心理学报 ›› 2025, Vol. 57 ›› Issue (1): 1-17.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2025.0001 cstr: 32110.14.2025.0001
• 研究报告 • 下一篇
收稿日期:
2024-01-07
发布日期:
2024-11-20
出版日期:
2025-01-25
通讯作者:
叶群, E-mail: qun.ye@zjnu.edu.cn作者简介:
夏莲香和刘凯歌为共同第一作者。
基金资助:
XIA Lianxiang1, LIU Kaige1, LI Xinyu1,2, YE Qun1,2()
Received:
2024-01-07
Online:
2024-11-20
Published:
2025-01-25
摘要:
时间顺序信息作为记忆项目之间的重要联结要素之一, 易受到情绪的干扰。本研究通过3项实验探究编码方式与叙事连贯性是否以及如何调节情绪对于时间顺序记忆的干扰。实验1采用2 (编码方式:叙事编码/离散编码) × 3 (情绪类型:正性/负性/中性)的混合实验设计, 其中编码方式为被试间变量, 探究编码方式是否能调节图片材料中情绪信息对时间顺序记忆的干扰。结果显示, 在离散编码组, 负性情绪条件下的时间顺序记忆正确率显著比其他两种条件低; 相反, 在叙事编码组, 三种情绪类型下的时间顺序记忆没有显著差异。实验2采用两种情绪类型(中性和负性)的词语刺激, 检验编码方式对时间顺序记忆的影响是否跨材料具有普适性。实验3通过操纵项目之间的叙事连贯性程度, 进一步探究上述效应是否依赖于项目间连贯性程度。结果显示, 上述叙事编码效应仅在项目之间具有高连贯性时出现。本研究不仅发现了叙事编码对于情绪记忆中时间信息的保持机制, 还指明了叙事编码效应中连贯性程度的重要性。
中图分类号:
夏莲香, 刘凯歌, 李新宇, 叶群. (2025). 编码方式与叙事连贯性调节情绪对时间顺序记忆的影响. 心理学报, 57(1), 1-17.
XIA Lianxiang, LIU Kaige, LI Xinyu, YE Qun. (2025). Encoding types and narrative coherence modulate the impact of emotions on temporal order memory. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 57(1), 1-17.
实验 | 效价(M ± SD) | 唤醒度(M ± SD) | 连贯性评分(M ± SD) |
---|---|---|---|
实验1 | |||
负性 | 1.71 ± 0.38 | 5.70 ± 0.39 | 8.71 ± 0.66 |
中性 | 3.93 ± 0.43 | 3.97 ± 0.44 | 8.84 ± 0.46 |
正性 | 5.55 ± 0.36 | 3.04 ± 0.43 | 8.77 ± 0.70 |
实验2 | |||
负性 | 2.92 ± 0.33 | 5.91 ± 0.52 | 9.21 ± 0.41 |
中性 | 4.95 ± 0.37 | 4.25 ± 0.47 | 9.23 ± 0.34 |
表1 实验材料评分结果
实验 | 效价(M ± SD) | 唤醒度(M ± SD) | 连贯性评分(M ± SD) |
---|---|---|---|
实验1 | |||
负性 | 1.71 ± 0.38 | 5.70 ± 0.39 | 8.71 ± 0.66 |
中性 | 3.93 ± 0.43 | 3.97 ± 0.44 | 8.84 ± 0.46 |
正性 | 5.55 ± 0.36 | 3.04 ± 0.43 | 8.77 ± 0.70 |
实验2 | |||
负性 | 2.92 ± 0.33 | 5.91 ± 0.52 | 9.21 ± 0.41 |
中性 | 4.95 ± 0.37 | 4.25 ± 0.47 | 9.23 ± 0.34 |
情绪 | 连贯性 | 连贯性评分(M ± SD) | 语义相似性(M ± SD) |
---|---|---|---|
负性 | 高连贯 | 9.21 ± 0.41 | 0.47 ± 0.07 |
低连贯 | 3.11 ± 0.80 | 0.31 ± 0.06 | |
中性 | 高连贯 | 9.23 ± 0.34 | 0.37 ± 0.07 |
低连贯 | 2.85 ± 0.84 | 0.27 ± 0.05 |
表2 词语组的连贯性评分
情绪 | 连贯性 | 连贯性评分(M ± SD) | 语义相似性(M ± SD) |
---|---|---|---|
负性 | 高连贯 | 9.21 ± 0.41 | 0.47 ± 0.07 |
低连贯 | 3.11 ± 0.80 | 0.31 ± 0.06 | |
中性 | 高连贯 | 9.23 ± 0.34 | 0.37 ± 0.07 |
低连贯 | 2.85 ± 0.84 | 0.27 ± 0.05 |
[1] |
Antony, J., Lozano, A., Dhoat, P., Chen, J., & Bennion, K. (2024). Causal and chronological relationships predict memory organization for nonlinear narratives. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 36(11), 2368-2385. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_02216.
pmid: 38991132 |
[2] |
Baldassano, C., Hasson, U., & Norman, K. A. (2018). Representation of real-world event schemas during narrative perception. Journal of Neuroscience, 38(45), 9689-9699.
doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0251-18.2018 pmid: 30249790 |
[3] |
Bauer, P. J. (2015). Development of episodic and autobiographical memory: The importance of remembering forgetting. Developmental Review, 38, 146-166.
pmid: 26644633 |
[4] |
Bellana, B., Mahabal, A., & Honey, C. J. (2022). Narrative thinking lingers in spontaneous thought. Nature Communications, 13(1), 4585.
doi: 10.1038/s41467-022-32113-6 pmid: 35933422 |
[5] |
Bellmund, J. L. S., Deuker, L., Montijn, N. D., & Doeller, C. F. (2022). Mnemonic construction and representation of temporal structure in the hippocampal formation. Nature Communications, 13(1), 3395
doi: 10.1038/s41467-022-30984-3 pmid: 35739096 |
[6] | Bisby, J. A., & Burgess, N. (2013). Negative affect impairs associative memory but not item memory. Learning & Memory, 21(1), 21-27. |
[7] |
Bisby, J. A., & Burgess, N. (2017). Differential effects of negative emotion on memory for items and associations, and their relationship to intrusive imagery. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 17, 124-132.
doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2017.07.012 pmid: 29238740 |
[8] |
Bisby, J. A., Burgess, N., & Brewin, C. R. (2020). Reduced memory coherence for negative events and its relationship to posttraumatic stress disorder. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 29(3), 267-272.
doi: 10.1177/0963721420917691 pmid: 33214741 |
[9] |
Bisby, J. A., Horner, A. J., Horlyck, L. D., & Burgess, N. (2016). Opposing effects of negative emotion on amygdalar and hippocampal memory for items and associations. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11(6), 981-990.
doi: 10.1093/scan/nsw028 pmid: 26969864 |
[10] |
Brewin, C. R., Gregory, J. D., Lipton, M., & Burgess, N. (2010). Intrusive images in psychological disorders: Characteristics, neural mechanisms, and treatment implications. Psychological Review, 117(1), 210-232.
doi: 10.1037/a0018113 pmid: 20063969 |
[11] | Brown, N. R. (2005). On the prevalence of event clusters in autobiographical memory. Social Cognition, 23(1), 35-69. |
[12] |
Clewett, D., DuBrow, S., & Davachi, L. (2019). Transcending time in the brain: How event memories are constructed from experience. Hippocampus, 29(3), 162-183.
doi: 10.1002/hipo.23074 pmid: 30734391 |
[13] | Cohn-Sheehy, B. I., Delarazan, A. I., Crivelli-Decker, J. E., Reagh, Z. M., Mundada, N. S., Yonelinas, A. P., ... Ranganath, C. (2022). Narratives bridge the divide between distant events in episodic memory. Memory & Cognition, 50(3), 478-494. |
[14] | Cohn-Sheehy, B. I., Delarazan, A. I., Reagh, Z. M., Crivelli-Decker, J. E., Kim, K., Barnett, A. J., Zacks, J. M., & Ranganath, C. (2021). The hippocampus constructs narrative memories across distant events. Current Biology, 31(22), 4935-4945. |
[15] | Coull, J. T., & Giersch, A. (2022). The distinction between temporal order and duration processing, and implications for schizophrenia. Nature Reviews Psychology, 1(5), 257-271. |
[16] | Cui, X., Tian, Y., Zhang, L., Chen, Y., Bai, Y., Li, D., Liu, J., Gable, P., & Yin, H. (2023). The role of valence, arousal, stimulus type, and temporal paradigm in the effect of emotion on time perception: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 30(1), 1-21. |
[17] | Dev, D. K., Wardell, V., Checknita, K. J., Te, A. A., Petrucci, A. S., Le, M. L., Madan, C. R., & Palombo, D. J. (2022). Negative emotion enhances memory for the sequential unfolding of a naturalistic experience. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 11(4), 510-521. |
[18] |
Dolleman-Van Der Weel, M. J., Lopes Da Silva, F. H., & Witter, M. P. (1997). Nucleus reuniens thalami modulates activity in hippocampal field CA1 through excitatory and inhibitory mechanisms. Journal of Neuroscience, 17(14), 5640-5650.
pmid: 9204945 |
[19] | DuBrow, S., & Davachi, L. (2013). The influence of context boundaries on memory for the sequential order of events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142(4), 1277-1286. |
[20] |
Easterbrook, J. A. (1959). The effect of emotion on cue utilization and the organization of behavior. Psychological Review, 66(3), 183-201.
pmid: 13658305 |
[21] |
Ebbinghaus, H. (2013). Memory: A contribution to experimental psychology. Annals of Neurosciences, 20(4), 155-156.
doi: 10.5214/ans.0972.7531.200408 pmid: 25206041 |
[22] |
Eichenbaum, H. (2017). Prefrontal-hippocampal interactions in episodic memory. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 18(9), 547-558.
doi: 10.1038/nrn.2017.74 pmid: 28655882 |
[23] | Fang, Y. H., & Zhang, J. J. (2009). Asymmetry in naming and categorizing of chinese words and pictures: Role of semantic radicals. Acta Paychologica Sinica, 41(2), 114-126. |
[方燕红, 张积家. (2009). 汉字词和图片命名与分类的比较. 心理学报, 41(2), 114-126.] | |
[24] | Franklin, N. T., Norman, K. A., Ranganath, C., Zacks, J. M., & Gershman, S. J. (2020). Structured event memory: A neuro- symbolic model of event cognition. Psychological Review, 127(3), 327-361. |
[25] |
Frühholz, S., Jellinghaus, A., & Herrmann, M. (2011). Time course of implicit processing and explicit processing of emotional faces and emotional words. Biological Psychology, 87(2), 265-274.
doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.03.008 pmid: 21440031 |
[26] |
Graesser, A. C., Singer, M., & Trabasso, T. (1994). Constructing inferences during narrative text comprehension. Psychological Review, 101(3), 371-395.
pmid: 7938337 |
[27] | Grall, C., Equita, J., & Finn, E. S. (2023). Neural unscrambling of temporal information during a nonlinear narrative. Cerebral Cortex, 33(11), 7001-7014. |
[28] | Grühn, D., & Sharifian, N. (2016). Lists of emotion stimuli. In H. Meiselman (Ed.), Emotion measurement (pp. 145-164). London, UK: Woodhead Publishing. |
[29] |
Habermas, T., Ott, L. M., Schubert, M., Schneider, B., & Pate, A. (2008). Stuck in the past: Negative bias, explanatory style, temporal order, and evaluative perspectives in life narratives of clinically depressed individuals. Depression and Anxiety, 25(11), E121-E132.
doi: 10.1002/da.20389 pmid: 17957809 |
[30] | Han, M., Mao, X., Kartvelishvili, N., Li, W., & Guo, C. (2018). Unitization mitigates interference by intrinsic negative emotion in familiarity and recollection of associative memory: Electrophysiological evidence. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 18, 1259-1268. |
[31] | Hinojosa, J. A., Carretie, L., Valcarcel, M. A., Méndez-Bértolo, C., & Pozo, M. A. (2009). Electrophysiological differences in the processing of affective information in words and pictures. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 9(2), 173-189. |
[32] | Huntjens, R. J., Wessel, I., Postma, A., van Wees-Cieraad, R., & de Jong, P. J. (2015). Binding temporal context in memory: Impact of emotional arousal as a function of state anxiety and state dissociation. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 203(7), 545-550. |
[33] | Jayakumar, M., Balusu, C., & Aly, M. (2023). Attentional fluctuations and the temporal organization of memory. Cognition, 235, 105408. |
[34] |
Kensinger, E. A. (2009). Remembering the details: Effects of emotion. Emotion Review, 1(2), 99-113.
doi: 10.1177/1754073908100432 pmid: 19421427 |
[35] | Kensinger, E. A., & Corkin, S. (2003). Memory enhancement for emotional words: Are emotional words more vividly remembered than neutral words?. Memory & Cognition, 31(8), 1169-1180. |
[36] | Kensinger, E. A., Garoff-Eaton, R. J., & Schacter, D. L. (2007). Effects of emotion on memory specificity: Memory trade- offs elicited by negative visually arousing stimuli. Journal of Memory and Language, 56(4), 575-591. |
[37] |
Kensinger, E. A., Piguet, O., Krendl, A. C., & Corkin, S. (2005). Memory for contextual details: Effects of emotion and aging. Psychology and Aging, 20(2), 241-250.
pmid: 16029088 |
[38] | Kensinger, E. A., & Schacter, D. L. (2006). Processing emotional pictures and words: Effects of valence and arousal. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 6(2), 110-126. |
[39] |
Kurby, C. A., & Zacks, J. M. (2008). Segmentation in the perception and memory of events. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(2), 72-79.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.11.004 pmid: 18178125 |
[40] |
LaBar, K. S., & Cabeza, R. (2006). Cognitive neuroscience of emotional memory. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7(1), 54-64.
doi: 10.1038/nrn1825 pmid: 16371950 |
[41] | Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (2008). International affective picture system (IAPS): Affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual. [Technical Report A-8]. Gainesville, FL: University of Florida.] |
[42] |
Lee, H., & Chen, J. (2022). Predicting memory from the network structure of naturalistic events. Nature Communications, 13(1), 4235.
doi: 10.1038/s41467-022-31965-2 pmid: 35869083 |
[43] | Lohnas, L. J., & Howard, M. W. (2024). The influence of emotion on temporal context models. Cognition and Emotion. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2024.2371075 |
[44] |
Lu, Y., Liang, J. Q., & Guo, C. Y. (2015). The influence of semantic integration between items on associative recognition: Evidence from ERPs study. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 47(4), 427-438.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2015.00427 |
[律原, 梁九清, 郭春彦. (2015). 项目间语义可整合性对联结再认的影响——来自ERPs研究证据. 心理学报, 47(4), 427-438.] | |
[45] | Madan, C. R., Caplan, J. B., Lau, C. S. M., & Fujiwara, E. (2012). Emotional arousal does not enhance association- memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 66(4), 695-716. |
[46] |
Madan, C. R., Fujiwara, E., Caplan, J. B., & Sommer, T. (2017). Emotional arousal impairs association-memory: Roles of amygdala and hippocampus. Neuroimage, 156, 14-28.
doi: S1053-8119(17)30384-1 pmid: 28483720 |
[47] |
Madan, C. R., Scott, S. M. E., & Kensinger, E. A. (2019). Positive emotion enhances association-memory. Emotion, 19(4), 733-740.
doi: 10.1037/emo0000465 pmid: 30124317 |
[48] | Maddock, R. J., & Frein, S. T. (2009). Reduced memory for the spatial and temporal context of unpleasant words. Cognition and Emotion, 23(1), 96-117. |
[49] | Makowski, D., Sperduti, M., Nicolas, S., & Piolino, P. (2017). “Being there” and remembering it: Presence improves memory encoding. Consciousness and Cognition, 53, 194-202. |
[50] |
Mao, X. R., Xu, H. F., & Guo, C. Y. (2015). Emotional memory enhancement effect in dual-processing recognition retrieval. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 47(9), 1111-1123.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2015.01111 |
[毛新瑞, 徐慧芳, 郭春彦. (2015). 双加工再认提取中的情绪记忆增强效应. 心理学报, 47(9), 1111-1123.] | |
[51] |
Mather, M. (2007). Emotional arousal and memory binding: An object-based framework. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2(1), 33-52.
doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00028.x pmid: 26151918 |
[52] |
Mather, M., & Sutherland, M. R. (2011). Arousal-biased competition in perception and memory. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6(2), 114-133.
doi: 10.1177/1745691611400234 pmid: 21660127 |
[53] |
McClay, M., Sachs, M. E., & Clewett, D. (2023). Dynamic emotional states shape the episodic structure of memory. Nature Communications, 14(1), 6533.
doi: 10.1038/s41467-023-42241-2 pmid: 37848429 |
[54] |
Milivojevic, B., Vicente-Grabovetsky, A., & Doeller, C. F. (2015). Insight reconfigures hippocampal-prefrontal memories. Current Biology, 25(7), 821-830.
doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.01.033 pmid: 25728693 |
[55] | Murray, B. D., & Kensinger, E. A. (2012). The effects of emotion and encoding strategy on associative memory. Memory & Cognition, 40(7), 1056-1069. |
[56] |
Naim, M., Katkov, M., & Tsodyks, M. (2021). Effects of order on memory of event times. Scientific Reports, 11(1), 17456.
doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-97032-w pmid: 34465836 |
[57] |
Nelson, K., & Fivush, R. (2004). The emergence of autobiographical memory: A social cultural developmental theory. Psychological Review, 111(2), 486-511.
doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.111.2.486 pmid: 15065919 |
[58] | Paivio, A. (1991). Dual coding theory: Retrospect and current status. Canadian Journal of Psychology / Revue canadienne de psychologie, 45(3), 255-287. |
[59] | Palombo, D. J. (2024). Beyond memory: The transcendence of episodic narratives. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie Expérimentale. 78(3), 155-162. |
[60] |
Palombo, D. J., & Cocquyt, C. (2020). Emotion in context: Remembering when. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 24(9), 687-690.
doi: S1364-6613(20)30142-X pmid: 32593502 |
[61] | Palombo, D. J., Te, A. A., Checknita, K. J., & Madan, C. R. (2021). Exploring the facets of emotional episodic memory: Remembering “what,” “when,” and “which”. Psychological Science, 32(7), 1104-1114. |
[62] |
Peirce, J., Gray, J. R., Simpson, S., MacAskill, M., Hochenberger, R., Sogo, H., Kastman, E., & Lindelov, J. K. (2019). PsychoPy2: Experiments in behavior made easy. Behavior Research Methods, 51(1), 195-203.
doi: 10.3758/s13428-018-01193-y pmid: 30734206 |
[63] | Peng, X. Z., & Zhou, X. L. (2005). Emotional information and attentional bias. Advances in Psychological Science, 13(4), 488-496. |
[彭晓哲, 周晓林. (2005). 情绪信息与注意偏向. 心理科学进展, 13(4), 488-496.] | |
[64] | Petrucci, A. S., & Palombo, D. J. (2021). A matter of time: How does emotion influence temporal aspects of remembering?. Cognition and Emotion, 35(8), 1499-1515. |
[65] |
Phelps, E. A. (2004). Human emotion and memory: Interactions of the amygdala and hippocampal complex. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 14(2), 198-202.
doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2004.03.015 pmid: 15082325 |
[66] |
Pu, Y., Kong, X. Z., Ranganath, C., & Melloni, L. (2022). Event boundaries shape temporal organization of memory by resetting temporal context. Nature Communications, 13(1), 622.
doi: 10.1038/s41467-022-28216-9 pmid: 35110527 |
[67] |
Rellecke, J., Palazova, M., Sommer, W., & Schacht, A. (2011). On the automaticity of emotion processing in words and faces: Event-related brain potentials evidence from a superficial task. Brain and Cognition, 77(1), 23-32.
doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2011.07.001 pmid: 21794970 |
[68] |
Rubin, D. C. (2006). The basic-systems model of episodic memory. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1(4), 277-311.
doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2006.00017.x pmid: 26151796 |
[69] | Schacter, D. L., & Addis, D. R. (2007). The cognitive neuroscience of constructive memory: Remembering the past and imagining the future. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 362(1481), 773-786. |
[70] | Schmidt, K., Patnaik, P., & Kensinger, E. A. (2011). Emotion's influence on memory for spatial and temporal context. Cognition and Emotion, 25(2), 229-243. |
[71] | Talmi, D. (2013). Enhanced emotional memory: Cognitive and neural mechanisms. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22(6), 430-436. |
[72] |
Talmi, D., Lohnas, L. J., & Daw, N. D. (2019). A retrieved context model of the emotional modulation of memory. Psychological Review, 126(4), 455-485.
doi: 10.1037/rev0000132 pmid: 30973247 |
[73] |
Talmi, D., Schimmack, U., Paterson, T., & Moscovitch, M. (2007). The role of attention and relatedness in emotionally enhanced memory. Emotion, 7(1), 89-102.
pmid: 17352566 |
[74] |
Todd, R. M., Talmi, D., Schmitz, T. W., Susskind, J., & Anderson, A. K. (2012). Psychophysical and neural evidence for emotion-enhanced perceptual vividness. Journal of Neuroscience, 32(33), 11201-11212.
doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0155-12.2012 pmid: 22895705 |
[75] | Tulving, E. (1972). Episodic and semantic memory. In E. Tulving & W. Donaldson (Eds.), Organization of memory (pp. 381-403). New York, NY: Academic Press. |
[76] | Ventura-Bort, C., Löw, A., Wendt, J., Moltó, J., Poy, R., Dolcos, F., Hamm, A. O., & Weymar, M. (2016). Binding neutral information to emotional contexts: Brain dynamics of long-term recognition memory. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 16(2), 234-247. |
[77] |
Wang, J., Tambini, A., & Lapate, R. C. (2022). The tie that binds: Temporal coding and adaptive emotion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 26(12), 1103-1118.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2022.09.005 pmid: 36302710 |
[78] |
Wang, X., Lu, J. M., & Chen, W. Y. (2019). The processing of emotional words and its emotional effect characteristics: Evidence from ERP studies. Advances in Psychological Science, 27(11), 1842-1852.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2019.01842 |
[王霞, 卢家楣, 陈武英. (2019). 情绪词加工过程及其情绪效应特点:ERP的证据. 心理科学进展, 27(11), 1842-1852.]
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2019.01842 |
|
[79] | Wang, Y. N., Zhou, L. M., & Luo, Y. J. (2008). The pilot establishment and evaluation of Chinese affective word system. Chinese Mental Health Journal, 22(8), 608-612. |
[王一牛, 周立明, 罗跃嘉. (2008). 汉语情感词系统的初步编制及评定. 中国心理卫生杂志, 22(8), 608-612.] | |
[80] |
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6), 1063-1070.
doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.54.6.1063 pmid: 3397865 |
[81] | Wen, T., & Egner, T. (2022). Retrieval context determines whether event boundaries impair or enhance temporal order memory. Cognition, 225, 105145. |
[82] | Wu, X., Viñals, X., Ben-Yakov, A., Staresina, B. P., & Fuentemilla, L. (2022). Post-encoding reactivation is related to learning of episodes in humans. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 35(1), 74-89. |
[83] |
Xu, X., & Kwok, S. C. (2019). Temporal-order iconicity bias in narrative event understanding and memory. Memory, 27(8), 1079-1090.
doi: 10.1080/09658211.2019.1622734 pmid: 31138015 |
[84] |
Zacks, J. M., Speer, N. K., Swallow, K. M., Braver, T. S., & Reynolds, J. R. (2007). Event perception: A mind-brain perspective. Psychological Bulletin, 133(2), 273-293.
doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.2.273 pmid: 17338600 |
[85] |
Zacks, J. M., & Tversky, B. (2001). Event structure in perception and conception. Psychological Bulletin, 127(1), 3-21.
pmid: 11271755 |
[1] | 张环, 秦锡权, 刘雨, 林琳, 吴捷. 不同强度趋近动机积极情绪对基于语义关联性错误记忆的影响及其神经机制[J]. 心理学报, 2025, 57(3): 349-362. |
[2] | 姚雨佳, 颜之悦, 林慧慧, 陈静全, 宣雨阳. 人际距离和策略创造性对人际情绪调节的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2025, 57(1): 125-134. |
[3] | 张环, 王晨, 李俊霞, 林琳, 吴捷. 情绪效价和动机强度对社会分享型提取诱发遗忘的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(8): 999-1014. |
[4] | 王丹, 付雨佳, 陈文锋. 社会情境对情绪感染的影响:一项基于EMG的超扫描研究[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(8): 1047-1060. |
[5] | 张明杨, 杨盈, 包寒吴霜, 蔡华俭. 中国人的积极理想情绪:近几十年来的变迁[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(7): 847-858. |
[6] | 黄顺森, 来枭雄, 张彩, 赵心媚, 代欣然, 祁梦迪, 王欢蕾, 王文荣, 王耘. 青少年手机压力与心理健康的关系:基于多元宇宙样分析和密集追踪方法[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(6): 745-758. |
[7] | 孟现鑫, 罗怡, 韩晨媛, 吴国伟, 常姣, 袁加锦, 千坤, 傅小兰. 社会接纳调节社会排斥对情绪冲突适应的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(5): 577-593. |
[8] | 童廷婷, 白幼玲, 冯廷勇. 情绪调节改善拖延行为的认知机制:任务厌恶中介作用[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(4): 458-468. |
[9] | 王伟晗, 曹斐臻, 余林伟, 曾珂, 杨鑫超, 徐强. 群体信息对面部表情识别的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(3): 268-280. |
[10] | 尹华站, 肖春花, 夏安妮, 袁中静, 崔晓冰, 李丹. 基本情绪对时距知觉的影响: 来自三水平元分析和网络元分析的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(12): 1676-1690. |
[11] | 张瑾, 许销冰, 庄晓涵. 产品包装的医药符号线索对产品偏好的影响: 感知有效性和规避情绪的中介作用[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(12): 1836-1850. |
[12] | 肖程元, 赵世瑞, 袁加锦. 积极认知重评对负性信息传播的调控及多维证据[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(11): 1471-1487. |
[13] | 李其容, 王春淼, 孙明慧. 创业激情的“错位”对创业努力和创业成瘾的影响机制[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(11): 1568-1584. |
[14] | 田杨阳, 李东, 闫向博, 李曌, 崔倩, 蒋重清. 动态高兴表情评价中的表征动量效应和参照依赖效应[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(1): 29-43. |
[15] | 陈沛琪, 张银玲, 胡馨木, 王静, 买晓琴. 10~12岁儿童社会价值取向对第三方利他行为的影响:情绪的作用[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(8): 1255-1269. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||