Acta Psychologica Sinica ›› 2025, Vol. 57 ›› Issue (5): 749-761.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2025.0749
• Reports of Empirical Studies • Previous Articles Next Articles
WANG Qiangqiang1, WU Yanwen2, SHI Wendian3(), YOU Xuqun4(
)
Received:
2023-08-02
Published:
2025-05-25
Online:
2025-03-06
Contact:
SHI Wendian,YOU Xuqun
E-mail:swd_nx@163.com;youxuqun@snnu.edu.cn
Supported by:
WANG Qiangqiang, WU Yanwen, SHI Wendian, YOU Xuqun. (2025). How magnitude and ordinal cues of stimuli influence the SNARC effect. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 57(5), 749-761.
Add to citation manager EndNote|Ris|BibTeX
URL: https://journal.psych.ac.cn/acps/EN/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2025.0749
Figure 1. Flowchart of the experiment. The figure depicts the flowchart for a single trial with four consecutive presentations of the letter B as the example. All screens were presented for 500 ms, except for the target stimulus (letter B on the next screen after fixation point ‘+’), which was presented for 3000 ms, and the last screen, which was presented for 1500 ms.
Figure 2. Magnitude and order SNARC effects in letter order classification. Error bars represent standard errors. * denotes p < 0.05. ** denotes p < 0.01. *** denotes p < 0.001. Error bars and asterisks in all subsequent figures in this study represent the same meaning as in this figure.
Figure 3. Error rates and standard errors on magnitude-spatial and ordinal-spatial consistency and inconsistency trials in letter order classification task.
Figure 6. Error rates and standard errors in magnitude-spatial and ordinal-spatial consistency and inconsistency trials in the letter magnitude classification task.
Figure 8. Error rates and standard errors of magnitude-spatial and ordinal-spatial consistency and inconsistency trials in letter color classification task.
[1] |
Abrahamse, E., van Dijck, J. P., & Fias, W. (2016). How does working memory enable number-induced spatial biases? Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 977. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00977.
pmid: 27445937 |
[2] |
Abrahamse, E., van Dijck, J. P., Majerus, S., & Fias, W. (2014). Finding the answer in space: The mental whiteboard hypothesis on serial order in working memory. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 932. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00932.
pmid: 25505394 |
[3] | Bueti, D., & Walsh, V. (2009). The parietal cortex and the representation of time, space, number and other magnitudes. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364(1525), 1831-1840. |
[4] | Casasanto, D., & Pitt, B. (2019). The faulty magnitude detector: why SNARC-like tasks cannot support a generalized magnitude system. Cognitive Science, 43, e12794. doi: 10.1111/cogs.12794. |
[5] | Cleland, A. A., Corsico, K., White, K., & Bull, R. (2020). Non-symbolic numerosities do not automatically activate spatial-numerical associations: Evidence from the SNARC effect. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 73(2), 295-308. |
[6] | Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122(3), 371-396. |
[7] | Dodd, M. D., Van der Stigchel, S., Leghari, M. A., Fung, G., & Kingstone, A. (2008). Attentional SNARC: There’s something special about numbers (let us count the ways). Cognition, 108, 810-818. |
[8] |
Dou, K., Nie, Y., Wang, Y., Li, J., & Shen, W. (2014). Ego depletion promotes impulsive decision: Evidences from behavioral and ERPs studies. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 46(10), 1564-1579.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2014.01564 |
[9] | Fumarola, A., Prpic, V., Pos, O. D., Murgia, M., Umiltà, C., & Agostini, T. (2014). Automatic spatial association for luminance. Attention Perception & Psychophysics, 76(3), 759-765. |
[10] | Gao, Z., Shu, R., Chen, J., Chen, W., Tian, Y., & Shen, M. (2009). The mechanism of negative numbers’ spatial representation. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 41(2), 95-102. |
[11] | Gevers, W., Reynvoet, B., & Fias, W. (2003). The mental representation of ordinal sequences is spatially organized. Cognition, 87(3), B87-B95. |
[12] |
Gevers, W., Reynvoet, B., & Fias, W. (2004). The mental representation of ordinal sequences is spatially organized: Evidence from days of the week. Cortex, 40(1), 171-172.
pmid: 15174454 |
[13] | Ginsburg, V., & Gevers, W. (2015). Spatial coding of ordinal information in short-and long-term memory. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 8. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00008 |
[14] | Han, M., Mao, X., Cai, M., Jia, X., & Guo, C. (2017). The effect of positive and negative signs on the SNARC effect in the magnitude judgment task. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 49(8), 995-1008. |
[15] | Hu, L., & Xiong, Z. (2013). Unconscious spacial representation of physical size and cover area. Journal of Psychological Science, 36(6), 1369-1374. |
[16] |
Huber, S., Klein, E., Moeller, K., & Willmes, K. (2016). Spatial-numerical and ordinal positional associations coexist in parallel. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 438. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00438
pmid: 27064216 |
[17] | Kong, F., Zhao, J., & You, X. (2012). Components representation of negative numbers: Evidence from auditory stimuli detection and number classification tasks. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(4), 691-701. |
[18] |
Li, J. (2013). The mechanism of why self-control resources and cognitive resources influence each other: An integrated model. Advances in Psychological Science, 21(2), 235-242.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2013.00235 |
[19] | Liu, C., Mai, X., & Fu, X. (2004). Spatial numerical association of response codes effect of number processing in different attention conditions. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 36(6), 671-680. |
[20] | Nan, W., Yan, L., Yang, G., Liu, X., & Fu, S. (2022). Two processing stages of the SNARC effect. Psychological Research, 86(2), 375-385. |
[21] | Nuerk, H. C., Wood, G., & Willmes, K. (2005). The universal SNARC effect. Experimental Psychology, 52(3), 187-194. |
[22] | Pan, Y., Shen, D., & Wang, J. (2009). The spatial numerical association of response codes effect of Chinese character number processing in different attention referential cues. Studies of Psychology and Behavior, 7(1), 21-26. |
[23] |
Previtali, P., de Hevia, M. D., & Girelli, L. (2010). Placing order in space: The SNARC effect in serial learning. Experimental Brain Research, 201(3), 599-605.
doi: 10.1007/s00221-009-2063-3 pmid: 19888566 |
[24] | Prpic, V., Basamh, Y. A., Goodridge, C. M., Agostini, T., & Murgia, M. (2023). Contrasting symbolic and non-symbolic numerical representations in a joint classification task. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 30(4), 1422-1430. |
[25] |
Prpic, V., Fumarola, A., De Tommaso, M., Luccio, R., Murgia, M., & Agostini, T. (2016). Separate mechanisms for magnitude and order processing in the spatial-numerical association of response codes (SNARC) effect: The strange case of musical note values. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42(8), 1241-1251.
doi: 10.1037/xhp0000217 pmid: 26950384 |
[26] | Prpic, V., Mingolo, S., Agostini, T., & Murgia, M. (2021). Magnitude and order are both relevant in SNARC and SNARC‐like effects: A commentary on Casasanto and Pitt (2019). Cognitive Science, 45(7), e13006. doi: 10.1111/cogs.13006 |
[27] | Richter, M., & Wühr, P. (2023). Spatial-numerical associations of manual response codes are strongly asymmetrical. Cognition, 238, 105538. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2023.105538. |
[28] | Schliephake, A., Bahnmueller, J., Willmes, K., Koch, I., & Moeller, K. (2023). Influences of cognitive control on number processing: New evidence from switching between two numerical tasks. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 76(11), 2514-2523. |
[29] | Shen, J., He, H., Wu, B., & Zhou, J. (2023). Influence of different spatial representations on the SNARC effect for letters: Electrophysiological evidence. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 76(11), 2613-2628. |
[30] | Shi, W., Wang, Q., Zhao, F., & Zhao, X. (2020). The spatial coding mechanism of ordinal symbols: A study based on the ordinal position effect. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 82(3), 1051-1062. |
[31] | Si, J., Zhou, C., Zhang, C., & Zhong, L. (2013). SNARC Effect of Non-Symbolic Number Information on Different Processing Levels: Eye tracking Evidence. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 45(1), 11-22. |
[32] | Sternberg, S. (1969). The discovery of processing stages: extensions of donders' method. Acta Psychologica, 30, 276-315. |
[33] | van Dijck, J. P., & Fias, W. (2011). A working memory account for spatial-numerical associations. Cognition, 119(1), 114-119. |
[34] |
Walsh, V. (2003). A theory of magnitude: Common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(11), 483-488.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2003.09.002 pmid: 14585444 |
[35] | Wang, Q., An, B., Yue, H., Tao, W., & Shi, W. (2021). Interaction mechanism between location and sequence in letter cognition. Acta Psychologica, 217, 103329. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2021.103329 |
[36] | Wang, Q., Chen, Y., Liu, S., Zhang, W., Ma, Y., & Wu, Y. (2023). The SNARC effect for nonsymbolic numbers is not observed when stimuli spatially orient attention. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 19(3), 261-272. |
[37] |
Wang, Q., Liu, M., Shi, W., & Kang, J. (2018). Mechanism of the SNARC effect in numerical magnitude, time sequence, and spatial sequence tasks: Involvement of LTM and WM. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1558. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01558.
pmid: 30186215 |
[38] | Wang, Q., Liu, S., Zhang, P., Wang, N., & Jin, G. (2022). Rotation angle and task demands influence encoding of ordinal stimuli in the ordinal stimuli's location activated context. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 18(3), 215-225. |
[39] | Wang, Q., Nie, T., Zhang, W., & Shi, W. (2019). The mechanism of the ordinal position effect: stability across sense modalities and the hands crossed context. I-perception, 10(2), 1-16. doi: 10.1177/2041669519841071. |
[40] | Wang, Q., Wu, Y., Zhang, F., Tao, W., & Wang, C. (2022). Processing directional information in stimuli inhibits the spatial association of luminance levels. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 84(8), 2596-2606. |
[41] |
Wang, Q., Zhang, Q., Shi, W., Wang, Z., & Zhang, P. (2022). Online construction of spatial representation of numbers: Evidence from the SNARC effect in number processing in interferential situations. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 54(7), 761-771.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2022.00761 |
[42] | Wang, Z., Zhu, X., & Jiang, Y. (2020). Influence of number location on the SNARC effect: Evidence from the processing of rotated traditional Chinese numerical words. I-Perception, 11(2), 1-18. doi: 10.1177/2041669520917169. |
[43] | Zhang, G., Cao, M., & Qiu, Y. (2017). Review of self-control induced ego depletion. Psychology: Techniques and Applications, 5(6), 353-358. |
[44] |
Zhang, M., Gao, X., Li, B., Yu, S., Gong, T., Jiang, T., Hu, Q., Chen, Y. (2016). Spatial representation of ordinal information. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 505. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00505.
pmid: 27092100 |
[1] | JIANG Jiali, QI Yue, LEI Xiuya, LU Lifei, YU Xiao. The development of symbolic and non-symbolic SNARC effects: The roles of phonological abilities, visuospatial abilities and working memory [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2024, 56(6): 714-730. |
[2] | WANG Chengcheng, ZHAO Yufei, SHENG Yingying, ZHAO Qingbai, XIAO Mengshi, HAN Lei. Occurrence stage of SNARC effect [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2024, 56(12): 1706-1717. |
[3] | JIANG Luyao, LI Bingbing. Subliminal speech priming of Mandarin Chinese: Evidence from auditory masked priming paradigm [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2023, 55(4): 529-541. |
[4] | WANG Qiangqiang, ZHANG Qi, SHI Wendian, WANG Zhiwei, ZHANG Pengcheng. Online construction of spatial representation of numbers: Evidence from the SNARC effect in number processing in interferential situations [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2022, 54(7): 761-771. |
[5] | YANG Jimei, CHAI Jieyu, QIU Tianlong, QUAN Xiaoshan, ZHENG Maoping. Relationship between empathy and emotion recognition in Chinese national music: An event-related potential study evidence [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2022, 54(10): 1181-1192. |
[6] | ZUO Bin, DAI Yuee, WEN Fangfang, GAO Jia, XIE Zhijie, HE Saifei. “You were what you eat”: Food-gender stereotypes and their impact on evaluation of impression [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2021, 53(3): 259-272. |
[7] | HAN Meng, MAO Xinrui, CAI Mengtong, JIA Xi, GUO Chunyan. The effect of positive and negative signs on the SNARC effect in the magnitude judgment task [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2017, 49(8): 995-1008. |
[8] | SUN Yueliang, ZHENG Weiyi, HE Xianyou. The processing of pure decimal numbers: Selective access or parallel access? [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2017, 49(5): 611-621. |
[9] | SI Jiwei;ZHOU Chao;ZHANG Chuanhua;ZHONG Leilei. SNARC Effect of Non-Symbolic Number Information on Different Processing Levels: Eyetracking Evidence [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2013, 45(1): 11-22. |
[10] | WANG Pei;HUO Peng-Fei;WANG Ling-Hui. Processing Levels and Its Prerequisite During Subliminal Perception: Evidences Based on Visual Masking Priming [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2012, 44(9): 1138-1148. |
[11] | ZHANG Li;CHEN Xue-Mei;WANG Qi;LI Hong. The Influence of Body Form and Social Environment on the SNARC Effect: Based on the Embodied Cognition [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2012, 44(10): 1309-1317. |
[12] | REN Jie,WATANABE Kazuhiko,MIYATANI Makoto. The Effect of Cognitive Task on Visual Control of Standing Posture [J]. , 2010, 42(03): 360-366. |
[13] | GAO Zai-Feng,SHUI Ren-De,CHEN Jing,CHEN Wen,TIAN Ying,,SHEN Mo-Wei. The Mechanism of Negative Numbers’ Spatial Representation [J]. , 2009, 41(02): 95-102. |
[14] | Zhang Qingfang. Phonological Encoding in Monosyllabic and Bisyllabic Mandarin Word Production: Implicit Priming Paradigm Study [J]. , 2008, 40(03): 253-262. |
[15] | Zhu Xiangru,Liu Chang. Conflict Adaptation under the SNARC Effect: An ERP Study [J]. , 2008, 40(03): 283-290. |
Viewed | ||||||
Full text |
|
|||||
Abstract |
|
|||||