Acta Psychologica Sinica ›› 2024, Vol. 56 ›› Issue (1): 29-43.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2024.00029
• Reports of Empirical Studies • Previous Articles Next Articles
TIAN Yangyang, LI Dong, YAN Xiangbo, LI Zhao, CUI Qian(), JIANG Zhongqing
Received:
2023-05-07
Published:
2024-01-25
Online:
2023-11-23
Contact:
CUI Qian
E-mail:jzqcjj@hotmail.com
TIAN Yangyang, LI Dong, YAN Xiangbo, LI Zhao, CUI Qian, JIANG Zhongqing. (2024). The representational momentum effect and the reference dependence effect on the evaluation of dynamic happy expressions. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 56(1), 29-43.
Add to citation manager EndNote|Ris|BibTeX
URL: https://journal.psych.ac.cn/acps/EN/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2024.00029
The average summary representation | |||
---|---|---|---|
75% happy | 25% happy | ||
direction of intensity change | From strong toweak | 100% happy – 50% happy | 50% happy – neutral |
From weak to strong | 50% happy – 100% happy | Neutral – 50% happy |
Table 1 Illustration of each experiment in Experiment 1
The average summary representation | |||
---|---|---|---|
75% happy | 25% happy | ||
direction of intensity change | From strong toweak | 100% happy – 50% happy | 50% happy – neutral |
From weak to strong | 50% happy – 100% happy | Neutral – 50% happy |
Figure 1. Schematic diagram of the dynamic change process of expression (source of faces in the figure: Gong et al., 2011). Note: A is 100% happy ? 50% happy; B is 50% happy ? neutral; C is 50% happy ? 100% happy; D is neutral ? 50% happy.
Figure 4. The influence of the average summary representation and intensity change direction on the dimensional evaluation of expression emotion. Note. “Strong?Weak” refers to the direction of intensity change “from strong to weak”, and “Weak?Strong” refers to the direction of intensity change “from weak to strong”.** indicates p < 0.01, ***indicates p < 0.001, error bars represent standard error, the same below.
Emotional dimension | Experimental conditions | M | t | df | p |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Valence | 100% Happy – 50% happy | 3.84 | –1.24 | 31 | 0.226 |
50% Happy – neutral | 3.08 | –7.17 | 31 | < 0.001 | |
50% Happy – 100% happy | 5.40 | 14.12 | 31 | < 0.001 | |
Neutral – 50% happy | 4.87 | 8.06 | 31 | < 0.001 | |
Arousal | 100% Happy – 50% happy | 3.97 | –0.25 | 31 | 0.803 |
50% Happy – neutral | 3.63 | –1.72 | 31 | 0.096 | |
50% Happy – 100% happy | 4.30 | 1.59 | 31 | 0.122 | |
Neutral – 50% happy | 3.93 | –0.51 | 31 | 0.616 | |
Dominance | 100% Happy – 50% happy | 4.24 | 2.28 | 31 | 0.029 |
50% Happy – neutral | 4.76 | 6.10 | 31 | < 0.001 | |
50% Happy – 100% happy | 3.22 | –5.38 | 31 | < 0.001 | |
Neutral – 50% happy | 3.49 | –5.15 | 31 | < 0.001 |
Table 2 One-sample t-tests for the three-dimensional scores of emotions for each group in Experiment 1 (test value of 4)
Emotional dimension | Experimental conditions | M | t | df | p |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Valence | 100% Happy – 50% happy | 3.84 | –1.24 | 31 | 0.226 |
50% Happy – neutral | 3.08 | –7.17 | 31 | < 0.001 | |
50% Happy – 100% happy | 5.40 | 14.12 | 31 | < 0.001 | |
Neutral – 50% happy | 4.87 | 8.06 | 31 | < 0.001 | |
Arousal | 100% Happy – 50% happy | 3.97 | –0.25 | 31 | 0.803 |
50% Happy – neutral | 3.63 | –1.72 | 31 | 0.096 | |
50% Happy – 100% happy | 4.30 | 1.59 | 31 | 0.122 | |
Neutral – 50% happy | 3.93 | –0.51 | 31 | 0.616 | |
Dominance | 100% Happy – 50% happy | 4.24 | 2.28 | 31 | 0.029 |
50% Happy – neutral | 4.76 | 6.10 | 31 | < 0.001 | |
50% Happy – 100% happy | 3.22 | –5.38 | 31 | < 0.001 | |
Neutral – 50% happy | 3.49 | –5.15 | 31 | < 0.001 |
The intensity of the last frame expression | |||
---|---|---|---|
50% Happy | 100% Happy | ||
Expression state | Dynamic | 100% Happy – 50% happy | 50% Happy – 100% happy |
Static | Static 50% happy | Static 100% happy |
Table 3 Illustration of each experimental treatment in Experiment 2a
The intensity of the last frame expression | |||
---|---|---|---|
50% Happy | 100% Happy | ||
Expression state | Dynamic | 100% Happy – 50% happy | 50% Happy – 100% happy |
Static | Static 50% happy | Static 100% happy |
Last frame expression | |||
---|---|---|---|
50% happy | Neutral | ||
Expression state | Dynamic | Neutral – 50% happy | 50% Happy – neutral |
Static | Static 50% happy | Static neutral |
Table 4 Illustration of each experimental treatment in Experiment 2b
Last frame expression | |||
---|---|---|---|
50% happy | Neutral | ||
Expression state | Dynamic | Neutral – 50% happy | 50% Happy – neutral |
Static | Static 50% happy | Static neutral |
The repeated stimulus condition | Emotional dimension | Position in Experiment 1 | Position in Experiment 2a | Position in Experiment 2b | t | p | Cohen’s d |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Static 50% happy | Valence | 3 | 2 | –4.60 | <0.001 | 1.19 | |
Arousal | 4 | 2 | –2.45 | 0.017 | 0.63 | ||
Dominance | 2 | 3 | 2.62 | 0.011 | 0.68 | ||
100% Happy – 50% Happy | Valence | 3 | 4 | 3.05 | 0.003 | 0.78 | |
Arousal | 2 | 3 | 0.68 | 0.51 | |||
Dominance | 2 | 1 | –2.79 | 0.007 | 0.71 | ||
50% Happy – 100% Happy | Valence | 1 | 1 | 0.66 | 0.51 | ||
Arousal | 1 | 1 | –0.88 | 0.38 | |||
Dominance | 4 | 3 | –2.20 | 0.031 | 0.56 | ||
Neutral – 50% Happy | Valence | 2 | 1 | –1.68 | 0.098 | 0.43 | |
Arousal | 3 | 1 | –2.05 | 0.044 | 0.52 | ||
Dominance | 3 | 4 | 0.18 | 0.86 | |||
50% Happy – neutral | Valence | 4 | 4 | –0.12 | 0.90 | ||
Arousal | 4 | 3 | 0.99 | 0.33 | |||
Dominance | 1 | 1 | 1.43 | 0.16 |
Table 5 The relative position of the ratings obtained for the repeated stimulus condition across experiments, and comparative results of differences in that condition across experiments.
The repeated stimulus condition | Emotional dimension | Position in Experiment 1 | Position in Experiment 2a | Position in Experiment 2b | t | p | Cohen’s d |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Static 50% happy | Valence | 3 | 2 | –4.60 | <0.001 | 1.19 | |
Arousal | 4 | 2 | –2.45 | 0.017 | 0.63 | ||
Dominance | 2 | 3 | 2.62 | 0.011 | 0.68 | ||
100% Happy – 50% Happy | Valence | 3 | 4 | 3.05 | 0.003 | 0.78 | |
Arousal | 2 | 3 | 0.68 | 0.51 | |||
Dominance | 2 | 1 | –2.79 | 0.007 | 0.71 | ||
50% Happy – 100% Happy | Valence | 1 | 1 | 0.66 | 0.51 | ||
Arousal | 1 | 1 | –0.88 | 0.38 | |||
Dominance | 4 | 3 | –2.20 | 0.031 | 0.56 | ||
Neutral – 50% Happy | Valence | 2 | 1 | –1.68 | 0.098 | 0.43 | |
Arousal | 3 | 1 | –2.05 | 0.044 | 0.52 | ||
Dominance | 3 | 4 | 0.18 | 0.86 | |||
50% Happy – neutral | Valence | 4 | 4 | –0.12 | 0.90 | ||
Arousal | 4 | 3 | 0.99 | 0.33 | |||
Dominance | 1 | 1 | 1.43 | 0.16 |
Figure 7. Schematic diagram of the process of evaluating emotions with dynamic expressions. Note. The dotted box above the arrow indicates the mechanical effect of each machining path.
[1] |
Ambadar Z., Schooler J. W., & Cohn J. F. (2005). Deciphering the enigmatic face: The importance of facial dynamics in interpreting subtle facial expressions. Psychological Science, 16(5), 403-410.
doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01548.x pmid: 15869701 |
[2] |
Bradley M. M., & Lang P. J. (1994). Measuring emotion: The self-assessment manikin and the semantic differential. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 25(1), 49-59.
doi: 10.1016/0005-7916(94)90063-9 pmid: 7962581 |
[3] |
Dong R. (2015). Facing orientation effect on representational momentum. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 47(2), 190-202.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2015.00190 |
[4] | Dozolme D., Prigent E., Yang Y. F., & Amorim M. A. (2018). The neuroelectric dynamics of the emotional anticipation of other people’s pain. PloS One, 13(8), e0200535. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0200535 |
[5] | Dyjas O., Bausenhart K. M., & Ulrich R. (2012). Trial-by-trial updating of an internal reference in discrimination tasks: Evidence from effects of stimulus order and trial sequence. Attention, Perception and Psychophysics, 74( 8), 1819-1841. |
[6] |
Ekman P., Freisen W. V., & Ancoli S. (1980). Facial signs of emotional experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39(6), 1125-1134.
doi: 10.1037/h0077722 URL |
[7] |
Fang X., van Kleef G. A., & Sauter D. A. (2018). Person perception from changing emotional expressions: Primacy, recency, or averaging effect? Cognition and Emotion, 32(8), 1597-1610.
doi: 10.1080/02699931.2018.1432476 URL |
[8] | Finke R. A., & Freyd J. J. (1985). Transformations of visual memory induced by implied motions of pattern elements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 11( 4), 780-794. |
[9] | Fiorentini C., & Viviani P. (2011). Is there a dynamic advantage for facial expressions? Journal of Vision, 11(3), 17. https://doi.org/10.1167/11.3.17 |
[10] |
Freyd J. J. (1987). Dynamic mental representations. Psychological Review, 94(4), 427-438.
pmid: 3317470 |
[11] | Goldman A. I. (2006). Simulating minds: The philosophy, psychology and neuroscience of mindreading. Oxford: Oxford University Press. |
[12] | Gong X., Huang Y. X., Wang Y., & Luo Y. J. (2011). Revision of the Chinese facial affective picture system. Chinese Mental Health Journal, 25(1), 40-46. |
[13] |
Hareli S., David S., & Hess U. (2016). The role of emotion transition for the perception of social dominance and affiliation. Cognition and Emotion, 30(7), 1260-1270.
doi: 10.1080/02699931.2015.1056107 URL |
[14] |
Hoffmann H., Kessler H., Eppel T., Rukavina S., & Traue H. C. (2010). Expression intensity, gender and facial emotion recognition: Women recognize only subtle facial emotions better than men. Acta Psychologica, 135(3), 278-283.
doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.07.012 pmid: 20728864 |
[15] |
Hubbard T. L. (2005). Representational momentum and related displacements in spatial memory: A review of the findings. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 12(5), 822-851.
doi: 10.3758/BF03196775 URL |
[16] |
Hudson M., & Jellema T. (2011). Resolving ambiguous behavioral intentions by means of involuntary prioritization of gaze processing. Emotion, 11(3), 681-686.
doi: 10.1037/a0023264 pmid: 21534666 |
[17] |
Jellema T., Pecchinenda A., Palumbo L., & Tan E. G. (2011). Biases in the perception and affective valence of neutral facial expressions induced by the immediate perceptual history. Visual Cognition, 19(5), 616-634.
doi: 10.1080/13506285.2011.569775 URL |
[18] |
Kahneman D., & Tversky A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47(2), 263-292. https://doi.org/10.2307/1914185
doi: 10.2307/1914185 URL |
[19] |
Korolkova O. A. (2018). The role of temporal inversion in the perception of realistic and morphed dynamic transitions between facial expressions. Vision Research, 143, 42-51.
doi: S0042-6989(17)30250-X pmid: 29274357 |
[20] |
Li H. J., Xu F. M., Xiang P., Kong S. X., & Meng Z. Z. (2013). Reference dependence based on prospect theory. Advances in Psychological Science, 21(2), 317-325.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2013.00317 |
[21] | Liu J. C., Ran G. M., Zang Q., Hu Y. H., Yu X. H., Zhang Q. Z., & Yang D. (2021). The development of Chinese undergraduate dynamic affective facial database. Chinese Journal of Applied Psychology. 27(3), 234-243. |
[22] |
Liu P., Lu Q., Zhang Z., Tang J., & Han B. (2021). Age-related differences in affective norms for Chinese words (AANC). Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 585666. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.585666
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.585666 URL |
[23] | Liu T. S., Ma H., Huang Y. X., Luo Y. J., Yan J., & Liu Z. W. (2006). Primary study of establishing an affective sound system of China. Chinese Mental Health Journal, 20(11), 709-712. |
[24] |
Liu W. J., Ding X. F., Cheng X. R., & Fan Z. (2022). Serial dependence effect: A novel “history effect”. Advances in Psychological Science, 30(10), 2228-2239.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2022.02228 URL |
[25] |
Mehrabian A., & Russell J. A. (1974). The basic emotional impact of environments. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 38(1), 283-301.
pmid: 4815507 |
[26] |
Mei G., Chen S., & Dong B. (2019). Working memory maintenance modulates serial dependence effects of perceived emotional expression. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 1610. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01610
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01610 URL pmid: 31354595 |
[27] |
Navon D. (1977). Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception. Cognitive Psychology, 9(3), 353-383.
doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(77)90012-3 URL |
[28] |
Öhman A., & Mineka S. (2001). Fears, phobias, and preparedness: Toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psychological Review, 108(3), 483-522.
pmid: 11488376 |
[29] |
Palumbo L., & Jellema T. (2013). Beyond face value: Does involuntary emotional anticipation shape the perception of dynamic facial expressions? PloS One, 8(2), e56003. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0056003
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0056003 URL |
[30] |
Parducci A., & Wedell D. H. (1986). The category effect with rating scales: Number of categories, number of stimuli, and method of presentation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 12(4), 496-516.
pmid: 2946806 |
[31] | Prigent E., Amorim M., & Oliveira A. M. (2018). Representational momentum in dynamic facial expressions is modulated by the level of expressed pain: Amplitude and direction effects. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 80(1), 82-93. |
[32] |
Reinl M., & Bartels A. (2015). Perception of temporal asymmetries in dynamic facial expressions. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1107. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01107
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01107 URL pmid: 26300807 |
[33] |
Sato W., Kochiyama T., & Yoshikawa S. (2010). Amygdala activity in response to forward versus backward dynamic facial expressions. Brain Research, 1315, 92-99.
doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.12.003 pmid: 20025856 |
[34] |
Sato W., & Yoshikawa S. (2004). The dynamic aspects of emotional facial expressions. Cognition and Emotion, 18(5), 701-710.
doi: 10.1080/02699930341000176 URL |
[35] |
Sato W., & Yoshikawa S. (2007). Enhanced experience of emotional arousal in response to dynamic facial expressions. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 31(2), 119-135.
doi: 10.1007/s10919-007-0025-7 URL |
[36] |
Slepian M. L., & Carr E. W. (2019). Facial expressions of authenticity: Emotion variability increases judgments of trustworthiness and leadership. Cognition, 183, 82-98.
doi: S0010-0277(18)30267-1 pmid: 30445313 |
[37] | Ueda Y., Nagoya K., Yoshikawa S., & Nomura M. (2017). Forming facial expressions influences assessment of others' dominance but not trustworthiness. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 2097. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02097 |
[38] |
Warriner A. B., Kuperman V., & Brysbaert M. (2013). Norms of valence, arousal, and dominance for 13, 915 English lemmas. Behavior Research Methods, 45(4), 1191-1207.
doi: 10.3758/s13428-012-0314-x pmid: 23404613 |
[39] |
Wedell D. H., Parducci A., & Geiselman R. E. (1987). A formal analysis of ratings of physical attractiveness: Successive contrast and simultaneous assimilation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 23(3), 230-249.
doi: 10.1016/0022-1031(87)90034-5 URL |
[40] |
Wen F. F., Ke W. L., He S. F., Zuo B., Li L. X., Ma S. H., & Wang J. (2022). The effect of group identity shifting on impression updating in older adults: The mediating role of common ingroup identity. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 54(9), 1059-1075.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2022.01059 |
[41] | Ying H., & Xu H. (2017). Adaptation reveals that facial expression averaging occurs during rapid serial presentation. Journal of Vision, 17(1), 1-19. |
[42] | Yoshikawa S., & Sato W. (2008). Dynamic facial expressions of emotion induce representational momentum. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, 8(1), 25-31. |
[43] | Zhai K., & Zhang Z. J. (2011). Theoretical models of representation momentum. Advances in Psychological Science, 19(4), 528-536. |
[44] |
Zhang Q., Yin T. Z., & Ran G. M. (2015). Psychological and neural mechanisms for the superiority effect of dynamic facial expressions. Advances in Psychological Science, 23(9), 1514-1522.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2015.01514 |
[1] | Bihua YAN, Xiaomin LIU, Haozhe LIU. Landmark attraction effect and landmark repulsion effect on representational momentum in airplane movement scene [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2018, 50(7): 703-714. |
[2] | DONG Rui. Facing Orientation Effect on Representational Momentum [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2015, 47(2): 190-202. |
Viewed | ||||||
Full text |
|
|||||
Abstract |
|
|||||