心理科学进展 ›› 2022, Vol. 30 ›› Issue (11): 2558-2569.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2022.02558
收稿日期:
2022-03-12
出版日期:
2022-11-15
发布日期:
2022-11-09
通讯作者:
苏怡
E-mail:sy-esther@csu.edu.cn
基金资助:
Received:
2022-03-12
Online:
2022-11-15
Published:
2022-11-09
Contact:
SU Yi (ESTHER)
E-mail:sy-esther@csu.edu.cn
摘要:
孤独症语言障碍的表现之一是词汇发展滞后, 可能与其注意和记忆损伤有关。当前研究结果表明, 孤独症儿童在学习词汇时难以利用社会注意提供的有效信息, 且其注意易受到无关刺激干扰, 这可能导致其形成的物体-词汇的联结不稳定, 影响其进一步将这种联结整合到心理词典并保存在记忆中。未来研究应探究联合注意影响孤独症儿童词汇学习的发展轨迹和机制, 儿童的词汇知识对其词汇记忆的影响, 并关注自然场景中孤独症儿童的词汇学习过程和个体差异。
中图分类号:
侯文文, 苏怡. (2022). 注意和记忆对孤独症儿童词汇发展滞后的影响. 心理科学进展 , 30(11), 2558-2569.
HOU Wenwen, SU Yi (ESTHER). (2022). The influence of atypical attention and memory on vocabulary delay in children with autism spectrum disorder. Advances in Psychological Science, 30(11), 2558-2569.
[1] |
Abdelaziz, A., Kover, S. T., Wagner, M., & Naigles, L. R. (2018). The shape bias in children with autism spectrum disorder: Potential sources of individual differences. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 61(11), 2685-2702. https://doi.org/10.1044/2018_jslhr-l-rsaut-18-0027
doi: 10.1044/2018_JSLHR-L-RSAUT-18-0027 URL |
[2] | Adamson, L. B., Bakeman, R., Suma, K., & Robins, D. L. (2017). An expanded view of joint attention: Skill, engagement, and language in typical development and autism. Child Development, 90(1), e1-e18. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12973 |
[3] |
Akechi, H., Senju, A., Kikuchi, Y., Tojo, Y., Osanai, H., & Hasegawa, T. (2011). Do children with ASD use referential gaze to learn the name of an object? An eye-tracking study. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5(3), 1230-1242. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd. 2011.01.013
doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2011.01.013 URL |
[4] | American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596 |
[5] |
Arunachalam, S., & Luyster, R. J. (2018). Lexical development in young children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD): How ASD may affect intake from the input. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 61(11), 2659-2672. https://doi.org/10.1044/2018_JSLHR-L-RSAUT-18-0024
doi: 10.1044/2018_JSLHR-L-RSAUT-18-0024 URL |
[6] |
Axelsson, E. L., Williams, S. E., & Horst, J. S. (2016). The effect of sleep on children's word retention and generalization. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1192. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01192
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01192 URL pmid: 27588007 |
[7] |
Bacon, E. C., Moore, A., Lee, Q., Carter Barnes, C., Courchesne, E., & Pierce, K. (2020). Identifying prognostic markers in autism spectrum disorder using eye tracking. Autism, 24(3), 658-669. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361319878578
doi: 10.1177/1362361319878578 URL pmid: 31647314 |
[8] | Bal, V. H., Katz, T., Bishop, S. L., & Krasileva, K. (2016). Understanding definitions of minimally verbal across instruments: Evidence for subgroups within minimally verbal children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 57(12), 1424-1433. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12609 |
[9] |
Baranek G. T., Woynaroski T. G., Nowell S., Turner-Brown L., DuBay M., Crais E. R., & Watson L. R. (2018). Cascading effects of attention disengagement and sensory seeking on social symptoms in a community sample of infants at-risk for a future diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 29, 30-40. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2017.08.006
doi: S1878-9293(16)30180-3 URL pmid: 28869201 |
[10] | Bean Ellawadi, A., & McGregor, K. K. (2016). Children with ASD can use gaze to map new words. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 51(2), 212-218. https://doi.org/10.1111/1460-6984.12192 |
[11] | Bhat, A. N., Galloway, J. C., & Landa, R. J. (2010). Social and non-social visual attention patterns and associative learning in infants at risk for autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 51(9), 989-997. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02262.x |
[12] |
Botting, N., & Baraka, N. (2017). Sleep behaviour relates to language skills in children with and without communication disorders. International Journal of Developmental Disabilities, 64(4-5), 238-243. https://doi.org/10.1080/20473869.2017.1283766
doi: 10.1080/20473869.2017.1283766 URL |
[13] |
Boucher, J., Mayes, A., & Bigham, S. (2012). Memory in autistic spectrum disorder. Psychological Bulletin, 138(3), 458-496. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026869
doi: 10.1037/a0026869 URL pmid: 22409507 |
[14] |
Bruner, J. S. (1974). From communication to language—A psychological perspective. Cognition, 3(3), 255-287.
doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(74)90012-2 URL |
[15] |
Bruyneel, E., Demurie, E., Warreyn, P., & Roeyers, H. (2019). The mediating role of joint attention in the relationship between motor skills and receptive and expressive language in siblings at risk for autism spectrum disorder. Infant Behavior and Development, 57, 101377. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2019.101377
doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2019.101377 URL |
[16] |
Campbell, D. J., Shic, F., Macari, S., & Chawarska, K. (2014). Gaze response to dyadic bids at 2 years related to outcomes at 3 years in autism spectrum disorders: A subtyping analysis. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44(2), 431-442. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-013-1885-9
doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1885-9 URL pmid: 23877749 |
[17] |
Carter, C. K., & Hartley, C. (2021). Are children with autism more likely to retain object names when learning from colour photographs or black-and-white cartoons? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 51(9), 3050-3062. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-020-04771-2
doi: 10.1007/s10803-020-04771-2 URL pmid: 33156474 |
[18] |
Çetinçelik, M., Rowland, C. F., & Snijders, T. M. (2021). Do the eyes have it? A systematic review on the role of eye gaze in infant language development. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 589096. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.589096
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.589096 URL |
[19] |
Charman, T., Drew, A., Baird, C., & Baird, G. (2003). Measuring early language development in preschool children with autism spectrum disorder using the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory (Infant Form). Journal of Child Language, 30(1), 213-236.https://doi.org/10.1017/s0305000902005482
URL pmid: 12718299 |
[20] |
Chawarska, K., Macari, S., & Shic, F. (2013). Decreased spontaneous attention to social scenes in 6-month-old infants later diagnosed with autism spectrum disorders. Biological Psychiatry, 74(3), 195-203. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.11.022
doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.11.022 URL pmid: 23313640 |
[21] |
Chen, C.-H., Houston, D. M., & Yu, C. (2021). Parent-child joint behaviors in novel object play create high-quality data for word learning. Child Development, 92(5), 1889-1905.https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13620
doi: 10.1111/cdev.13620 URL pmid: 34463350 |
[22] |
Choi, B., Leech, K. A., Tager-flusberg, H., & Nelson, C. A. (2018). Development of fine motor skills is associated with expressive language outcomes in infants at high and low risk for autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders, 10(14), 1-11. https://doi.org/10.1186/s11689-018-9231-3
doi: 10.1186/s11689-017-9222-9 URL |
[23] | Davis, M. H., & Gaskell, M. G. (2009). A complementary systems account of word learning: Neural and behavioural evidence. Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 364( 1536), 3773-3800.https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2009.0111 |
[24] |
Edmunds, S. R., Ibanez, L. V., Warren, Z., Messinger, D. S., & Stone, W. L. (2017). Longitudinal prediction of language emergence in infants at high and low risk for autism spectrum disorder. Development and Psychopathology, 29(1), 319-329. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579416000146
doi: 10.1017/S0954579416000146 URL pmid: 27048735 |
[25] |
Ellis Weismer, S., Lord, C., & Esler, A. (2010). Early language patterns of toddlers on the autism spectrum compared to toddlers with developmental delay. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(10), 1259-1273. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-010-0983-1
doi: 10.1007/s10803-010-0983-1 URL pmid: 20195735 |
[26] |
Field, C., Lewis, C., & Allen, M. L. (2019). Referent selection in children with autism spectrum condition and intellectual disabilities: Do social cues affect word-to- object or word-to-location mappings? Research in Developmental Disabilities, 91, 103425. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2019.05.004
doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2019.05.004 URL |
[27] |
Fitch, A., Lieberman, A. M., Luyster, R. J., & Arunachalam, S. (2020). Toddlers' word learning through overhearing: Others' attention matters. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 193, 104793. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2019.104793
doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2019.104793 URL |
[28] | Fletcher, F. E., Knowland, V., Walker, S., Gaskell, M. G., Norbury, C., & Henderson, L. M. (2020). Atypicalities in sleep and semantic consolidation in autism. Developmental Science, 23(3), e12906. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12906 |
[29] |
Franchini, M., Duku, E., Armstrong, V., Brian, J., Bryson, S. E., Garon, N., … Smith, I. M. (2018). Variability in verbal and nonverbal communication in infants at risk for autism spectrum disorder: Predictors and outcomes. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 48(10), 3417-3431. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-018-3607-9
doi: 10.1007/s10803-018-3607-9 URL pmid: 29767824 |
[30] |
Gangopadhyay, I., & Kaushanskaya, M. (2020). The role of speaker eye gaze and mutual exclusivity in novel word learning by monolingual and bilingual children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 197, 104878. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2020.104878
doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2020.104878 URL |
[31] |
Gaskell, M. G., & Dumay, N. (2003). Lexical competition and the acquisition of novel words. Cognition, 89(2), 105-132. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0010-0277(03)00070-2
URL pmid: 12915296 |
[32] |
Gernsbacher, M. A., & Morson, E. (2019). Fast mapping is a laboratory task, not a cognitive capacity. Cognitive Neuroscience, 10(4), 223-225. https://doi.org/10.1080/17588928.2019.1573810
doi: 10.1080/17588928.2019.1573810 URL pmid: 30669945 |
[33] |
Gliga, T., Elsabbagh, M., Hudry, K., Charman, T., Johnson, M. H., & BASIS, Team. (2012). Gaze following, gaze reading, and word learning in children at risk for autism. Child Development, 83(3), 926-938.https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01750.x
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01750.x URL pmid: 22462503 |
[34] | Gliga, T., Skolnick, A., Liersch, U., Charman, T., Johnson, M. H., Bedford, R., & BASIS, Team. (2021). Investigating the mechanisms driving referent selection and retention in toddlers at typical and elevated likelihood for autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Child Language. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000921000416 |
[35] | Grandgeorge, M., Hausberger, M., Tordjman, S., Deleau, M., Lazartigues, A., & Lemonnier, E. (2009). Environmental factors influence language development in children with autism spectrum disorders. PLoS One, 4(4), e4683. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0004683 |
[36] |
Habayeb, S., Tsang, T., Saulnier, C., Klaiman, C., Jones, W., Klin, A., & Edwards, L. A. (2021). Visual traces of language acquisition in toddlers with autism spectrum disorder during the second year of life. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 51(7), 2519-2530. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-020-04730-x
doi: 10.1007/s10803-020-04730-x URL |
[37] |
Haebig, E., Jiménez, E., Cox, C. R., & Hills, T. T. (2021). Characterizing the early vocabulary profiles of preverbal and minimally verbal children with autism spectrum disorder. Autism, 25(4), 958-970. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361320973799
doi: 10.1177/1362361320973799 URL pmid: 33246365 |
[38] |
Hartley, C., Bird, L.-A., & Monaghan, P. (2019). Investigating the relationship between fast mapping, retention, and generalisation of words in children with autism spectrum disorder and typical development. Cognition, 187, 126-138. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2019.03.001
doi: S0010-0277(19)30056-3 URL pmid: 30861409 |
[39] |
Hartley, C., Bird, L.-A., & Monaghan, P. (2020). Comparing cross-situational word learning, retention, and generalisation in children with autism and typical development. Cognition, 200, 104265. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104265
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104265 URL |
[40] |
He, A. X., Huang, S., Waxman, S., & Arunachalam, S. (2020). Two-year-olds consolidate verb meanings during a nap. Cognition, 198, 104205. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104205
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104205 URL |
[41] |
Henderson, L., Powell, A., Gaskell, M. G., & Norbury, C. (2014). Learning and consolidation of new spoken words in autism spectrum disorder. Developmental Science, 17(6), 858-871. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12169
doi: 10.1111/desc.12169 URL pmid: 24636285 |
[42] |
Hillairet de Boisferon, A., Tift, A. H., Minar, N. J., & Lewkowicz, D. J. (2018). The redeployment of attention to the mouth of a talking face during the second year of life. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 172, 189-200. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2018.03.009
doi: S0022-0965(17)30302-8 URL pmid: 29627481 |
[43] |
Hudac, C. M., DesChamps, T. D., Arnett, A. B., Cairney, B. E., Ma, R., Webb, S. J., & Bernier, R. A. (2018). Early enhanced processing and delayed habituation to deviance sounds in autism spectrum disorder. Brain and Cognition, 123, 110-119. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2018.03.004
doi: S0278-2626(17)30373-1 URL pmid: 29550506 |
[44] |
James, E., Gaskell, M. G., Weighall, A., & Henderson, L. (2017). Consolidation of vocabulary during sleep: The rich get richer? Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 77, 1-13. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.01.054
doi: S0149-7634(16)30578-4 URL pmid: 28274725 |
[45] |
Jiménez, E., Haebig, E., & Hills, T. T. (2021). Identifying areas of overlap and distinction in early lexical profiles of children with autism spectrum disorder, late talkers, and typical talkers. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 51(9), 3109-3125.https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-020-04772-1
doi: 10.1007/s10803-020-04772-1 URL pmid: 33156473 |
[46] |
Johnels, J. A., Gillberg, C., Falck-Ytter, T., & Miniscalco, C. (2014). Face-viewing patterns in young children with autism spectrum disorders: Speaking up for the role of language comprehension. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 57(6), 2246-2252. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2018.03.004
doi: 10.1044/2014_JSLHR-L-13-0268 URL |
[47] | Jones, W., & Klin, A. (2013). Attention to eyes is present but in decline in 2-6 -month-old infants later diagnosed with autism. Nature, 504(7480), 427-431. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature12715 |
[48] | Joseph, R. M., Skwerer, D. P., Eggleston, B., Meyer, S. R., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2019). An experimental study of word learning in minimally verbal children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Autism & Developmental Language Impairments, 4, 1-13. https://doi.org/10.1177/2396941519834717 |
[49] |
Kleberg, J. L., Thorup, E., & Falck-Ytter, T. (2017). Reduced visual disengagement but intact phasic alerting in young children with autism. Autism Research, 10(3), 539-545. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1675
doi: 10.1002/aur.1675 URL pmid: 27696688 |
[50] |
Kover, S. T., & Ellis Weismer, S. (2014). Lexical characteristics of expressive vocabulary in toddlers with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 57(4), 1428-1441. https://doi.org/10.1044/2014_JSLHR-L-13-0006
doi: 10.1044/2014_JSLHR-L-13-0006 URL |
[51] |
Kujala, T., Aho, E., Lepistö, T., Jansson-Verkasalo, E., Nieminen-von Wendt, T., von Wendt, L., & Näätänen, R. (2007). Atypical pattern of discriminating sound features in adults with Asperger syndrome as reflected by the mismatch negativity. Biological Psychology, 75(1), 109-114. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.12.007
URL pmid: 17257732 |
[52] |
Landau, B., Smith, L. B., & Jones, S. S. (1988). The importance of shape in early lexical learning. Cognitive Development, 3(3), 299-321.
doi: 10.1016/0885-2014(88)90014-7 URL |
[53] |
Lewkowicz, D. J., & Hansen-Tift, A. M. (2012). Infants deploy selective attention to the mouth of a talking face when learning speech. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109(5), 1431-1436. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1114783109
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1114783109 URL pmid: 22307596 |
[54] |
Liu, Q., Wang, Q., Li, X., Gong, X., Luo, X., Yin, T., Liu, J., & Yi, L. (2021). Social synchronization during joint attention in children with autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research, 14(10), 2120-2130.https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.2553
doi: 10.1002/aur.2553 URL pmid: 34105871 |
[55] |
Luyster, R., Lopez, K., & Lord, C. (2007). Characterizing communicative development in children referred for autism spectrum disorders using the MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventory (CDI). Journal of Child Language, 34(3), 623-654. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0305000907008094
URL pmid: 17822142 |
[56] |
Maes, P., Stercq, F., & Kissine, M. (2021). Attention to intentional versus incidental pointing gestures in young autistic children: An eye-tracking study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 210, 105205. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2021.105205
doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2021.105205 URL |
[57] |
Marchman, V. A., Adams, K. A., Loi, E. C., Fernald, A., & Feldman, H. M. (2016). Early language processing efficiency predicts later receptive vocabulary outcomes in children born preterm. Child Neuropsychology, 22(6), 649-665. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2015.1038987
doi: 10.1080/09297049.2015.1038987 URL pmid: 26031342 |
[58] | Mathée-Scott, J., Larson, C., Venker, C., Pomper, R., Edwards, J., Saffran, J., & Ellis Weismer, S. (2021). Use of mutual exclusivity and its relationship to language ability in toddlers with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-021-05321-0 |
[59] | McGregor, K. K., Rost, G., Arenas, R., Farris-Trimble, A., & Stiles, D. (2013). Children with ASD can use gaze in support of word recognition and learning. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 54(7), 745-753. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12073 |
[60] |
Norbury, C. F., Griffiths, H., & Nation, K. (2010). Sound before meaning: Word learning in autistic disorders. Neuropsychologia, 48(14), 4012-4019. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.10.015
doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.10.015 URL pmid: 20951710 |
[61] |
Nyström, P., Thorup, E., Bölte, S., & Falck-Ytter, T. (2019). Joint attention in infancy and the emergence of autism. Biological Psychiatry, 86(8), 631-638. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2019.05.006
doi: S0006-3223(19)31372-1 URL pmid: 31262432 |
[62] |
Paparella, T., Goods, K. S., Freeman, S., & Kasari, C. (2011). The emergence of nonverbal joint attention and requesting skills in young children with autism. Journal of Communication Disorders, 44(6), 569-583. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2011.08.002
doi: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2011.08.002 URL pmid: 21907346 |
[63] |
Reinhartsen, D. B., Tapia, A. L., Watson, L., Crais, E., Bradley, C., Fairchild, J., Herring, A. H., & Daniels, J. (2019). Expressive dominant versus receptive dominant language patterns in young children: Findings from the study to explore early development. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 49(6), 2447-2460. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-019-03999-x
doi: 10.1007/s10803-019-03999-x URL pmid: 30937735 |
[64] |
Russo, S., Calignano, G., Dispaldro, M., & Valenza, E. (2021). An integrated perspective on spatio-temporal attention and infant language acquisition. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 18(4), 1592. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18041592
doi: 10.3390/ijerph18041592 URL |
[65] |
Spiegel, C., & Halberda, J. (2011). Rapid fast-mapping abilities in 2-year-olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 109(1), 132-140. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2010.10.013
doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2010.10.013 URL pmid: 21145067 |
[66] |
Stanovich, K. E. (1986). Matthew effects in reading: Some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy. Reading Research Quarterly, 21(4), 360-407.
doi: 10.1598/RRQ.21.4.1 URL |
[67] |
Su, Y. E., Naigles, L. R., & Su, L.-Y. (2018). Uneven expressive language development in mandarin-exposed preschool children with ASD: Comparing vocabulary, grammar, and the decontextualized use of language via the PCDI-Toddler Form. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 48(10), 3432-3448. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-018-3614-x
doi: 10.1007/s10803-018-3614-x URL pmid: 29781066 |
[68] |
Suarez-Rivera, C., Smith, L. B., & Yu, C. (2019). Multimodal parent behaviors within joint attention support sustained attention in infants. Developmental Psychology, 55(1), 96-109. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000628
doi: 10.1037/dev0000628 URL pmid: 30489136 |
[69] |
Takashima, A., Bakker, I., van Hell, J. G., Janzen, G., & McQueen, J. M. (2017). Interaction between episodic and semantic memory networks in the acquisition and consolidation of novel spoken words. Brain and Language, 167, 44-60. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2016.05.009
doi: S0093-934X(15)30037-7 URL pmid: 27291335 |
[70] |
Tecoulesco, L., Fein, D., & Naigles, L. R. (2021). What categorical induction variability reveals about typical and atypical development. Journal of Child Language, 48(3), 515-540. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000920000392
doi: 10.1017/S0305000920000392 URL pmid: 33198848 |
[71] |
Tenenbaum, E. J., Amso, D., Abar, B., & Sheinkopf, S. J. (2014). Attention and word learning in autistic, language delayed and typically developing children. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 490. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00490
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00490 URL pmid: 24904503 |
[72] |
Tenenbaum, E. J., Amso, D., Righi, G., & Sheinkopf, S. J. (2017). Attempting to "Increase Intake from the Input": Attention and word learning in children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 47(6), 1791-1805. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-017-3098-0
doi: 10.1007/s10803-017-3098-0 URL pmid: 28342164 |
[73] |
Tomasello, M., & Farrar, M. J. (1986). Joint attention and early language. Child Development, 57(6), 1454-1463.
pmid: 3802971 |
[74] | Tovar, A. E., Rodriguez-Granados, A., & Arias-Trejo, N. (2020). Atypical shape bias and categorization in autism: Evidence from children and computational simulations. Developmental Science, 23(2), e12885. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12885 |
[75] |
Tsang, T., Atagi, N., & Johnson, S. P. (2018). Selective attention to the mouth is associated with expressive language skills in monolingual and bilingual infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 169, 93-109. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2018.01.002
doi: S0022-0965(17)30306-5 URL pmid: 29406126 |
[76] |
Venker, C. E. (2017). Spoken word recognition in children with autism spectrum disorder: The role of visual disengagement. Autism, 21(7), 821-829. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361316653230
doi: 10.1177/1362361316653230 URL pmid: 27335107 |
[77] |
Venker, C. E. (2019). Cross-situational and ostensive word learning in children with and without autism spectrum disorder. Cognition, 183, 181-191. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2018.10.025
doi: S0010-0277(18)30283-X URL pmid: 30468980 |
[78] |
Venker, C. E., Bean, A., & Kover, S. T. (2018). Auditory- visual misalignment: A theoretical perspective on vocabulary delays in children with ASD. Autism Research, 11(12), 1621-1628. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.2038
doi: 10.1002/aur.2038 URL |
[79] |
Venker, C. E., Mathee, J., Neumann, D., Edwards, J., Saffran, J., & Ellis Weismer, S. (2021). Competing perceptual salience in a visual word recognition task differentially affects children with and without autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research, 14(6), 1147-1162. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.2457
doi: 10.1002/aur.2457 URL |
[80] | Vlach, H. A., & DeBrock, C. A. (2019). Statistics learned are statistics forgotten: Children's retention and retrieval of cross-situational word learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 45(4), 700-711. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000611 |
[81] |
Walker, S., Henderson, L. M., Fletcher, F. E., Knowland, V. C. P., Cairney, S. A., & Gaskell, M. G. (2019). Learning to live with interfering neighbours: The influence of time of learning and level of encoding on word learning. Royal Society Open Science., 6(4), 181842. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.181842
doi: 10.1098/rsos.181842 URL |
[82] |
Wang, S., Jiang, M., Duchesne, X. M., Laugeson, E. A., Kennedy, D. P., Adolphs, R., & Zhao, Q. (2015). Atypical visual saliency in autism spectrum disorder quantified through model-based eye tracking. Neuron, 88(3), 604-616. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.042
doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.042 URL pmid: 26593094 |
[83] |
Weighall, A. R., Henderson, L. M., Barr, D. J., Cairney, S. A., & Gaskell, M. G. (2017). Eye-tracking the time-course of novel word learning and lexical competition in adults and children. Brain and Language, 167, 13-27.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2016.07.010
doi: S0093-934X(15)30161-9 URL pmid: 27562102 |
[84] |
Young, G. S., Merin, N., Rogers, S. J., & Ozonoff, S. (2009). Gaze behavior and affect at 6 months: Predicting clinical outcomes and language development in typically developing infants and infants at risk for autism. Developmental Science, 12(5), 798-814. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00833.x
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00833.x URL pmid: 19702771 |
[85] |
Yu, C., & Smith, L. B. (2012). Embodied attention and word learning by toddlers. Cognition, 125(2), 244-262.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2012.06.016
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.06.016 URL pmid: 22878116 |
[86] | Yu, C., Suanda, S. H., & Smith, L. B. (2019). Infant sustained attention but not joint attention to objects at 9 months predicts vocabulary at 12 and 15 months. Developmental Science, 22(1), e12735. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12735 |
[1] | 崔楠, 王久菊, 赵婧. 注意缺陷多动障碍-发展性阅读障碍共患儿童的干预效果及其内在机理[J]. 心理科学进展, 2023, 31(4): 622-630. |
[2] | 张明霞, 李雨欣, 李瑾, 刘勋. 内外动机对青少年记忆的影响及其神经机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2023, 31(1): 1-9. |
[3] | 王松雪, 程思, 蒋挺, 刘勋, 张明霞. 外在奖赏对陈述性记忆的影响[J]. 心理科学进展, 2023, 31(1): 78-86. |
[4] | 赵冰洁, 张琪涵, 陈怡馨, 章鹏, 白学军. 智力运动专家领域内知觉与记忆的加工特点及其机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(9): 1993-2003. |
[5] | 蒋涵. 注意缺陷多动障碍儿童教育康复线上模式的构建[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(8): 1747-1758. |
[6] | 周国梅, 郑若颖, 林佳, 刘心阁. 面孔吸引力的整体表征及其动态性增强[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(7): 1429-1438. |
[7] | 王鑫麟, 邱晓悦, 翁旭初, 杨平. 工作记忆的神经振荡调控:基于神经振荡夹带现象[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(4): 802-816. |
[8] | 李俊娇, 陈伟, 石佩, 董媛媛, 郑希付. 预期错误在恐惧记忆更新中的作用与机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(4): 834-850. |
[9] | 刘彦秀, 谢桐, 傅世敏. 物体表征强度对基于动态物体的注意的影响[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(3): 591-600. |
[10] | 张琳琳, 魏坤琳, 李晶. 儿童的人际运动同步[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(3): 623-634. |
[11] | 刘敏, 胡洋, 刘巧云. 孤独症儿童潜在早期识别标志——发声异常及原因探析[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(3): 635-647. |
[12] | 陈幸明, 付彤, 刘昌, 张宾, 伏云发, 李恩泽, ZHANG Jian, 陈盛强, 党彩萍. 工作记忆训练诱发的神经可塑性——基于系列fMRI实验的脑区分布递减时空模型[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(2): 255-274. |
[13] | 陈玉田, 陈睿, 李鹏. 工作记忆中“组块”概念的演化及理论模型[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(12): 2708-2717. |
[14] | 叶丽群, 谭欣, 姚堃, 丁玉珑. 正常老化对视觉早期注意的影响——来自ERP的证据[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(12): 2746-2763. |
[15] | 张雯, 董亓易如, 龚丽娟, 尚琪, 程琛, 丁雪辰. 运算动量效应的理论解释及其发展性预测因素[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(12): 2777-2788. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||