Advances in Psychological Science ›› 2022, Vol. 30 ›› Issue (11): 2487-2496.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2022.02487
• Regular Articles • Previous Articles Next Articles
MA Yanan, HUANG Yanli(), SHI Yujing, XIE Jiushu()
Received:
2021-10-18
Online:
2022-11-15
Published:
2022-11-09
Contact:
HUANG Yanli,XIE Jiushu
E-mail:yhuangpsy@163.com;jiusxie@outlook.com
CLC Number:
MA Yanan, HUANG Yanli, SHI Yujing, XIE Jiushu. The mechanism of sound symbolism: Innate and acquired interaction model based on the sensitive period[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(11): 2487-2496.
[1] | 谢久书, 沈光银, 黄艳利. (印刷中). 双语者跨语言正字法迁移的争议与整合. 心理科学. |
[2] |
Asano, M., Imai, M., Kita, S., Kitajo, K., Okada, H., & Thierry, G. (2015). Sound symbolism scaffolds language development in preverbal infants. Cortex, 63, 196-205. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2014.08.025
doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2014.08.025 URL pmid: 25282057 |
[3] |
Blasi, D. E., Wichmann, S., Hammarström, H., Stadler, P. F., & Christiansen, M. H. (2016). Sound-meaning association biases evidenced across thousands of languages. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113(39), 10818-10823. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1605782113
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1605782113 URL |
[4] |
Bottini, R., Barilari, M., & Collignon, O. (2019). Sound symbolism in sighted and blind. The role of vision and orthography in sound-shape correspondences. Cognition, 185, 62-70. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2019.01.006
doi: S0010-0277(19)30006-X URL pmid: 30660923 |
[5] |
Brand, J., Monaghan, P., & Walker, P. (2018). The changing role of sound-symbolism for small versus large vocabularies. Cognitive Science, 42(2), 578-590. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12565
doi: 10.1111/cogs.12565 URL |
[6] |
Chow, H. M., & Ciaramitaro, V. (2019). What makes a shape "baba"? The shape features prioritized in sound-shape correspondence change with development. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 179, 73-89. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2018.10.005
doi: S0022-0965(18)30139-5 URL pmid: 30476696 |
[7] |
Cuskley, C., Simner, J., & Kirby, S. (2017). Phonological and orthographic influences in the Bouba-Kiki effect. Psychological Research, 81(1), 119-130. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-015-0709-2
doi: 10.1007/s00426-015-0709-2 URL pmid: 26403463 |
[8] |
D'Anselmo, A., Prete, G., Zdybek, P., Tommasi, L., & Brancucci, A.(2019). Guessing meaning from word sounds of unfamiliar languages: A cross-cultural sound symbolism study. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 593. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00593
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00593 URL pmid: 30941080 |
[9] | Delgado, J., Pereira, R., Ferreira, M. F., Farinha-Fernandes, A., Guerreiro, J. C., Faustino, B., Domingues, M., Ventura, P. (2020). Sound symbolism is modulated by linguistic experience. Revista da Associação Portuguesa de Linguística, 7, 137-150. https://doi.org/10.26334/2183-9077/rapln7ano2020a9 |
[10] |
Dingemanse, M., Blasi, D. E., Lupyan, G., Christiansen, M. H., & Monaghan, P. (2015). Arbitrariness, iconicity and systematicity in language. Trends Cognitive Science, 19(10), 603-615. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2015.07.013
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.07.013 URL |
[11] | Ernst, M. O. (2007). Learning to integrate arbitrary signals from vision and touch. Journal of Vision, 7(5), 7-14. https://doi.org/10.1167/7.5.7 %J Journal of Vision |
[12] | Fort, M., Lammertink, I., Peperkamp, S., Guevara-Rukoz, A., Fikkert, P., & Tsuji, S. (2018). Symbouki: A meta-analysis on the emergence of sound symbolism in early language acquisition. Developmental Science, 21(5), e12659. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12659 |
[13] | Fort, M., Weiss, A., Martin, A., & Peperkamp, S. (2013). Looking for the bouba-kiki effect in prelexical infants. In S. Ouni, F. Berthomier, & A. Jesse (Eds.),The 12th International Conference on Auditory-Visual Speech Processing (pp. 71-76). INRIA. |
[14] |
Fryer, L., Freeman, J., & Pring, L. (2014). Touching words is not enough: How visual experience influences haptic- auditory associations in the "Bouba-Kiki" effect. Cognition, 132(2), 164-173. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2014.03.01
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.03.015 URL |
[15] |
Gabard-Durnam, L., & McLaughlin, K. A. (2020). Sensitive periods in human development: Charting a course for the future. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 36, 120-128. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2020.09.003
doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2020.09.003 URL |
[16] |
Gold, R., & Segal, O. (2020). The Bouba-Kiki effect in persons with prelingual auditory deprivation. Language Learning and Development, 16(1), 49-60. https://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2019.1685386
doi: 10.1080/15475441.2019.1685386 URL |
[17] |
Graven, T., & Desebrock, C. (2018). Bouba or kiki with and without vision: Shape-audio regularities and mental images. Acta Psychologica, 188, 200-212. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2018.05.011
doi: S0001-6918(17)30517-6 URL pmid: 29982038 |
[18] |
Hamilton-Fletcher, G., Pisanski, K., Reby, D., Stefanczyk, M., Ward, J., & Sorokowska, A. (2018). The role of visual experience in the emergence of cross-modal correspondences. Cognition, 175, 114-121. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2018.02.023
doi: S0010-0277(18)30059-3 URL pmid: 29502009 |
[19] | Hensch, T. K. (2018). Chapter 6-Critical periods in cortical development. In R.Gibb & B.Kolb (Eds.), The Neurobiology of Brain and Behavioral Development (pp. 133-151). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-804036-2.00006-6 |
[20] | Imai, M., & Kita, S. (2014). The sound symbolism bootstrapping hypothesis for language acquisition and language evolution. Philosophical Transactions of The Royal Society B-biological Sciences, 369(1651), 20130298. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2013.0298 |
[21] | Imai, M., Miyazaki, M., Yeung, H. H., Hidaka, S., Kantartzis, K., Okada, H., & Kita, S. (2015). Sound symbolism facilitates word learning in 14-month-olds. Plos One, 10(2), e0116494. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0116494 |
[22] | Jo, J., & Ko, E. S. (2018). Korean mothers attune the frequency and acoustic saliency of sound symbolic words to the linguistic maturity of their children. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 2225. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02225 |
[23] |
Kantartzis, K., Imai, M., Evans, D., & Kita, S. (2019). Sound symbolism facilitates long-term retention of the semantic representation of novel verbs in three-year-olds. Languages, 4(2), 21. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages4020021
doi: 10.3390/languages4020021 URL |
[24] | Kantartzis, K. (2011). Children and adults' understanding and use of sound-symbolism in novel words (Unpublished doctorial dissertation). University of Birmingham. |
[25] | Köhler, W. (1929). Gestalt psychology: An introduction to new concepts in modern psychology. New York: Liveright. |
[26] |
Kuhl, P. K. (2010). Brain mechanisms in early language acquisition. Neuron, 67(5), 713-727.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2010.08.038
doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.08.038 URL pmid: 20826304 |
[27] |
Laing, C. E. (2017). A perceptual advantage for onomatopoeia in early word learning: Evidence from eye-tracking. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 161, 32-45. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.03.017
doi: S0022-0965(16)30246-6 URL pmid: 28460299 |
[28] | Lewis, M. P., Simons, G. F., & Fennig, C. D. (2015). Ethnologue: Languages of Africa and Europe (18th ed.). Dallas, TX: SIL International. |
[29] |
Lewkowicz, D. J., & Ghazanfar, A. A. (2009). The emergence of multisensory systems through perceptual narrowing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(11), 470-478. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.08.004
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.08.004 URL pmid: 19748305 |
[30] |
Ludwig, V. U., Adachi, I., & Matsuzawa, T. (2011). Visuoauditory mappings between high luminance and high pitch are shared by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 108(51), 20661-20665.https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1112605108
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1112605108 URL |
[31] | Margiotoudi, K., Allritz, M., Bohn, M., & Pulvermuller, F. (2019). Sound symbolic congruency detection in humans but not in great apes. Scientific Report, 9, 12705. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-49101-4 |
[32] |
Mayberry, R. I., & Kluender, R. (2018). Rethinking the critical period for language: New insights into an old question from American Sign Language. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 21(5), 886-905. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728917000724
doi: 10.1017/S1366728917000724 URL |
[33] | Monaghan, P., Shillcock, R. C., Christiansen, M. H., & Kirby, S. (2014). How arbitrary is language? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences, 369(1651), 20130299. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2013.0299 |
[34] |
Nielsen, A. K. S., & Dingemanse, M. (2020). Iconicity in word learning and beyond: A critical review. Language and Speech, 64(1), 52-72. https://doi.org/10.1177/0023830920914339
doi: 10.1177/0023830920914339 URL |
[35] |
Ortiz-Mantilla, S., Realpe-Bonilla, T., & Benasich, A. A. (2019). Early interactive acoustic experience with non-speech generalizes to speech and confers a syllabic processing advantage at 9 months. Cerebral Cortex, 29(4), 1789-1801. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhz001
doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhz001 URL |
[36] |
Owren, M. J., & Rendall, D. (2001). Sound on the rebound: Bringing form and function back to the forefront in understanding nonhuman primate vocal signaling. Evolutionary Anthropology, 10(2), 58-71. https://doi.org/10.1002/evan.1014
doi: 10.1002/evan.1014 URL |
[37] |
Ozturk, O., Krehm, M., & Vouloumanos, A. (2013). Sound symbolism in infancy: Evidence for sound-shape cross-modal correspondences in 4-month-olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114(2), 173-186. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.05.004
doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.05.004 URL pmid: 22960203 |
[38] |
Pant, R., Kanjlia, S., & Bedny, M. (2020). A sensitive period in the neural phenotype of language in blind individuals. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 41, 100744. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2019.100744
doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2019.100744 URL |
[39] |
Parise, C. V. (2016). Crossmodal correspondences: Standing issues and experimental guidelines. Multisensory Research, 29(1-3), 7-28. https://doi.org/10.1163/22134808-00002502
URL pmid: 27311289 |
[40] |
Park, J., Motoki, K., Pathak, A., & Spence, C. (2021). A sound brand name: The role of voiced consonants in pharmaceutical branding. Food Quality and Preference, 90, 104104. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foodqual.2020.104104
doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2020.104104 URL |
[41] |
Pathak, A., & Calvert, G. A. (2020). Sounds sweet, sounds bitter: How the presence of certain sounds in a brand name can alter expectations about the product’s taste. Food Quality and Preference, 83, 103918. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foodqual.2020.103918
doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2020.103918 URL |
[42] |
Pejovic, J., & Molnar, M. (2017). The development of spontaneous sound-shape matching in monolingual and bilingual infants during the first year. Developmental Psychology, 53(3), 581-586. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000237
doi: 10.1037/dev0000237 URL pmid: 27854461 |
[43] | Perry, L. K., Perlman, M., & Lupyan, G. (2015). Iconicity in english and spanish and its relation to lexical category and age of acquisition. Plos One, 10(9), e0137147. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0137147 |
[44] | Perry, L. K., Perlman, M., Winter, B., Massaro, D. W., & Lupyan, G. (2018). Iconicity in the speech of children and adults. Developmental Science, 21(3), e12572. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12572 |
[45] | Piaget, J., & Duckworth, E. (1970). Genetic epistemology. American Behavioral Scientist, 13(3), 459-480. https://doi.org/10.1177/000276427001300320 |
[46] |
Preziosi, M. A., & Coane, J. H. (2017). Remembering that big things sound big: Sound symbolism and associative memory. Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications, 2, 10. https://doi.org/10.1186/s41235-016-0047-y
doi: 10.1186/s41235-016-0047-y URL |
[47] | Ramachandran, V. S., & Hubbard, E. M. (2001). Synaesthesia -- A window into perception, thought and language. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 8(12), 3-34. |
[48] |
Reinking, D., Labbo, L., & McKenna, M. (2000). From assimilation to accommodation: A developmental framework for integrating digital technologies into literacy research and instruction. Journal of Research in Reading, 23(2), 110-122. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.00108
doi: 10.1111/1467-9817.00108 URL |
[49] | Rendall, D., & Owren, M. J. (2010). Chapter 5.4-Vocalizations as tools for influencing the affect and behavior of others. In S. M. Brudzynski (Ed.), Handbook of Behavioral Neuroscience (Vol. 19, pp. 177-185). Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-374593-4.00018-8 |
[50] |
Rogers, S. K., & Ross, A. S. (1975). A cross-cultural test of the Maluma-Takete phenomenon. Perception, 4(1), 105-106. https://doi.org/10.1068/p040105
URL pmid: 1161435 |
[51] | Saussure, F. d. (1959). Course in general linguistics. New York: The Philosophical Library. |
[52] | Sedley, D. N. (2003). Plato’s Cratylus. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. |
[53] | Shang, N., & Styles, S. J. (2017). Is a high tone pointy? Speakers of different languages match mandarin chinese tones to visual shapes differently. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 2139. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02139 |
[54] |
Sidhu, D. M., & Pexman, P. M. (2018). Five mechanisms of sound symbolic association. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25(5), 1619-1643. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-017-1361-1
doi: 10.3758/s13423-017-1361-1 URL |
[55] |
Sonier, R. P., Poirier, M., Guitard, D., & Saint-Aubin, J. (2020). A round Bouba is easier to remember than a curved Kiki: Sound-symbolism can support associative memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 27(4), 776-782. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-020-01733-8
doi: 10.3758/s13423-020-01733-8 URL |
[56] |
Sourav, S., Kekunnaya, R., Shareef, I., Banerjee, S., Bottari, D., & Röder, B. (2019). A protracted sensitive period regulates the development of cross-modal sound-shape associations in humans. Psychological Science, 30(10), 1473-1482. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797619866625
doi: 10.1177/0956797619866625 URL pmid: 31483197 |
[57] |
Spector, F., & Maurer, D. (2013). Synesthesia: A new approach to understanding the development of perception. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice, 1(S),108-129. https://doi.org/10.1037/2326-5523.1.S.108
doi: 10.1037/2326-5523.1.S.108 URL |
[58] | Styles, S. J., & Gawne, L. (2017). When does Maluma/Takete fail? Two key failures and a meta-analysis suggest that phonology and phonotactics matter. Iperception, 8(4). https://doi.org/10.1177/2041669517724807 |
[59] |
Tzeng, C. Y., Nygaard, L. C., & Namy, L. L. (2017). Developmental change in children's sensitivity to sound symbolism. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 160, 107-118. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.03.004
doi: S0022-0965(17)30173-X URL pmid: 28433821 |
[60] |
Werker, J. F., & Hensch, T. K. (2015). Critical periods in speech perception: New directions. Annual Review of Psychology, 66(1), 173-196. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015104
doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015104 URL |
[61] |
Yang, J., Asano, M., Kanazawa, S., Yamaguchi, M. K., & Imai, M. (2019). Sound symbolism processing is lateralized to the right temporal region in the prelinguistic infant brain. Science Report, 9(1), 13435. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-49917-0
doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-49917-0 URL |
[62] |
Zangenehpour, S., & Zatorre, R. J. (2010). Crossmodal recruitment of primary visual cortex following brief exposure to bimodal audiovisual stimuli. Neuropsychologia, 48(2), 591-600. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.10.022.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.10.022 URL pmid: 19883668 |
[1] | YE Shuqi, YIN Junting, LI Zhaoxian, LUO Junlong. The influence mechanism of emotion on intuitive and analytical processing [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2023, 31(5): 736-746. |
[2] | LI Yadan, DU Ying, XIE Cong, LIU Chunyu, YANG Yilong, LI Yangping, QIU Jiang. A meta-analysis of the relationship between semantic distance and creative thinking [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2023, 31(4): 519-534. |
[3] | YU Jie, CHEN Youguo. Spatiotemporal interference effect: An explanation based on Bayesian models [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2023, 31(4): 597-607. |
[4] | WANG Yongli, GE Shengnan, Lancy Lantin Huang, WAN Qin, LU Haidan. Neural mechanism of speech imagery [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2023, 31(4): 608-621. |
[5] | YANG Qing, LI Yaqin. Is uncertainty bad? Mixed findings and explanatory model of error processing under uncertainty [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2023, 31(3): 338-349. |
[6] | WANG Xudong, HE Yaji, FAN Huiyong, LUO Yangmei, CHEN Xuhai. The advantages and disadvantages of interpersonal anger: Evidence from meta-analysis [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2023, 31(3): 386-401. |
[7] | LI Qingyang, YIN Junting, LUO Junlong. Legs move, thoughts flow: Physical exercise influences creative thinking [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2023, 31(3): 455-466. |
[8] | CHEN Zi-Wei, FU Di, LIU Xun. Better to misidentify than to miss: A review of occurrence mechanisms and applications of face pareidolia [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2023, 31(2): 240-255. |
[9] | WANG Songxue, CHENG Si, JIANG Ting, LIU Xun, ZHANG Mingxia. The effect of external rewards on declarative memory [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2023, 31(1): 78-86. |
[10] | XIE Caifeng, WU Jiahua, XU Liying, YU Feng, ZHAND Yuyan, XIE Yingying. The process motivation model of algorithmic decision-making approach and avoidance [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2023, 31(1): 60-77. |
[11] | YE Weihao, YU Meiqi, ZHANG Lihui, GAO Qi, FU Mingzhu, LU Jiamei. Negative emotion granularity: Its mechanisms and related interventions [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 0, (): 0-0. |
[12] | ZHU Chuanlin, LIU Dianzhi, LUO Wenbo. The cognitive and brain mechanisms of how emotional experience affecting individuals’ utilization of estimation strategies [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(12): 2639-2649. |
[13] | SHI Hanwen, LI Yutong, SUI Xue. Effects of emotional word types: behavioral and neural evidence for discrimination between emotion-label and emotion-laden words [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(12): 2696-2707. |
[14] | CHEN Yutian, CHEN Rui, LI Peng. The development of concept and theoretical models of “chunking” in working memory [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(12): 2708-2717. |
[15] | SHI Huiying, TANG Jie, LIU Pingping. Instability of the watching eyes effect and perceived norms: A new perspective [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(12): 2718-2734. |
Viewed | ||||||
Full text |
|
|||||
Abstract |
|
|||||