俞国良. 2010~2020中国内地小学生心理健康问题检出率的元分析[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(5): 953-964. |
[11] | 张亚利, 靳娟娟, 俞国良. 2010~2020中国内地初中生心理健康问题检出率的元分析[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(5): 965-977. |
[12] | 于晓琪, 张亚利, 俞国良. 2010~2020中国内地高中生心理健康问题检出率的元分析[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(5): 978-990. |
[13] | 陈雨濛, 张亚利, 俞国良. 2010~2020中国内地大学生心理健康问题检出率的元分析[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(5): 991-1004. |
[14] | 方杰, 温忠麟. 基于两水平回归模型的调节效应分析及其效应量[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(5): 1183-1190. |
[15] | 齐玥, 秦邵天, 王可昕, 陈文锋. 面孔可信度评价调节:经验迁移假说的提出与验证[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(4): 715-722. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||