Advances in Psychological Science ›› 2023, Vol. 31 ›› Issue (12): 2368-2379.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2023.02368
• Regular Articles • Previous Articles Next Articles
SHAO Hongtao1, REN Guiqin1(), DING Xiaoqian1, SHI Mengmeng2, LI Ruiyan1, LI Yang1
Received:
2023-04-17
Online:
2023-12-15
Published:
2023-09-11
CLC Number:
SHAO Hongtao, REN Guiqin, DING Xiaoqian, SHI Mengmeng, LI Ruiyan, LI Yang. The influence of mindfulness meditation on mind wandering and its mechanism[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2023, 31(12): 2368-2379.
研究者 | 干预形式 | 被试 | 主要测量方式 | 走神指标变化 |
---|---|---|---|---|
Mrazek et al., | 8分钟 正念呼吸 | 60名本科生随机分为实验组、新闻阅读组和自由放松组 | SART任务 | 与主动控制组和放松组相比, 实验组的SART错误率和时间变异性显著更低。 |
Mrazek et al., | 2周的正念 减压训练 | 48名本科生随机分为实验组(26人)和营养组(22人) | OSPAN任务, 探针检测和自我检测 | 与营养组或正念减压训练前相比, 正念冥想训练减少了GRE考试期间走神的发生频率 |
Giannandrea et al., | 8周正念 减压训练 | 60名正念冥想爱好者随机分为实验组(30人)和控制组(30人) | SART任务, 探针检测和自我检测 | 探针检测和自我检测的走神频率在组间和干预前后均无显著差异。而与干预前相比, 正念减压训练后的个体在SART任务上错误率指标(对目标刺激作反应)显著降低 |
Somaraju et al., 2021 | 15分钟 身体扫描 | 55名成年人随机分为正念冥想组(21人)、肌肉放松组(15人)和TED演讲组(19人) | SART任务和走神问卷 | 无显著改善 |
Rahl et al., | 4天注意监控和接纳正念冥想 | 145名成年参与者随机分配到四组, 分别是仅注意监控正念组(41人), 注意监控加接纳正念组(41人), 放松组(38人)和控制组(22人) | SART任务 | 与其他三组相比, 注意监控和接纳的正念冥想组表现出最少的错误率 |
Ueberholz & Fiocco, | 10分钟聚焦 注意冥想 | 220名成年参与者随机分配到正念冥想组(78人)、启动加正念冥想组(63人)以及启动组(79人) | SART任务 | 与启动组相比, 启动加正念冥想组和正念冥想组表现出更少的压力感知以及更少的遗漏错误。然而, 相对于启动组, 启动加正念冥想组在SART任务上表现出更高的正确率和更少的错误(对目标刺激不作反应)率。反应时的变异性在三组之间没有显著差异。 |
Sanger & Dorjee, | 学校正念 训练课程 | 40名青少年学生完成前后测 | ERP, Oddball范式 | 干预后被试对不相关的频繁刺激和颜色偏差的非目标刺激的反应中, N2的波幅显著增加。这表明, 正念冥想训练使青少年增强了抑制无关刺激的能力, 更加专注。 |
Whitmoyer et al., | 4周基于注意的正念干预训练(与正念减压训练相比, 更侧重聚焦注意冥想) | 74名平均年龄约66岁老年人随机分配到正念冥想训练组(37人)和生活方式教育组(37人) | GO-NOGO任务的变式, 探针检测 | 与生活方式教育组相比, 正念冥想组老年人反应时变异性和任务无关想法显著减少, 任务相关的想法并没有显著变化。此外, 正念冥想训练对老年人与任务相关的想法并没有显著减少。 |
Brandmeyer & Delorme, | 1年以上正念冥想规律练习 | 24名参与者 | EEG, 听觉探针 | 与走神时相比, 专家在正念冥想时额叶皮层的θ波和躯体感觉皮层的α波能量的增强; 非专业组则并没有发现两者的差异。 |
Rodriguez-Larios et al., | 3年以上正念冥想经验 | 58名参与者, 专家组(29人), 控制组(即无经验者29人) | EEG, 探针检测 | 与走神时相比, 新手组在正念冥想期间α波波幅下降, 正念冥想组则没有显著差异。 |
Liu et al., | 21天训练包括正念呼吸、身体扫描、正念行走和正念饮食 | 45名大学生随机分配到正念冥想组(21人)和对照组(24人) | ERP, SART任务 | 相对于前测, 正念冥想组在后测中的N2成分波幅幅度显著变大。N2成分波幅越大意味着更多的意识专注于当前任务。N2的增强也表明注意力监控以及反应抑制等。与前测和对照组相比, 正念冥想后个体的P3波幅幅度明显更大。P3波幅增加表明了个体在正念冥想后注意维持增强。 |
Brewer et al., | 10年以上正念冥想经验 | 12名经验丰富专家组, 13名健康成年人为对照组 | fMRI | 在正念冥想条件下, 专家组的PCC活跃度低于对照组; 在聚焦注意冥想期间, 专家组的后扣带皮层和左角回活跃程度低于对照组; 在开放监控冥想期间, 专家组的上颞回和内侧颞回的活跃程度低于对照组。此外, 功能连接分析显示, 专家组在正念冥想时, 后扣带、背前扣带和背外侧前额叶皮质之间的耦合更强。 |
研究者 | 干预形式 | 被试 | 主要测量方式 | 走神指标变化 |
---|---|---|---|---|
Mrazek et al., | 8分钟 正念呼吸 | 60名本科生随机分为实验组、新闻阅读组和自由放松组 | SART任务 | 与主动控制组和放松组相比, 实验组的SART错误率和时间变异性显著更低。 |
Mrazek et al., | 2周的正念 减压训练 | 48名本科生随机分为实验组(26人)和营养组(22人) | OSPAN任务, 探针检测和自我检测 | 与营养组或正念减压训练前相比, 正念冥想训练减少了GRE考试期间走神的发生频率 |
Giannandrea et al., | 8周正念 减压训练 | 60名正念冥想爱好者随机分为实验组(30人)和控制组(30人) | SART任务, 探针检测和自我检测 | 探针检测和自我检测的走神频率在组间和干预前后均无显著差异。而与干预前相比, 正念减压训练后的个体在SART任务上错误率指标(对目标刺激作反应)显著降低 |
Somaraju et al., 2021 | 15分钟 身体扫描 | 55名成年人随机分为正念冥想组(21人)、肌肉放松组(15人)和TED演讲组(19人) | SART任务和走神问卷 | 无显著改善 |
Rahl et al., | 4天注意监控和接纳正念冥想 | 145名成年参与者随机分配到四组, 分别是仅注意监控正念组(41人), 注意监控加接纳正念组(41人), 放松组(38人)和控制组(22人) | SART任务 | 与其他三组相比, 注意监控和接纳的正念冥想组表现出最少的错误率 |
Ueberholz & Fiocco, | 10分钟聚焦 注意冥想 | 220名成年参与者随机分配到正念冥想组(78人)、启动加正念冥想组(63人)以及启动组(79人) | SART任务 | 与启动组相比, 启动加正念冥想组和正念冥想组表现出更少的压力感知以及更少的遗漏错误。然而, 相对于启动组, 启动加正念冥想组在SART任务上表现出更高的正确率和更少的错误(对目标刺激不作反应)率。反应时的变异性在三组之间没有显著差异。 |
Sanger & Dorjee, | 学校正念 训练课程 | 40名青少年学生完成前后测 | ERP, Oddball范式 | 干预后被试对不相关的频繁刺激和颜色偏差的非目标刺激的反应中, N2的波幅显著增加。这表明, 正念冥想训练使青少年增强了抑制无关刺激的能力, 更加专注。 |
Whitmoyer et al., | 4周基于注意的正念干预训练(与正念减压训练相比, 更侧重聚焦注意冥想) | 74名平均年龄约66岁老年人随机分配到正念冥想训练组(37人)和生活方式教育组(37人) | GO-NOGO任务的变式, 探针检测 | 与生活方式教育组相比, 正念冥想组老年人反应时变异性和任务无关想法显著减少, 任务相关的想法并没有显著变化。此外, 正念冥想训练对老年人与任务相关的想法并没有显著减少。 |
Brandmeyer & Delorme, | 1年以上正念冥想规律练习 | 24名参与者 | EEG, 听觉探针 | 与走神时相比, 专家在正念冥想时额叶皮层的θ波和躯体感觉皮层的α波能量的增强; 非专业组则并没有发现两者的差异。 |
Rodriguez-Larios et al., | 3年以上正念冥想经验 | 58名参与者, 专家组(29人), 控制组(即无经验者29人) | EEG, 探针检测 | 与走神时相比, 新手组在正念冥想期间α波波幅下降, 正念冥想组则没有显著差异。 |
Liu et al., | 21天训练包括正念呼吸、身体扫描、正念行走和正念饮食 | 45名大学生随机分配到正念冥想组(21人)和对照组(24人) | ERP, SART任务 | 相对于前测, 正念冥想组在后测中的N2成分波幅幅度显著变大。N2成分波幅越大意味着更多的意识专注于当前任务。N2的增强也表明注意力监控以及反应抑制等。与前测和对照组相比, 正念冥想后个体的P3波幅幅度明显更大。P3波幅增加表明了个体在正念冥想后注意维持增强。 |
Brewer et al., | 10年以上正念冥想经验 | 12名经验丰富专家组, 13名健康成年人为对照组 | fMRI | 在正念冥想条件下, 专家组的PCC活跃度低于对照组; 在聚焦注意冥想期间, 专家组的后扣带皮层和左角回活跃程度低于对照组; 在开放监控冥想期间, 专家组的上颞回和内侧颞回的活跃程度低于对照组。此外, 功能连接分析显示, 专家组在正念冥想时, 后扣带、背前扣带和背外侧前额叶皮质之间的耦合更强。 |
[1] |
程凯, 曹贵康. (2014). 走神的理论假设、影响因素及其神经机制. 心理科学进展, 22(9), 1435-1445.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2014.01435 |
[2] | 刘永. (2017). 思维漫游及正念冥想干预的生理机制研究 (硕士学位论文). 西南大学, 重庆. |
[3] | 宋晓兰, 王晓, 唐孝威. (2011). 心智游移: 现象、机制及意义. 心理科学进展, 19(4), 499-509. |
[4] |
王鑫麟, 邱晓悦, 翁旭初, 杨平. (2022). 工作记忆的神经振荡调控: 基于神经振荡夹带现象. 心理科学进展, 30(4), 802-816.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2022.00802 |
[5] | 吴国来, 周曼, 阴晓娟, 李海英, 李娇花. (2017). 走神对中文阅读绩效的影响. 心理与行为研究, 15(3), 329-334. |
[6] |
Abd-Eldayem, R. M. A., & Shaheen, N. A. E. A. (2021). Mindfulness as a mediator between mind-wandering and creative abilities. American Journal of Applied Psychology, 10(6), 136-145.
doi: 10.11648/j.ajap.20211006.12 URL |
[7] | Ahuja, S., & Vashishtha, S. (2019). Effect of surat-shabd- yoga meditation on mind wandering & metacognition. Journal of Consciousness Exploration & Research, 10(8), 621-629. |
[8] |
Ainsworth, B., Eddershaw, R., Meron, D., Baldwin, D. S., & Garner, M. (2013). The effect of focused attention and open monitoring meditation on attention network function in healthy volunteers. Psychiatry Research, 210(3), 1226-1231.
doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2013.09.002 pmid: 24135553 |
[9] |
Alain, C., & Woods, D. L. (1997). Attention modulates auditory pattern memory as indexed by event-related brain potentials. Psychophysiology, 34(5), 534-546.
pmid: 9299908 |
[10] |
Badart, P., McDowall, J., & Prime, S. L. (2018). Multimodal sustained attention superiority in concentrative meditators compared to nonmeditators. Mindfulness, 9(3), 824-835.
doi: 10.1007/s12671-017-0822-y URL |
[11] |
Banks, J. B., Jha, A. P., Hood, A. V., Goller, H. G., & Craig, L. L. (2019). Reducing the TUTs that hurt: The impact of a brief mindfulness induction on emotionally valenced mind wandering. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 31(8), 785-799.
doi: 10.1080/20445911.2019.1676759 URL |
[12] | Belardi, A., Chaieb, L., Rey-Mermet, A., Mormann, F., Rothen, N., Fell, J., & Reber, T. P. (2022). On the relationship between mind wandering and mindfulness. Scientific Reports, 12(1), 7755. |
[13] |
Braboszcz, C., & Delorme, A. (2011). Lost in thoughts: Neural markers of low alertness during mind wandering. Neuroimage, 54(4), 3040-3047.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.10.008 pmid: 20946963 |
[14] |
Brandmeyer, T., & Delorme, A. (2018). Reduced mind wandering in experienced meditators and associated EEG correlates. Experimental Brain Research, 236(9), 2519-2528.
doi: 10.1007/s00221-016-4811-5 pmid: 27815577 |
[15] |
Brandmeyer, T., & Delorme, A. (2021). Meditation and the wandering mind: A theoretical framework of underlying neurocognitive mechanisms. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 16(1), 39-66.
doi: 10.1177/1745691620917340 URL |
[16] |
Brewer, J. A., Worhunsky, P. D., Gray, J. R., Tang, Y. Y., Weber, J., & Kober, H. (2011). Meditation experience is associated with differences in default mode network activity and connectivity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(50), 20254-20259.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1112029108 URL |
[17] |
Cahn, B. R., Delorme, A., & Polich, J. (2010). Occipital gamma activation during Vipassana meditation. Cognitive Processing, 11(1), 39-56.
doi: 10.1007/s10339-009-0352-1 pmid: 20013298 |
[18] | Chaieb, L., Hoppe, C., & Fell, J. (2022). Mind wandering and depression:A status report. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 133, 104505. |
[19] |
Chou, Y. H., Sundman, M., Whitson, H. E., Gaur, P., Chu, M. L., Weingarten, C. P.,... Chen, N. K. (2017). Maintenance and representation of mind wandering during resting-state fMRI. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 1-11.
doi: 10.1038/s41598-016-0028-x |
[20] |
Christoff, K., Gordon, A. M., Smallwood, J., Smith, R., & Schooler, J. W. (2009). Experience sampling during fMRI reveals default network and executive system contributions to mind wandering. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(21), 8719-8724.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.0900234106 URL |
[21] |
Desbordes, G., Gard, T., Hoge, E. A., Hölzel, B. K., Kerr, C., Lazar, S. W.,... Vago, D. R. (2015). Moving beyond mindfulness: Defining equanimity as an outcome measure in meditation and contemplative research. Mindfulness, 6(2), 356-372.
doi: 10.1007/s12671-013-0269-8 URL |
[22] |
Folstein, J. R., & Van Petten, C. (2008). Influence of cognitive control and mismatch on the N2 component of the ERP: A review. Psychophysiology, 45(1), 152-170.
doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00602.x pmid: 17850238 |
[23] | Garrison, K. A., Zeffiro, T. A., Scheinost, D., Constable, R. T., & Brewer, J. A. (2015). Meditation leads to reduced default mode network activity beyond an active task. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 15(3), 712-720. |
[24] |
Giannandrea, A., Simione, L., Pescatori, B., Ferrell, K., Olivetti Belardinelli, M., Hickman, S. D., & Raffone, A. (2019). Effects of the mindfulness-based stress reduction program on mind wandering and dispositional mindfulness facets. Mindfulness, 10(1), 185-195.
doi: 10.1007/s12671-018-1070-5 |
[25] |
Hasenkamp, W., Wilson-Mendenhall, C. D., Duncan, E., & Barsalou, L. W. (2012). Mind wandering and attention during focused meditation: A fine-grained temporal analysis of fluctuating cognitive states. Neuroimage, 59(1), 750-760.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.07.008 pmid: 21782031 |
[26] |
He, H., Hu, L., Zhang, X., & Qiu, J. (2021). Pleasantness of mind wandering is positively associated with focus back effort in daily life: Evidence from resting state fMRI. Brain and Cognition, 150, 105731.
doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2021.105731 URL |
[27] |
Hinterberger, T., Schmidt, S., Kamei, T., & Walach, H. (2014). Decreased electrophysiological activity represents the conscious state of emptiness in meditation. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 99.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00099 pmid: 24596562 |
[28] |
Ho, N. S. P., Poerio, G., Konu, D., Turnbull, A., Sormaz, M., Leech, R.,... Smallwood, J. (2020). Facing up to the wandering mind: Patterns of off-task laboratory thought are associated with stronger neural recruitment of right fusiform cortex while processing facial stimuli. Neuroimage, 214, 116765.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116765 URL |
[29] |
Hofmann, S. G., Grossman, P., & Hinton, D. E. (2011). Loving-kindness and compassion meditation: Potential for psychological interventions. Clinical Psychology Review, 31(7), 1126-1132.
doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2011.07.003 pmid: 21840289 |
[30] |
Jankowski, T., & Holas, P. (2020). Effects of brief mindfulness meditation on attention switching. Mindfulness, 11(5), 1150-1158.
doi: 10.1007/s12671-020-01314-9 |
[31] |
Kabat-Zinn, J. (2003). Mindfulness-based interventions in context: Past, present, and future. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 10(2), 144-156.
doi: 10.1093/clipsy.bpg016 URL |
[32] |
King, B. G., Conklin, Q. A., Zanesco, A. P., & Saron, C. D. (2019). Residential meditation retreats: Their role in contemplative practice and significance for psychological research. Current Opinion in Psychology, 28, 238-244.
doi: S2352-250X(18)30183-0 pmid: 30878004 |
[33] |
Klee, D., Colgan, D. D., Hanes, D., & Oken, B. (2020). The effects of an internet-based mindfulness meditation intervention on electrophysiological markers of attention. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 158, 103-113.
doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2020.10.002 pmid: 33080294 |
[34] |
Konjedi, S., & Maleeh, R. (2021). The dynamic framework of mind wandering revisited: How mindful meta- awareness affects mental states’ constraints. Consciousness and Cognition, 95, 103194.
doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2021.103194 URL |
[35] |
Liu, Y., Hou, Y., Quan, H., Zhao, D., Zhao, J., Cao, B.,... Yuan, H. (2023). Mindfulness training improves attention: Evidence from behavioral and event-related potential analyses. Brain Topography, 36(4), 243-254.
doi: 10.1007/s10548-023-00938-z |
[36] |
Lomas, T., Ivtzan, I., & Fu, C. H. (2015). A systematic review of the neurophysiology of mindfulness on EEG oscillations. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 57, 401-410.
doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.09.018 URL |
[37] |
Lutz, A., Slagter, H. A., Dunne, J. D., & Davidson, R. J. (2008). Attention regulation and monitoring in meditation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(4), 163-169.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.01.005 pmid: 18329323 |
[38] |
Lykins, E. L., Baer, R. A., & Gottlob, L. R. (2012). Performance-based tests of attention and memory in long-term mindfulness meditators and demographically matched nonmeditators. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 36(1), 103-114.
doi: 10.1007/s10608-010-9318-y URL |
[39] | Martel, A., Arvaneh, M., Robertson, I., Smallwood, J., & Dockree, P. (2019). Distinct neural markers for intentional and unintentional task unrelated thought. BioRxiv. Advance online publication. 10.1101/705061 |
[40] |
McVay, J. C., & Kane, M. J. (2010). Does mind wandering reflect executive function or executive failure? comment on Smallwood and Schooler (2006) and Watkins (2008). Psychological Bulletin, 136(2), 188-197.
doi: 10.1037/a0018298 pmid: 20192557 |
[41] |
Mittner, M., Boekel, W., Tucker, A. M., Turner, B. M., Heathcote, A., & Forstmann, B. U. (2014). When the brain takes a break: A model-based analysis of mind wandering. Journal of Neuroscience, 34(49), 16286-16295.
doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2062-14.2014 pmid: 25471568 |
[42] |
Mo, X., Qin, Q., Wu, F., Li, H., Tang, Y., Cheng, Q., & Wen, Y. (2021). Effects of breathing meditation training on sustained attention level, mindfulness attention awareness level, and mental state of operating room nurses. American Journal of Health Behavior, 45(6), 993-1001.
doi: 10.5993/AJHB.45.6.4 pmid: 34969411 |
[43] |
Mooneyham, B. W., Mrazek, M. D., Mrazek, A. J., Mrazek, K. L., Phillips, D. T., & Schooler, J. W. (2017). States of mind: Characterizing the neural bases of focus and mind-wandering through dynamic functional connectivity. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 29(3), 495-506.
doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01066 pmid: 27779908 |
[44] |
Mrazek, M. D., Franklin, M. S., Phillips, D. T., Baird, B., & Schooler, J. W. (2013). Mindfulness training improves working memory capacity and GRE performance while reducing mind wandering. Psychological Science, 24(5), 776-781.
doi: 10.1177/0956797612459659 pmid: 23538911 |
[45] |
Mrazek, M. D., Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2012). Mindfulness and mind-wandering: Finding convergence through opposing constructs. Emotion, 12(3), 442-448.
doi: 10.1037/a0026678 pmid: 22309719 |
[46] |
Norris, C. J., Creem, D., Hendler, R., & Kober, H. (2018). Brief mindfulness meditation improves attention in novices: Evidence from ERPs and moderation by neuroticism. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 12, 315.
doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00315 pmid: 30127731 |
[47] |
Ooishi, Y., Fujino, M., Inoue, V., Nomura, M., & Kitagawa, N. (2021). Differential effects of focused attention and open monitoring meditation on autonomic cardiac modulation and cortisol secretion. Frontiers in Physiology, 12, 675899.
doi: 10.3389/fphys.2021.675899 URL |
[48] |
Phillips, N. E., Mills, C., D'Mello, S., & Risko, E. F. (2016). On the influence of re-reading on mind wandering. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69(12), 2338-2357.
doi: 10.1080/17470218.2015.1107109 URL |
[49] | Rahl, H. A., Lindsay, E. K., Pacilio, L. E., Brown, K. W., & Creswell, J. D. (2017). Brief mindfulness meditation training reduces mind wandering: The critical role of acceptance. Emotion, 17(2), 224. |
[50] |
Ray, W. J., & Cole, H. W. (1985). EEG alpha activity reflects attentional demands, and beta activity reflects emotional and cognitive processes. Science, 228(4700), 750-752.
pmid: 3992243 |
[51] |
Reangsing, C., Rittiwong, T., & Schneider, J. K. (2021). Effects of mindfulness meditation interventions on depression in older adults: A meta-analysis. Aging & Mental Health, 25(7), 1181-1190.
doi: 10.1080/13607863.2020.1793901 URL |
[52] |
Rodriguez-Larios, J., de Oca, E. A. B. M., & Alaerts, K . (2021). The EEG spectral properties of meditation and mind wandering differ between experienced meditators and novices. Neuroimage, 245, 118669.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118669 URL |
[53] |
Ruby, F. J., Smallwood, J., Sackur, J., & Singer, T. (2013). Is self-generated thought a means of social problem solving?. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 962.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00962 pmid: 24391621 |
[54] |
Sanger, K. L., & Dorjee, D. (2016). Mindfulness training with adolescents enhances metacognition and the inhibition of irrelevant stimuli: Evidence from event- related brain potentials. Trends in Neuroscience and Education, 5(1), 1-11.
doi: 10.1016/j.tine.2016.01.001 URL |
[55] |
Sayette, M. A., Reichle, E. D., & Schooler, J. W. (2009). Lost in the sauce: The effects of alcohol on mind wandering. Psychological Science, 20(6), 747-752.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02351.x pmid: 19422627 |
[56] |
Scheibner, H. J., Bogler, C., Gleich, T., Haynes, J. D., & Bermpohl, F. (2017). Internal and external attention and the default mode network. Neuroimage, 148, 381-389.
doi: S1053-8119(17)30050-2 pmid: 28110087 |
[57] |
Schooler, J. W., Smallwood, J., Christoff, K., Handy, T. C., Reichle, E. D., & Sayette, M. A. (2011). Meta-awareness, perceptual decoupling and the wandering mind. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(7), 319-326.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.05.006 pmid: 21684189 |
[58] |
Seli, P., Ralph, B. C., Konishi, M., Smilek, D., & Schacter, D. L. (2017). What did you have in mind? Examining the content of intentional and unintentional types of mind wandering. Consciousness and Cognition, 51, 149-156.
doi: S1053-8100(17)30013-2 pmid: 28371688 |
[59] |
Smallwood, J. (2013). Distinguishing how from why the mind wanders: A process-occurrence framework for self-generated mental activity. Psychological Bulletin, 139(3), 519-535.
doi: 10.1037/a0030010 pmid: 23607430 |
[60] |
Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2006). The restless mind. Psychological Bulletin, 132(6), 946-958.
doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.6.946 pmid: 17073528 |
[61] |
Smallwood, J., Ruby, F. J., & Singer, T. (2013). Letting go of the present: Mind-wandering is associated with reduced delay discounting. Consciousness and Cognition, 22(1), 1-7.
doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2012.10.007 pmid: 23178292 |
[62] | Somaraju, L. H., Temple, E. C., Bizo, L. A., & Cocks, B. (2021). Brief mindfulness meditation: Can it make a real difference?. Current Psychology, 36, 5530-5542. |
[63] |
Tanaka, G. K., Peressutti, C., Teixeira, S., Cagy, M., Piedade, R., Nardi, A. E.,... Velasques, B. (2014). Lower trait frontal theta activity in mindfulness meditators. Arquivos de Neuro-Psiquiatria, 72(9), 687-693.
doi: S0004-282X2014000900687 pmid: 25252232 |
[64] |
Tang, Y. Y., & Posner, M. I. (2009). Attention training and attention state training. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(5), 222-227.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.01.009 URL |
[65] |
Ueberholz, R. Y., & Fiocco, A. J. (2022). The effect of a brief mindfulness practice on perceived stress and sustained attention: Does priming matter?. Mindfulness, 13(7), 1757-1768.
doi: 10.1007/s12671-022-01913-8 pmid: 35693031 |
[66] |
Wammes, J. D., Seli, P., Cheyne, J. A., Boucher, P. O., & Smilek, D. (2016). Mind wandering during lectures II: Relation to academic performance. Scholarship of Teaching and Learning in Psychology, 2(1), 33-48.
doi: 10.1037/stl0000055 URL |
[67] |
Webb, C. A., Israel, E. S., Belleau, E., Appleman, L., Forbes, E. E., & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2021). Mind-wandering in adolescents predicts worse affect and is linked to aberrant default mode network-salience network connectivity. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 60(3), 377-387.
doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2020.03.010 URL |
[68] |
Whitmoyer, P., Fountain-Zaragoza, S., Andridge, R., Bredemeier, K., Londeree, A., Kaye, L., & Prakash, R. S. (2020). Mindfulness training and attentional control in older adults: A randomized controlled trial. Mindfulness, 11(1), 203-218.
doi: 10.1007/s12671-019-01218-3 |
[69] |
Williams, J. M. G., & Kabat-Zinn, J. (2011). Mindfulness: Diverse perspectives on its meaning, origins, and multiple applications at the intersection of science and dharma. Contemporary Buddhism, 12(1), 1-18.
doi: 10.1080/14639947.2011.564811 URL |
[70] |
Wong, K. F., Massar, A. A. S., Chee, M. W., & Lim, J. (2018). Towards an objective measure of mindfulness: Replicating and extending the features of the breath-counting task. Mindfulness, 9(5), 1402-1410.
doi: 10.1007/s12671-017-0880-1 pmid: 30294387 |
[71] |
Xu, M., Purdon, C., Seli, P., & Smilek, D. (2017). Mindfulness and mind wandering: The protective effects of brief meditation in anxious individuals. Consciousness and Cognition, 51, 157-165.
doi: S1053-8100(16)30314-2 pmid: 28376373 |
[72] |
Yoshida, K., Sawamura, D., Yagi, M., Nakashima, Y., Saito, R., Yamamura, N.,... Sakai, S. (2023). Detecting inattentiveness caused by mind-wandering during a driving task: A behavioral study. Applied Ergonomics, 106, 103892.
doi: 10.1016/j.apergo.2022.103892 URL |
[73] |
Zanesco, A. P., King, B. G., MacLean, K. A., & Saron, C. D. (2018). Cognitive aging and long-term maintenance of attentional improvements following meditation training. Journal of Cognitive Enhancement, 2, 259-275.
doi: 10.1007/s41465-018-0068-1 |
[1] | ZHENG Hao, CHEN Rongrong, MAI Xiaoqin. The cognitive and neural mechanism of third-party punishment [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2024, 32(2): 398-412. |
[2] | CAO Jinjing, QIU Shiming, DING Xianfeng, CHENG Xiaorong, FAN Zhao. The gradedness and richness of consciousness: Two pathways toward decoding consciousness [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2023, 31(7): 1172-1185. |
[3] | WANG Yongli, GE Shengnan, Lancy Lantin Huang, WAN Qin, LU Haidan. Neural mechanism of speech imagery [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2023, 31(4): 608-621. |
[4] | LIU Yong, CHEN Hong. Neural mechanism of food-related working memory in individuals with overweight/obesity and related intervention [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2023, 31(10): 1775-1784. |
[5] | ZHANG Mingxia, LI Yuxin, LI Jin, LIU Xun. The influence of extrinsic and intrinsic motivation on memory in adolescents and the underlying neural mechanisms [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2023, 31(1): 1-9. |
[6] | WANG Songxue, CHENG Si, JIANG Ting, LIU Xun, ZHANG Mingxia. The effect of external rewards on declarative memory [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2023, 31(1): 78-86. |
[7] | DENG Xun, CHEN Ning, WANG Dandan, ZHAO Huanhuan, HE Wen. Neural mechanism of NSSI and comparative study with comorbidities [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(7): 1561-1573. |
[8] | DENG Yao, WANG Mengmeng, RAO Hengyi. Risk-taking research based on the Balloon Analog Risk Task [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(6): 1377-1392. |
[9] | LI Liang, LI Hong. Cognitive mechanism and neural basis of shyness [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(5): 1038-1049. |
[10] | ZHANG Lina, XUAN Bin. Neural mechanisms and time course of the age-related word frequency effect in language production [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(2): 333-342. |
[11] | LI Hehui, HUANG Huiya, DONG Lin, LUO Yuejia, TAO Wuhai. Developmental dyslexia and cerebellar abnormalities: Multiple roles of the cerebellum and causal relationships between the two [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(2): 343-353. |
[12] | HU Jia-Bao, LEI Yang, DING Xian-Feng, CHENG Xiao-Rong, FAN Zhao. Shared vs. private aesthetic tastes: The cognitive and neural mechanisms [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(2): 354-364. |
[13] | CHEN Qunlin, DING Ke. Serial order effect during divergent thinking: A new perspective on the dynamic mechanism of creative thought processes [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(11): 2507-2517. |
[14] | HUANG Jianping, XU Jingxian, WAN Xiaoang. Influence of associative learning on consumer behavior: From the perspective of product search experience [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(11): 2414-2423. |
[15] | WANG Zile, ZHANG Qi. The internal mechanisms of attentional templates in facilitating visual search [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(10): 2206-2218. |
Viewed | ||||||
Full text |
|
|||||
Abstract |
|
|||||