心理学报 ›› 2024, Vol. 56 ›› Issue (1): 29-43.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2024.00029
收稿日期:
2023-05-07
发布日期:
2023-11-23
出版日期:
2024-01-25
通讯作者:
崔倩,蒋重清
E-mail:cuiqian119@163.com;jzqcjj@hotmail.com
TIAN Yangyang, LI Dong, YAN Xiangbo, LI Zhao, CUI Qian(), JIANG Zhongqing()
Received:
2023-05-07
Online:
2023-11-23
Published:
2024-01-25
Contact:
CUI Qian, JIANG Zhongqing
E-mail:cuiqian119@163.com;jzqcjj@hotmail.com
摘要:
已有的表情面孔研究大多使用静态面孔图片材料, 对动态表情研究相对不足。然而, 在生活中人们的表情往往是动态的。为了探究动态表情的情绪加工特点, 本研究通过3个包含了动态和静态高兴表情图片材料的实验, 考察了动态高兴表情的强度变化方向和总体平均表征对情绪三维度评价的影响。结果发现:总体平均表征越高的动态表情面孔会得到更高的唤醒度评分。以与动态表情最后一帧表情强度相同的静态表情为对比条件, 从强到弱变化的动态表情会获得更低的效价评分和更高的优势度评分, 而从弱到强变化的动态表情则获得更高的效价评分, 即出现了表征动量效应。相对于从弱到强, 从强到弱变化的动态表情所产生的知觉表征动量效应更大。此外, 知觉者会基于实验中所见过的表情图片形成内部参照标准, 进而影响当前表情图片的情绪评价, 内部参照标准越低, 目标表情图片所得情绪评分越高, 反之亦然, 表现出参照依赖效应。这些加工特征提示今后研究者采用表情材料进行研究时, 需要注意动静差异以及材料间的影响。
中图分类号:
田杨阳, 李东, 闫向博, 李曌, 崔倩, 蒋重清. (2024). 动态高兴表情评价中的表征动量效应和参照依赖效应. 心理学报, 56(1), 29-43.
TIAN Yangyang, LI Dong, YAN Xiangbo, LI Zhao, CUI Qian, JIANG Zhongqing. (2024). The representational momentum effect and the reference dependence effect on the evaluation of dynamic happy expressions. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 56(1), 29-43.
总体平均表征 | |||
---|---|---|---|
75%高兴 | 25%高兴 | ||
强度变 化方向 | 从强到弱 | 100%高兴−50%高兴 | 50%高兴−中性 |
从弱到强 | 50%高兴−100%高兴 | 中性−50%高兴 |
表1 实验1各实验处理示意
总体平均表征 | |||
---|---|---|---|
75%高兴 | 25%高兴 | ||
强度变 化方向 | 从强到弱 | 100%高兴−50%高兴 | 50%高兴−中性 |
从弱到强 | 50%高兴−100%高兴 | 中性−50%高兴 |
情绪维度 | 实验条件 | M | t | df | p |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
效价 | 100%高兴−50%高兴 | 3.84 | -1.24 | 31 | 0.226 |
50%高兴−中性 | 3.08 | -7.17 | 31 | < 0.001 | |
50%高兴−100%高兴 | 5.40 | 14.12 | 31 | < 0.001 | |
中性−50%高兴 | 4.87 | 8.06 | 31 | < 0.001 | |
唤醒度 | 100%高兴−50%高兴 | 3.97 | -0.25 | 31 | 0.803 |
50%高兴−中性 | 3.63 | -1.72 | 31 | 0.096 | |
50%高兴−100%高兴 | 4.30 | 1.59 | 31 | 0.122 | |
中性−50%高兴 | 3.93 | -0.51 | 31 | 0.616 | |
优势度 | 100%高兴−50%高兴 | 4.24 | 2.28 | 31 | 0.029 |
50%高兴−中性 | 4.76 | 6.10 | 31 | < 0.001 | |
50%高兴−100%高兴 | 3.22 | -5.38 | 31 | < 0.001 | |
中性−50%高兴 | 3.49 | -5.15 | 31 | < 0.001 |
表2 实验1各组表情图片情绪三维度评分的单样本t检验(检验值为4)
情绪维度 | 实验条件 | M | t | df | p |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
效价 | 100%高兴−50%高兴 | 3.84 | -1.24 | 31 | 0.226 |
50%高兴−中性 | 3.08 | -7.17 | 31 | < 0.001 | |
50%高兴−100%高兴 | 5.40 | 14.12 | 31 | < 0.001 | |
中性−50%高兴 | 4.87 | 8.06 | 31 | < 0.001 | |
唤醒度 | 100%高兴−50%高兴 | 3.97 | -0.25 | 31 | 0.803 |
50%高兴−中性 | 3.63 | -1.72 | 31 | 0.096 | |
50%高兴−100%高兴 | 4.30 | 1.59 | 31 | 0.122 | |
中性−50%高兴 | 3.93 | -0.51 | 31 | 0.616 | |
优势度 | 100%高兴−50%高兴 | 4.24 | 2.28 | 31 | 0.029 |
50%高兴−中性 | 4.76 | 6.10 | 31 | < 0.001 | |
50%高兴−100%高兴 | 3.22 | -5.38 | 31 | < 0.001 | |
中性−50%高兴 | 3.49 | -5.15 | 31 | < 0.001 |
最后一帧表情强度 | |||
---|---|---|---|
50%高兴 | 100%高兴 | ||
表情 状态 | 动态 | 100%高兴−50%高兴 | 50%高兴−100%高兴 |
静态 | 静态50%高兴 | 静态100%高兴 |
表3 实验2a各实验处理示意
最后一帧表情强度 | |||
---|---|---|---|
50%高兴 | 100%高兴 | ||
表情 状态 | 动态 | 100%高兴−50%高兴 | 50%高兴−100%高兴 |
静态 | 静态50%高兴 | 静态100%高兴 |
最后一帧表情 | |||
---|---|---|---|
50%高兴 | 中性 | ||
表情 状态 | 动态 | 中性−50%高兴 | 50%高兴−中性 |
静态 | 静态50%高兴 | 静态中性 |
表4 实验2b各实验处理示意
最后一帧表情 | |||
---|---|---|---|
50%高兴 | 中性 | ||
表情 状态 | 动态 | 中性−50%高兴 | 50%高兴−中性 |
静态 | 静态50%高兴 | 静态中性 |
重复条件 | 实验1 中位置 | 实验2a 中位置 | 实验2b 中位置 | t | p | Cohen's d |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
静态50%高兴 | ||||||
效价 | 3 | 2 | −4.60 | <0.001 | 1.19 | |
唤醒度 | 4 | 2 | −2.45 | 0.017 | 0.63 | |
优势度 | 2 | 3 | 2.62 | 0.011 | 0.68 | |
100%高兴− 50%高兴 | ||||||
效价 | 3 | 4 | 3.05 | 0.003 | 0.78 | |
唤醒度 | 2 | 3 | 0.68 | 0.51 | ||
优势度 | 2 | 1 | −2.79 | 0.007 | 0.71 | |
50%高兴− 100%高兴 | ||||||
效价 | 1 | 1 | 0.66 | 0.51 | ||
唤醒度 | 1 | 1 | −0.88 | 0.38 | ||
优势度 | 4 | 3 | −2.20 | 0.031 | 0.56 | |
中性−50%高兴 | ||||||
效价 | 2 | 1 | −1.68 | 0.098 | 0.43 | |
唤醒度 | 3 | 1 | −2.05 | 0.044 | 0.52 | |
优势度 | 3 | 4 | 0.18 | 0.86 | ||
50%高兴−中性 | ||||||
效价 | 4 | 4 | −0.12 | 0.90 | ||
唤醒度 | 4 | 3 | 0.99 | 0.33 | ||
优势度 | 1 | 1 | 1.43 | 0.16 |
表5 重复刺激条件在各个实验中所得评分的相对位置以及该条件在不同实验中的差异比较结果
重复条件 | 实验1 中位置 | 实验2a 中位置 | 实验2b 中位置 | t | p | Cohen's d |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
静态50%高兴 | ||||||
效价 | 3 | 2 | −4.60 | <0.001 | 1.19 | |
唤醒度 | 4 | 2 | −2.45 | 0.017 | 0.63 | |
优势度 | 2 | 3 | 2.62 | 0.011 | 0.68 | |
100%高兴− 50%高兴 | ||||||
效价 | 3 | 4 | 3.05 | 0.003 | 0.78 | |
唤醒度 | 2 | 3 | 0.68 | 0.51 | ||
优势度 | 2 | 1 | −2.79 | 0.007 | 0.71 | |
50%高兴− 100%高兴 | ||||||
效价 | 1 | 1 | 0.66 | 0.51 | ||
唤醒度 | 1 | 1 | −0.88 | 0.38 | ||
优势度 | 4 | 3 | −2.20 | 0.031 | 0.56 | |
中性−50%高兴 | ||||||
效价 | 2 | 1 | −1.68 | 0.098 | 0.43 | |
唤醒度 | 3 | 1 | −2.05 | 0.044 | 0.52 | |
优势度 | 3 | 4 | 0.18 | 0.86 | ||
50%高兴−中性 | ||||||
效价 | 4 | 4 | −0.12 | 0.90 | ||
唤醒度 | 4 | 3 | 0.99 | 0.33 | ||
优势度 | 1 | 1 | 1.43 | 0.16 |
[1] |
Ambadar Z., Schooler J. W., & Cohn J. F. (2005). Deciphering the enigmatic face: The importance of facial dynamics in interpreting subtle facial expressions. Psychological Science, 16(5), 403-410.
doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01548.x pmid: 15869701 |
[2] |
Bradley M. M., & Lang P. J. (1994). Measuring emotion: The self-assessment manikin and the semantic differential. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 25(1), 49-59.
doi: 10.1016/0005-7916(94)90063-9 pmid: 7962581 |
[3] |
Dong R. (2015). Facing orientation effect on representational momentum. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 47(2), 190-202.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2015.00190 |
[ 董蕊. (2015). 表征动量的朝向效应. 心理学报, 47(2), 190-202.] | |
[4] |
Dozolme D., Prigent E., Yang Y. F., & Amorim M. A. (2018). The neuroelectric dynamics of the emotional anticipation of other people's pain. PloS One, 13(8), e0200535. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0200535
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0200535 URL |
[5] | Dyjas O., Bausenhart K. M., & Ulrich R. (2012). Trial-by- trial updating of an internal reference in discrimination tasks: Evidence from effects of stimulus order and trial sequence. Attention, Perception and Psychophysics, 74(8), 1819-1841. |
[6] |
Ekman P., Freisen W. V., & Ancoli S. (1980). Facial signs of emotional experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39(6), 1125-1134.
doi: 10.1037/h0077722 URL |
[7] |
Fang X., van Kleef G. A., & Sauter D. A. (2018). Person perception from changing emotional expressions: Primacy, recency, or averaging effect? Cognition and Emotion, 32(8), 1597-1610.
doi: 10.1080/02699931.2018.1432476 URL |
[8] | Finke R. A., & Freyd J. J. (1985). Transformations of visual memory induced by implied motions of pattern elements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 11(4), 780-794. |
[9] | Fiorentini C., & Viviani P. (2011). Is there a dynamic advantage for facial expressions? Journal of Vision, 11(3), 17. https://doi.org/10.1167/11.3.17 |
[10] |
Freyd J. J. (1987). Dynamic mental representations. Psychological Review, 94(4), 427-438.
pmid: 3317470 |
[11] | Goldman A. I. (2006). Simulating minds: The philosophy, psychology and neuroscience of mindreading. Oxford: Oxford University Press. |
[12] | Gong X., Huang Y. X., Wang Y., & Luo Y. J. (2011). Revision of the Chinese facial affective picture system. Chinese Mental Health Journal, 25(1), 40-46. |
[ 龚栩, 黄宇霞, 王妍, 罗跃嘉. (2011). 中国面孔表情图片系统的修订. 中国心理卫生杂志, 25(1), 40-46.] | |
[13] |
Hareli S., David S., & Hess U. (2016). The role of emotion transition for the perception of social dominance and affiliation. Cognition and Emotion, 30(7), 1260-1270.
doi: 10.1080/02699931.2015.1056107 URL |
[14] |
Hoffmann H., Kessler H., Eppel T., Rukavina S., & Traue H. C. (2010). Expression intensity, gender and facial emotion recognition: Women recognize only subtle facial emotions better than men. Acta Psychologica, 135(3), 278-283.
doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.07.012 pmid: 20728864 |
[15] |
Hubbard T. L. (2005). Representational momentum and related displacements in spatial memory: A review of the findings. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 12(5), 822-851.
doi: 10.3758/BF03196775 URL |
[16] |
Hudson M., & Jellema T. (2011). Resolving ambiguous behavioral intentions by means of involuntary prioritization of gaze processing. Emotion, 11(3), 681-686.
doi: 10.1037/a0023264 pmid: 21534666 |
[17] |
Jellema T., Pecchinenda A., Palumbo L., & Tan E. G. (2011). Biases in the perception and affective valence of neutral facial expressions induced by the immediate perceptual history. Visual Cognition, 19(5), 616-634.
doi: 10.1080/13506285.2011.569775 URL |
[18] |
Kahneman D., & Tversky A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47(2), 263-292. https://doi.org/10.2307/1914185
doi: 10.2307/1914185 URL |
[19] |
Korolkova O. A. (2018). The role of temporal inversion in the perception of realistic and morphed dynamic transitions between facial expressions. Vision Research, 143, 42-51.
doi: S0042-6989(17)30250-X pmid: 29274357 |
[20] |
Li H. J., Xu F. M., Xiang P., Kong S. X., & Meng Z. Z. (2013). Reference dependence based on prospect theory. Advances in Psychological Science, 21(2), 317-325.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2013.00317 |
[ 李海军, 徐富明, 相鹏, 孔诗晓, 孟贞贞. (2013). 基于预期理论的参照依赖. 心理科学进展, 21(2), 317-325.]
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2013.00317 |
|
[21] | Liu J. C., Ran G. M., Zang Q., Hu Y. H., Yu X. H., Zhang Q. Z., & Yang D. (2021). The development of Chinese undergraduate dynamic affective facial database. Chinese Journal of Applied Psychology. 27(3), 234-243. |
[ 刘俊材, 冉光明, 张琪, 胡艳红, 余晓惠, 张琼之, 杨蝶. (2021). 中国大学生动态情绪面孔库的建设. 应用心理学, 27(3), 234-243.] | |
[22] |
Liu P., Lu Q., Zhang Z., Tang J., & Han B. (2021). Age- related differences in affective norms for Chinese words (AANC). Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 585666. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.585666
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.585666 URL |
[23] | Liu T. S., Ma H., Huang Y. X., Luo Y. J., Yan J., & Liu Z. W. (2006). Primary study of establishing an affective sound system of China. Chinese Mental Health Journal, 20(11), 709-712. |
[ 刘涛生, 马慧, 黄宇霞, 罗跃嘉, 严进, 刘伟志. (2006). 建立情绪声音刺激库的初步研究. 中国心理卫生杂志, 20(11), 709-712.] | |
[24] |
Liu W. J., Ding X. F., Cheng X. R., & Fan Z. (2022). Serial dependence effect: A novel “history effect”. Advances in Psychological Science, 30(10), 2228-2239.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2022.02228 URL |
[ 柳王娟, 定险峰, 程晓荣, 范炤. (2022). 序列依赖效应——一种全新的“历史效应”. 心理科学进展, 30(10), 2228-2239.]
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2022.02228 |
|
[25] |
Mehrabian A., & Russell J. A. (1974). The basic emotional impact of environments. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 38(1), 283-301.
pmid: 4815507 |
[26] |
Mei G., Chen S., & Dong B. (2019). Working memory maintenance modulates serial dependence effects of perceived emotional expression. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 1610. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01610
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01610 URL pmid: 31354595 |
[27] |
Navon D. (1977). Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception. Cognitive Psychology, 9(3), 353-383.
doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(77)90012-3 URL |
[28] |
Öhman A., & Mineka S. (2001). Fears, phobias, and preparedness: Toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psychological Review, 108(3), 483-522.
pmid: 11488376 |
[29] |
Palumbo L., & Jellema T. (2013). Beyond face value: Does involuntary emotional anticipation shape the perception of dynamic facial expressions? PloS One, 8(2), e56003. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0056003
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0056003 URL |
[30] |
Parducci A., & Wedell D. H. (1986). The category effect with rating scales: Number of categories, number of stimuli, and method of presentation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 12(4), 496-516.
pmid: 2946806 |
[31] | Prigent E., Amorim M., & Oliveira A. M. (2018). Representational momentum in dynamic facial expressions is modulated by the level of expressed pain: Amplitude and direction effects. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 80(1), 82-93. |
[32] |
Reinl M., & Bartels A. (2015). Perception of temporal asymmetries in dynamic facial expressions. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1107. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01107
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01107 URL pmid: 26300807 |
[33] |
Sato W., Kochiyama T., & Yoshikawa S. (2010). Amygdala activity in response to forward versus backward dynamic facial expressions. Brain Research, 1315, 92-99.
doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.12.003 pmid: 20025856 |
[34] |
Sato W., & Yoshikawa S. (2004). The dynamic aspects of emotional facial expressions. Cognition and Emotion, 18(5), 701-710.
doi: 10.1080/02699930341000176 URL |
[35] |
Sato W., & Yoshikawa S. (2007). Enhanced experience of emotional arousal in response to dynamic facial expressions. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 31(2), 119-135.
doi: 10.1007/s10919-007-0025-7 URL |
[36] |
Slepian M. L., & Carr E. W. (2019). Facial expressions of authenticity: Emotion variability increases judgments of trustworthiness and leadership. Cognition, 183, 82-98.
doi: S0010-0277(18)30267-1 pmid: 30445313 |
[37] |
Ueda Y., Nagoya K., Yoshikawa S., & Nomura M. (2017). Forming facial expressions influences assessment of others' dominance but not trustworthiness. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 2097. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02097
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02097 URL pmid: 29250018 |
[38] |
Warriner A. B., Kuperman V., & Brysbaert M. (2013). Norms of valence, arousal, and dominance for 13, 915 English lemmas. Behavior Research Methods, 45(4), 1191-1207.
doi: 10.3758/s13428-012-0314-x pmid: 23404613 |
[39] |
Wedell D. H., Parducci A., & Geiselman R. E. (1987). A formal analysis of ratings of physical attractiveness: Successive contrast and simultaneous assimilation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 23(3), 230-249.
doi: 10.1016/0022-1031(87)90034-5 URL |
[40] |
Wen F. F., Ke W. L., He S. F., Zuo B., Li L. X., Ma S. H., & Wang J. (2022). The effect of group identity shifting on impression updating in older adults: The mediating role of common ingroup identity. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 54(9), 1059-1075.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2022.01059 |
[ 温芳芳, 柯文琳, 何赛飞, 佐斌, 李兰心, 马书瀚, 王晶. (2022). 群体身份变换性对老年人印象更新的影响:共同内群体认同的中介作用. 心理学报, 54(9), 1059-1075.]
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2022.01059 |
|
[41] | Ying H., & Xu H. (2017). Adaptation reveals that facial expression averaging occurs during rapid serial presentation. Journal of Vision, 17(1), 1-19. |
[42] | Yoshikawa S., & Sato W. (2008). Dynamic facial expressions of emotion induce representational momentum. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, 8(1), 25-31. |
[43] | Zhai K., & Zhang Z. J. (2011). Theoretical models of representation momentum. Advances in Psychological Science, 19(4), 528-536. |
[ 翟坤, 张志杰. (2011). 表征动量的理论模型述评. 心理科学进展, 19(4), 528-536.] | |
[44] |
Zhang Q., Yin T. Z., & Ran G. M. (2015). Psychological and neural mechanisms for the superiority effect of dynamic facial expressions. Advances in Psychological Science, 23(9), 1514-1522.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2015.01514 |
[ 张琪, 尹天子, 冉光明. (2015). 动态面孔表情优势效应的心理机制及神经基础. 心理科学进展, 23(9), 1514-1522.]
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2015.01514 |
[1] | 晏碧华, 刘晓敏, 刘浩哲. 飞行场景中表征动量的地标吸引效应和排斥效应[J]. 心理学报, 2018, 50(7): 703-714. |
[2] | 董蕊. 表征动量的朝向效应[J]. 心理学报, 2015, 47(2): 190-202. |
[3] | 黄希庭,梁建春. 内隐时间表征的实验研究[J]. 心理学报, 2002, 34(03): 15-21. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||