心理学报 ›› 2022, Vol. 54 ›› Issue (2): 111-121.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2022.00111
• 研究报告 • 下一篇
收稿日期:
2020-11-10
发布日期:
2021-12-24
出版日期:
2022-02-25
通讯作者:
罗文波
E-mail:luowb@lnnu.edu.cn
基金资助:
Received:
2020-11-10
Online:
2021-12-24
Published:
2022-02-25
Contact:
LUO Wenbo
E-mail:luowb@lnnu.edu.cn
摘要:
具体性和抽象性是词汇同一特性的两极, 是直接影响词汇加工过程的重要因素。本研究采用快速序列视觉呈现范式, 结合脑电技术探讨在有限注意资源内, 词汇具体性对情绪名词加工过程的影响。结果发现:名词加工早期和晚期阶段的ERP成分受到情绪效价的调节, 情绪词比中性词诱发了更大的N170和LPC波幅; 名词加工早期和晚期阶段的ERP成分也受到词汇具体性的调节, 具体词比抽象词诱发了更大的N170和LPC波幅; 词汇具体性影响情绪名词加工的晚期阶段, LPC波幅能够分辨出不同情绪效价的抽象词, 对于具体词仅能区分出情绪与非情绪, 这说明抽象词可能比具体词负载了更多的情绪信息, 反映出对情绪信息的精细加工过程。
中图分类号:
罗文波, 齐正阳. (2022). 词汇具体性对情绪名词效价加工影响的ERP研究. 心理学报, 54(2), 111-121.
LUO Wenbo, QI Zhengyang. (2022). The influence of concreteness on emotional nouns valence processing: An ERP study. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 54(2), 111-121.
统计量 | 具体词 | 抽象词 | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
正性(M ± SD) | 中性(M ± SD) | 负性(M ± SD) | 正性(M ± SD) | 中性(M ± SD) | 负性(M ± SD) | |
唤醒度 | 4.90 ± 0.45 | 4.56 ± 0.81 | 5.05 ± 0.57 | 5.07 ± 0.28 | 5.04 ± 0.81 | 5.36 ± 0.40 |
效价 | 6.82 ± 0.38 | 5.21 ± 0.50 | 2.70 ± 0.38 | 6.81 ± 0.18 | 5.36 ± 0.31 | 2.99 ± 0.28 |
具体性 | 4.12 ± 0.16 | 4.11 ± 0.41 | 4.19 ± 0.15 | 2.56 ± 0.09 | 2.62 ± 0.11 | 2.51 ± 0.21 |
整词笔画 | 19.67 ± 2.07 | 19.17 ± 3.92 | 18.33 ± 2.34 | 20.33 ± 3.83 | 18.83 ± 1.33 | 19.67 ± 3.93 |
首字笔画 | 10.67 ± 2.58 | 10.83 ± 4.75 | 8.50 ± 3.33 | 8.00 ± 2.19 | 8.67 ± 3.20 | 9.83 ± 2.93 |
尾字笔画 | 9.00 ± 2.97 | 8.33 ± 4.97 | 9.83 ± 3.06 | 12.33 ± 3.01 | 10.17 ± 2.32 | 9.83 ± 2.64 |
整词频度(对数) | 3.89 ± 0.52 | 3.54 ± 0.71 | 3.96 ± 0.39 | 3.79 ± 0.59 | 4.23 ± 0.25 | 3.92 ± 0.29 |
首字频度(对数) | 7.80 ± 1.18 | 5.90 ± 1.76 | 6.72 ± 2.01 | 7.48 ± 1.27 | 7.71 ± 1.90 | 5.47 ± 0.86 |
尾字频度(对数) | 6.85 ± 2.63 | 7.53 ± 1.22 | 6.31 ± 1.50 | 6.79 ± 1.55 | 7.14 ± 1.45 | 6.25 ± 0.88 |
表1 具体词和抽象词的唤醒度、效价、具体性、词频、笔画参数
统计量 | 具体词 | 抽象词 | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
正性(M ± SD) | 中性(M ± SD) | 负性(M ± SD) | 正性(M ± SD) | 中性(M ± SD) | 负性(M ± SD) | |
唤醒度 | 4.90 ± 0.45 | 4.56 ± 0.81 | 5.05 ± 0.57 | 5.07 ± 0.28 | 5.04 ± 0.81 | 5.36 ± 0.40 |
效价 | 6.82 ± 0.38 | 5.21 ± 0.50 | 2.70 ± 0.38 | 6.81 ± 0.18 | 5.36 ± 0.31 | 2.99 ± 0.28 |
具体性 | 4.12 ± 0.16 | 4.11 ± 0.41 | 4.19 ± 0.15 | 2.56 ± 0.09 | 2.62 ± 0.11 | 2.51 ± 0.21 |
整词笔画 | 19.67 ± 2.07 | 19.17 ± 3.92 | 18.33 ± 2.34 | 20.33 ± 3.83 | 18.83 ± 1.33 | 19.67 ± 3.93 |
首字笔画 | 10.67 ± 2.58 | 10.83 ± 4.75 | 8.50 ± 3.33 | 8.00 ± 2.19 | 8.67 ± 3.20 | 9.83 ± 2.93 |
尾字笔画 | 9.00 ± 2.97 | 8.33 ± 4.97 | 9.83 ± 3.06 | 12.33 ± 3.01 | 10.17 ± 2.32 | 9.83 ± 2.64 |
整词频度(对数) | 3.89 ± 0.52 | 3.54 ± 0.71 | 3.96 ± 0.39 | 3.79 ± 0.59 | 4.23 ± 0.25 | 3.92 ± 0.29 |
首字频度(对数) | 7.80 ± 1.18 | 5.90 ± 1.76 | 6.72 ± 2.01 | 7.48 ± 1.27 | 7.71 ± 1.90 | 5.47 ± 0.86 |
尾字频度(对数) | 6.85 ± 2.63 | 7.53 ± 1.22 | 6.31 ± 1.50 | 6.79 ± 1.55 | 7.14 ± 1.45 | 6.25 ± 0.88 |
正性词 | 中性词 | 负性词 | |
---|---|---|---|
具体词 | 鲜花 笑脸 首饰 绿地 新房 别墅 | 总管 护照 赛场 警卫 狮子 邮票 | 苍蝇 棺材 老鼠 遗体 病房 太监 |
抽象词 | 信誉 美德 名声 诚意 深情 乐趣 | 廉价 对策 焦点 国情 视野 江湖 | 耻辱 漏洞 创伤 隐患 虚荣 困境 |
附表1 实验中选用的情绪名词
正性词 | 中性词 | 负性词 | |
---|---|---|---|
具体词 | 鲜花 笑脸 首饰 绿地 新房 别墅 | 总管 护照 赛场 警卫 狮子 邮票 | 苍蝇 棺材 老鼠 遗体 病房 太监 |
抽象词 | 信誉 美德 名声 诚意 深情 乐趣 | 廉价 对策 焦点 国情 视野 江湖 | 耻辱 漏洞 创伤 隐患 虚荣 困境 |
[1] |
Barber, H. A., Otten, L. J., Kousta, S. T., & Vigliocco, G. (2013). Concreteness in word processing: ERP and behavioral effects in a lexical decision task. Brain and Language, 125(1), 47-53.
doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2013.01.005 URL |
[2] |
Barca, L., Burani, C., & Arduino, L. S. (2002). Word naming times and psycholinguistic norms for Italian nouns. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 34(3), 424-434.
doi: 10.3758/BF03195471 URL |
[3] |
Bayer, M., Sommer, W., & Schacht, A. (2012). P1 and beyond: Functional separation of multiple emotion effects in word recognition. Psychophysiology, 49(7), 959-969.
doi: 10.1111/psyp.2012.49.issue-7 URL |
[4] |
Bechtold, L., Ghio, M., & Bellebaum, C. (2018). The effect of training-induced visual imageability on electrophysiological correlates of novel word processing. Biomedicines, 6(3), 75.
doi: 10.3390/biomedicines6030075 URL |
[5] |
Bonin, P., Méot, A., & Bugaiska, A. (2018). Concreteness norms for 1, 659 French words: Relationships with other psycholinguistic variables and word recognition times. Behavior Research Methods, 50(6), 2366-2387.
doi: 10.3758/s13428-018-1014-y URL |
[6] |
Borghi, A. M., Binkofski, F., Castelfranchi, C., Cimatti, F., Scorolli, C., & Tummolini, L. (2017). The challenge of abstract concepts. Psychological Bulletin, 143(3), 263-292.
doi: 10.1037/bul0000089 pmid: 28095000 |
[7] |
Citron, F. M., Weekes, B. S., & Ferstl, E. C. (2013). Effects of valence and arousal on written word recognition: Time course and ERP correlates. Neuroscience Letters, 533, 90-95.
doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2012.10.054 URL |
[8] |
Crawford, L. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2002). Learning where to look for danger: Integrating affective and spatial information. Psychological Science, 13(5), 449-453.
doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00479 URL |
[9] |
Dalla Volta, R., Fabbri-Destro, M., Gentilucci, M., & Avanzini, P. (2014). Spatiotemporal dynamics during processing of abstract and concrete verbs: An ERP study. Neuropsychologia, 61, 163-174.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.06.019 URL |
[10] | Delaney-Busch, N., Wilkie, G., & Kuperberg, G. (2016). Vivid: How valence and arousal influence word processing under different task demands. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 16(3), 415-432. |
[11] |
Doyle, C. M., & Lindquist, K. A. (2018). When a word is worth a thousand pictures: Language shapes perceptual memory for emotion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 147(1), 62-73.
doi: 10.1037/xge0000361 URL |
[12] |
Fan, C., Chen, S. S., Zhang, L. C., Qi, Z. Y., Jin, Y. L., Wang, Q., ... Luo, W. B. (2015). N170 changes reflect competition between faces and identifiable characters during early visual processing. NeuroImage, 110, 32-38.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.01.047 URL |
[13] | Gratton, G., & Coles, M. G. H. (1989). Generalization and evaluation of eye-movement correction procedures. Journal of Psychophysiology, 3, 14-16. |
[14] |
Hajcak, G., MacNamara, A., & Olvet, D. M. (2010). Event-related potentials, emotion, and emotion regulation: An integrative review. Developmental Neuropsychology, 35(2), 129-155.
doi: 10.1080/87565640903526504 pmid: 20390599 |
[15] |
Herbert, C., Junghofer, M., & Kissler, J. (2008). Event related potentials to emotional adjectives during reading. Psychophysiology, 45(3), 487-498.
doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00638.x URL |
[16] |
Hinojosa, J. A., Albert, J., López-Martín, S., & Carretié, L. (2014). Temporospatial analysis of explicit and implicit processing of negative content during word comprehension. Brain and Cognition, 87, 109-121.
doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2014.03.008 pmid: 24732955 |
[17] |
Hinojosa, J. A., Méndez‐Bértolo, C., & Pozo, M. A. (2010). Looking at emotional words is not the same as reading emotional words: Behavioral and neural correlates. Psychophysiology, 47(4), 748-757.
doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.00982.x pmid: 20158677 |
[18] | Hofmann, M. J., Kuchinke, L., Tamm, S., Võ, M. L., & Jacobs, A. M. (2009). Affective processing within 1/10th of a second: High arousal is necessary for early facilitative processing of negative but not positive words. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 9( 4), 389-397. |
[19] |
Kanske, P., & Kotz, S. A. (2007). Concreteness in emotional words: ERP evidence from a hemifield study. Brain Research, 1148, 138-148.
doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.02.044 URL |
[20] |
Kim, J., Schultz, J., Rohe, T., Wallraven, C., Lee, S. W., & Bülthoff, H. H. (2015). Abstract representations of associated emotions in the human brain. Journal of Neuroscience, 35(14), 5655-5663.
doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4059-14.2015 URL |
[21] | Kissler, J., Assadollahi, R., & Herbert, C. (2006). Emotional and semantic networks in visual word processing: Insights from ERP studies. Progress in Brain Research, 156, 147-183. |
[22] |
Kissler, J., & Herbert, C. (2013). Emotion, Etmnooi, or Emitoon? - Faster lexical access to emotional than to neutral words during reading. Biological Psychology, 92(3), 464-479.
doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.09.004 URL |
[23] |
Kousta, S. T., Vigliocco, G., Vinson, D. P., Andrews, M., & Del Campo, E. (2011). The representation of abstract words: Why emotion matters. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140(1), 14-34.
doi: 10.1037/a0021446 URL |
[24] |
Kousta, S. T., Vinson, D. P., & Vigliocco, G. (2009). Emotion words, regardless of polarity, have a processing advantage over neutral words. Cognition, 112(3), 473-481.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.06.007 URL |
[25] | Liu, Y., Liang, N. Y., Wang, D. J., Zhang, S. Y., Yang, T. Y., Jie, C, Y., & Sun, W.(Eds). (1990). Dictionary of usage frequency of modern Chinese words. Beijing, China: Yuhang Press. |
[ 刘源, 梁南元, 王德进, 张社英, 杨铁鹰, 揭春雨, 孙伟. (编). (1990). 现代汉语常用词词频词典. 北京: 宇航出版社. ] | |
[26] |
Luo, W. B., Feng, W. F., He, W. Q., Wang, N. Y., & Luo, Y. J. (2010). Three stages of facial expression processing: ERP study with rapid serial visual presentation. NeuroImage, 49(2), 1857-1867.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.09.018 URL |
[27] | Paivio, A . (1986). Mental representations: A dual coding approach. New York: Oxford Psychology Press. |
[28] |
Paivio, A. (1991). Dual coding theory: Retrospect and current status. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 45(3), 255-287.
doi: 10.1037/h0084295 URL |
[29] |
Palazova, M., Mantwill, K., Sommer, W., & Schacht, A. (2011). Are effects of emotion in single words non-lexical? Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Neuropsychologia, 49(9), 2766-2775.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.06.005 pmid: 21684295 |
[30] |
Peng, Y. B., Liu, Y., & Guo, C. Y. (2019). Examining the neural mechanism behind testing effect with concrete and abstract words. NeuroReport, 30(2), 113-119.
doi: 10.1097/WNR.0000000000001169 URL |
[31] |
Ponari, M., Norbury, C. F., & Vigliocco, G. (2017). Acquisition of abstract concepts is influenced by emotional valence. Developmental Science, 21(2), e12549.
doi: 10.1111/desc.12549 URL |
[32] |
Raymond, J. E., Shapiro, K. L., & Arnell, K. M. (1992). Temporary suppression of visual processing in an RSVP task: An attentional blink? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18(3), 849-860.
doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.18.3.849 URL |
[33] |
Rossion, B., Joyce, C. A., Cottrell, G. W., & Tarr, M. J. (2003). Early lateralization and orientation tuning for face, word, and object processing in the visual cortex. NeuroImage, 20(3), 1609-1624.
pmid: 14642472 |
[34] |
Russell, J. A. (2003). Core affect and the psychological construction of emotion. Psychological Review, 110(1), 145-172.
pmid: 12529060 |
[35] | Schacht, A., & Sommer, W. (2009). Emotions in word and face processing: Early and late cortical responses. Brain & Cognition, 69(3), 538-550. |
[36] |
Schupp, H. T., Cuthbert, B. N., Bradley, M. M., Cacioppo, J. T., Ito, T., & Lang, P. J. (2000). Affective picture processing: The late positive potential is modulated by motivational relevance. Psychophysiology, 37(2), 257-261.
pmid: 10731776 |
[37] |
Schwanenflugel, P. J., Akin, C., & Luh, W. M. (1992). Context availability and the recall of abstract and concrete words. Memory & Cognition, 20(1), 96-104.
doi: 10.3758/BF03208259 URL |
[38] |
Schwanenflugel, P. J., Harnishfeger, K. K., & Stowe, R. W. (1988). Context availability and lexical decisions for abstract and concrete words. Journal of Memory and Language, 27(5), 499-520.
doi: 10.1016/0749-596X(88)90022-8 URL |
[39] |
Sergent, C., Baillet, S., & Dehaene, S. (2005). Timing of the brain events underlying access to consciousness during the attentional blink. Nature Neuroscience, 8(10), 1391-1400.
pmid: 16158062 |
[40] |
Skerry, A. E., & Saxe, R. (2015). Neural representations of emotion are organized around abstract event features. Current Biology, 25(15), 1945-1954.
doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.06.009 pmid: 26212878 |
[41] |
Stróżak, P., Bird, C. W., Corby, K., Frishkoff, G., & Curran, T. (2016). FN400 and LPC memory effects for concrete and abstract words. Psychophysiology, 53(11), 1669-1678.
doi: 10.1111/psyp.12730 pmid: 27463978 |
[42] |
van Hooff, J. C., Dietz, K. C., Sharma, D., & Bowman, H. (2008). Neural correlates of intrusion of emotion words in a modified Stroop task. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 67(1), 23-34.
doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2007.09.002 URL |
[43] |
Vigliocco, G., Kousta, S. T., Della Rosa, P. A., Vinson, D. P., Tettamanti, M., Devlin, J. T., & Cappa, S. F. (2013). The neural representation of abstract words: The role of emotion. Cerebral Cortex, 24(7), 1767-1777.
doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht025 URL |
[44] |
Vigliocco, G., Ponari, M., & Norbury, C. (2018). Learning and processing abstract words and concepts: Insights from typical and atypical development. Topics in Cognitive Science, 10(3), 533-549.
doi: 10.1111/tops.12347 URL |
[45] |
Wang, X. S, Wu, W., Ling, Z. H., Xu, Y. W., Fang, Y. X., Wang, X. Y., ... Bi, Y. C. (2018). Organizational principles of abstract words in the human brain. Cerebral Cortex, 28(12), 4305-4318.
doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhx283 URL |
[46] | Wang, Y. N., Zhou, L. M., & Luo, Y. J. (2008). The pilot establishment and evaluation of Chinese affective words system. Chinese Mental Health Journal, 22(8), 608-612. |
[ 王一牛, 周立明, 罗跃嘉. (2008). 汉语情感词系统的初步编制及评定. 中国心理卫生杂志, 22(8), 608-612.] | |
[47] |
Wang, Z. H., & Yao, Z. (2012). Concreteness effects of emotional noun words: Evidences from ERP. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 44(2), 154-165.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2012.00154 URL |
[ 王振宏, 姚昭. (2012). 情绪名词的具体性效应: 来自ERP的证据. 心理学报, 44(2), 154-165.] | |
[48] |
West, W. C., & Holcomb, P. J. (2000). Imaginal, semantic, and surface-level processing of concrete and abstract words: An electrophysiological investigation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(6), 1024-1037.
pmid: 11177422 |
[49] |
Williamson, S., Harpur, T. J., & Hare, R. D. (1991). Abnormal processing of affective words by psychopaths. Psychophysiology, 28(3), 260-273.
pmid: 1946892 |
[50] |
Yi, S. N., He, W. Q., Zhan, L., Qi, Z. Y., Zhu, C. L., Luo, W. B., & Li, H. (2015). Emotional noun processing: An ERP study with rapid serial visual presentation. PLoS ONE, 10(3), e0118924.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0118924 URL |
[51] |
Zhang, D. D., He, W. Q., Wang, T., Luo, W. B., Zhu, X. R., Gu, R. L., ... Luo, Y. J. (2014). Three stages of emotional word processing: An ERP study with rapid serial visual presentation. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(12), 1897-1903.
doi: 10.1093/scan/nst188 URL |
[52] |
Zhang, D. D., Luo, W. B., & Luo, Y. J. (2013a). Single-trial ERP analysis reveals facial expression category in a three-stage scheme. Brain Research, 1512, 78-88.
doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2013.03.044 URL |
[53] |
Zhang, D. D., Luo, W. B., & Luo, Y. J. (2013b). Single-trial ERP evidence for the three-stage scheme of facial expression processing. Science China Life Sciences, 56(9), 835-847.
doi: 10.1007/s11427-013-4527-8 URL |
[54] |
Zhang, Q., Guo, C. Y., Ding, J. H., & Wang, Z. Y. (2006). Concreteness effects in the processing of Chinese words. Brain and Language, 96(1), 59-68.
pmid: 15913753 |
[55] |
Zhang, Q., Jiao, L. H., & Cui, L. X. (2014). Influence of emotional context on concreteness effects in words processing for field-independent and field-dependent individuals. NeuroReport, 25(9), 661-667.
doi: 10.1097/WNR.0000000000000153 pmid: 24709915 |
[1] | 张文芸, 卓诗维, 郑倩倩, 关颖琳, 彭微微. 自闭特质对疼痛共情的影响:疼痛负性情绪和认知的中介作用[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(9): 1501-1517. |
[2] | 钟毅平, 牛娜娜, 范伟, 任梦梦, 李梅. 动作自主性与社会距离对主动控制感的影响:来自行为与ERPs的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(12): 1932-1948. |
[3] | 李梅, 李琎, 张冠斐, 钟毅平, 李红. 承诺水平与社会距离对信任投资的影响:来自行为与ERPs的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(11): 1859-1871. |
[4] | 覃慧怡, 丁丽洪, 段威, 雷旭. 脑电的重测信度:在多项静息态和任务态实验中的对比[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(10): 1587-1596. |
[5] | 占友龙, 肖啸, 谭千保, 李琎, 钟毅平. 声誉关注与社会距离对伤害困境中道德决策的影响:来自行为与ERPs的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2022, 54(6): 613-627. |
[6] | 范伟, 任梦梦, 张文洁, 钟毅平. 反馈对自我欺骗的影响:来自ERP的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2022, 54(5): 481-496. |
[7] | 李建花, 解佳佳, 庄锦英. 生理周期对情景记忆的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2022, 54(5): 466-480. |
[8] | 杨集梅, 柴洁余, 邱天龙, 全小山, 郑茂平. 共情与中国民族音乐情绪识别的关系:来自ERP的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2022, 54(10): 1181-1192. |
[9] | 陈杰, 伍可, 史宇鹏, 艾小青. 特质性自我构念与内外群体疼痛共情的关系: 来自事件相关电位的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(6): 629-638. |
[10] | 李琎, 孙宇, 杨子鹿, 钟毅平. 社会价值取向对自我社会奖赏加工的影响——来自ERPs的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(6): 786-800. |
[11] | 李雄, 李祚山, 向滨洋, 孟景. 注意线索对自闭特质个体疼痛共情的影响:来自事件相关电位的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(3): 294-306. |
[12] | 孙岩, 吕娇娇, 兰帆, 张丽娜. 自我关注重评和情境关注重评情绪调节策略及对随后认知控制的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(12): 1393-1406. |
[13] | 黄亮, 杨雪, 黄志华, 王益文. 理解双人肢体运动表达的友好和敌对意图的ERP证据[J]. 心理学报, 2019, 51(5): 557-570. |
[14] | 王丽丽, 冯文锋, 贾丽娜, 朱湘茹, 罗文波, 杨苏勇, 罗跃嘉. 赢分与输分运动员面孔表情和身体姿势情绪的加工机制*[J]. 心理学报, 2018, 50(8): 892-906. |
[15] | 杨青青, 胡娜, 陈旭, 牛娟, 翟晶. 恋人亲密情景下的回避型与安全型 依恋个体情绪调节电生理差异[J]. 心理学报, 2018, 50(3): 306-316. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||