Loading...
ISSN 0439-755X
CN 11-1911/B

Current Issue

    25 May 2025, Volume 57 Issue 5 Previous Issue    Next Issue

    Reports of Empirical Studies

    For Selected: Toggle Thumbnails
    Reports of Empirical Studies
    Offline memory representations have robustness superiority in visual working memory
    LI Ziyuan, REN Guofang, YUAN Zixin, YU Qingqing, WU Yue, LIU Qiang
    2025, 57 (5):  739-748.  doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2025.0739
    Abstract ( 145 )   HTML ( 27 )  
    PDF (317KB) ( 62 )  
    Figures and Tables | References | Related Articles | Metrics
    How magnitude and ordinal cues of stimuli influence the SNARC effect
    WANG Qiangqiang, WU Yanwen, SHI Wendian, YOU Xuqun
    2025, 57 (5):  749-761.  doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2025.0749
    Abstract ( 86 )   HTML ( 6 )  
    PDF (675KB) ( 62 )  

    The Spatial-Numerical Association of Response Codes (SNARC) effect is prevalent in various forms of number and numerical magnitude stimuli processing, but the mechanism by which the magnitude and order cues in the stimuli affect the SNARC effect is still unclear. Herein, a letter magnitude activation task was designed to systematically investigate this mechanism and the impacts of magnitude and order cues on the SNARC effect. After various numbers of letters were continuously presented to activate the letters’ magnitude information, the participants were asked to perform the letter order classification task (Experiment 1), the letter magnitude parity classification task (Experiment 2) and the letter color classification task (Experiment 3). The results showed that: (1) The magnitude SNARC effect and the order SNARC effect occurred simultaneously in the letter order classification task (Experiment 1), but their interaction was not significant. (2) In the parity classification task of letter magnitude (Experiment 2), this interaction was significant, which was manifested in the phenomenon where the magnitude SNARC effect appeared only under the condition of ordinal-spatial consistency, and the order SNARC effect appeared only under the condition of numerical-spatial consistency. (3) In the letter color classification task (Experiment 3), only the order SNARC effect appeared without the magnitude SNARC effect, and their interaction was not significant. The above indicated that: (1) The SNARC effect can be caused by both the magnitude cues and the order cues of the stimuli. (2) The magnitude and order cues had different influencing mechanisms on the SNARC effect and showed the task context dependence. These findings support and improve the magnitude ordinality dual action theory.

    Figures and Tables | References | Related Articles | Metrics
    The cognitive neural mechanism of deinhibition: An ERP study
    CHEN Jiwen, MAI Xiaoqin, LI Fuhong
    2025, 57 (5):  762-774.  doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2025.0762
    Abstract ( 48 )   HTML ( 3 )  
    PDF (556KB) ( 23 )  
    Figures and Tables | References | Related Articles | Metrics
    The Self-reference Effect in Prospective Memory and Its Cognitive Mechanisms in Elementary School Students Aged 7 to 11 Years
    XIN Cong, CHEN Youzhen, TIAN Mi, LIU Guoxiong
    2025, 57 (5):  775-791.  doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2025.0775
    Abstract ( 117 )   HTML ( 13 )  
    PDF (993KB) ( 88 )  

    This study employed the ownership paradigm for the first time to investigate the existence of the self-reference effect (SRE) in prospective memory (PM) among elementary school students aged 7 to 11. Through three experiments, the findings revealed that: (1) Regardless of whether ownership was actively selected (Experiment 1) or passively assigned (Experiment 2), elementary school students exhibited the SRE in PM, with older students demonstrating a stronger effect. (2) Under a high cognitive load condition, the SRE in PM was absent in the salient and non-salient target conditions among elementary school students. Conversely, under a low cognitive load condition, the SRE was observed in the non-salient PM targets but not in the salient targets. Additionally, individuals flexibly allocated cognitive resources before and after the appearance of PM targets in self- and other-referenced conditions based on task demands. When cognitive resources were insufficient, individuals prioritized self-reference PM targets (Experiment 3). In conclusion, a stable SRE can be observed in the PM of elementary school students when employing the ownership paradigm, with large effects in the upper elementary school grades. The generation of the SRE in PM requires cognitive resources, which are deployed flexibly based on task demands and showcase a dynamic processing characteristic. The finding supports the notion that the SRE in PM involves cognitive resource-intensive dynamic processing, which can further enrich the dynamic processing theory of PM.

    Figures and Tables | References | Related Articles | Metrics
    The effects of rumination on social anxiety: The role of negative self-beliefs
    GENG Li, FENG Qiuyang, LI Yu, QIU Jiang
    2025, 57 (5):  792-804.  doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2025.0792
    Abstract ( 138 )   HTML ( 11 )  
    PDF (256KB) ( 79 )  
    Figures and Tables | References | Related Articles | Metrics
    Interactive Feedback in persuader-persuadee interaction Enhances Persuasion: An fNIRS hyperscanning study
    LI Yangzhuo, ZHANG Ruqian, SONG Sensen, LI Xianchun, LUO Junlong
    2025, 57 (5):  805-819.  doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2025.0805
    Abstract ( 83 )   HTML ( 5 )  
    PDF (4689KB) ( 60 )  

    Persuasion is a fundamental social skill in humans, essential for information propagation and the social influence. Existing research has primarily focused on the aspects of persuasive content, neglecting the interactive dynamics inherent to persuasion. The present study complemented by functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) to examines the effects of interactive feedback on interpersonal persuasion and its underlying neurocognitive mechanisms, employing a dyadic persuasion paradigm with high ecological validity. Behavioral results indicate that, relative to the no-interactive-feedback condition, interactive feedback condition enhanced both persuasion outcomes and perceived persuasiveness. In terms of neural activity, results revealed that interactive feedback enhanced brain-to-brain synchrony between the persuader and persuadee in the right frontal cortex and left temporoparietal regions, with this synchrony positively predicting persuasion outcomes. Granger causality further elucidated the directional characteristics of brain-to-brain synchrony, offering insights into the interaction patterns between the persuader and persuadee during the persuasive process. Regarding the employ of persuasion strategies, persuaders in the interactive feedback condition employed refutational strategies more frequently than supportive strategies. Moreover, brain-to-brain synchrony during the refutational strategy was significantly higher than during the supportive strategy. Furthermore, brain-to-brain synchrony gradually increased following, rather than preceding, the employment of the refutational strategy. This finding underscores the importance of refutational strategies in enhancing persuasion, brain-to-brain synchrony between the persuader’s left superior temporal gyrus and the persuadee’s right frontal gyrus effectively tracking and identifying the use of this strategy. In summary, the present study offers a novel perspective on how interactive feedback enhances interpersonal persuasion, contributing to a deeper understanding of the complicate and authentic processes of persuasion through interpersonal neuroscience.

    Figures and Tables | References | Related Articles | Metrics
    Threshold Effects of Distinctiveness: Psychological Mechanisms Underlying Group Identity
    KE Wenlin, WEN Fangfang, ZUO Bin
    2025, 57 (5):  820-837.  doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2025.0820
    Abstract ( 67 )   HTML ( 5 )  
    PDF (1304KB) ( 15 )  

    Finding a balance between the self and the group is a central issue in human social life. Within the context of intergroup comparison, the Optimal Distinctiveness Model proposes an inverted U-shaped relationship between distinctiveness and group identification. However, how individuals balance the self and the group in an intragroup context remains unclear. Through three experiments, this paper systematically explores the impact of self-group distinctiveness on group identification across different levels of distinctiveness and group attributes. The findings reveal that, overall, distinctiveness exerts a non-linear, inverted S-shaped negative effect on group identification, with a distinctiveness threshold at the moderate level where the decline in group identification is most pronounced. Once distinctiveness surpasses this threshold, individuals exhibit complete disidentification from the group (Experiment 1). Both the level and nature of distinctiveness moderate this threshold effect (Experiment 1, 2, and 3). Additionally, intragroup cognitive dissonance mediates this relationship, whereby distinctiveness positively predicts intragroup cognitive dissonance, which in turn negatively predicts group identification (Experiment 3). The identification threshold identified in this study not only aids individuals in maintaining psychological boundaries but also serves as an effective warning signal for organizational management and social harmony.

    Figures and Tables | References | Related Articles | Metrics
    Too pretty to be healthy? The influence of food aesthetics on consumer perception of food healthiness
    ZHENG Xiaoying, ZHENG Lijing, LIU Chunlei, HAN Han
    2025, 57 (5):  833-833.  doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2025.0883
    Abstract ( 74 )   HTML ( 13 )  
    PDF (151KB) ( 29 )  
    Figures and Tables | References | Related Articles | Metrics
    The impact of residential mobility on consumers’ preference for feasible products
    WANG Lili, SU Xiao, LIANG Keyin
    2025, 57 (5):  896-914.  doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2025.0896
    Abstract ( 45 )   HTML ( 7 )  
    PDF (325KB) ( 43 )  
    Figures and Tables | References | Related Articles | Metrics
    Moderated Mediation Analyses of Intensive Longitudinal Data
    FANG Jie, WEN Zhonglin, WANG Huihui, GU Honglei
    2025, 57 (5):  915-928.  doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2025.0915
    Abstract ( 68 )   HTML ( 3 )  
    PDF (338KB) ( 19 )  

    Intensive longitudinal data (ILD) is increasing in fields such as psychology and management, yet research on analytical methods for ILD remains relatively scant. Traditionally, the ILD is statistically modeled as a two-level structure, with Level 1 being the time and Level 2 being individuals. Existing analytical methods treat longitudinal moderated mediation as multilevel moderated mediation, without considering the lagged relationship between variables. This paper describes in detail how to construct four intensive longitudinal moderated mediation (ILMM) models with dynamic structural equation model (DSEM). A simulation study is conducted to examine the estimation accuracy of the 1-1-1 intensive longitudinal mediation model moderated by a level 2 moderator. An example is employed to demonstrate how to conduct ILMM analysis with DSEM by Mplus. Mplus codes for analyzing all these ILMM models are provided (The complete dataset, Mplus syntax files, and analysis outputs can be downloaded at https://osf.io/e273c/).

    Figures and Tables | References | Related Articles | Metrics