心理学报 ›› 2024, Vol. 56 ›› Issue (12): 1676-1690.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2024.01676
尹华站, 肖春花, 夏安妮, 袁中静, 崔晓冰, 李丹
收稿日期:
2024-02-18
发布日期:
2024-11-04
出版日期:
2024-12-25
基金资助:
YIN Huazhan, XIAO Chunhua, XIA Anni, YUAN Zhongjing, CUI Xiaobing, LI Dan
Received:
2024-02-18
Online:
2024-11-04
Published:
2024-12-25
摘要: 准确感知时间是人类生存的必要能力, 然而人类的时距知觉具有适应性, 会受诸多因素影响, 从而发生扭曲。基本情绪是扭曲时距知觉的重要因素, 但是以往研究存在诸多争议。为澄清该争议, 本文基于情绪分类观, 对基本情绪影响时距知觉的研究进行元分析。经过文献检索和筛选后, 共纳入36篇文献, 提供105个效应量, 涉及3795名被试。三水平随机效应模型发现, 基本情绪影响时距知觉的主效应显著, g = 0.25, 95% CI [0.13, 0.36], p < 0.001; 对每种基本情绪进行主效应分析, 结果发现高兴、恐惧和愤怒的时距知觉长于中性刺激。网络元分析发现, 恐惧和愤怒的时距知觉长于悲伤情绪的时距知觉。该模式符合适应性观点, 即情绪延长时距知觉是适应性的产物, 对“避害”越重要的情绪, 其诱发的时距知觉越长。
尹华站, 肖春花, 夏安妮, 袁中静, 崔晓冰, 李丹. (2024). 基本情绪对时距知觉的影响: 来自三水平元分析和网络元分析的证据. 心理学报, 56(12), 1676-1690.
YIN Huazhan, XIAO Chunhua, XIA Anni, YUAN Zhongjing, CUI Xiaobing, LI Dan. (2024). The influence of basic emotions on duration perception: Evidence from three-level meta-analysis and network meta-analysis. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 56(12), 1676-1690.
*为元分析纳入文献 [1] Angrilli A., Cherubini P., Pavese A., & Manfredini S. (1997). The influence of affective factors on time perception. Perception & Psychophysics, 59(6), 972-982. [2] Assink, M., & Wibbelink, C. J. (2016). Fitting three-level meta-analytic models in R: A step-by-step tutorial. The Quantitative Methods for Psychology, 12(3), 154-174. [3] Assink, M., & Wibbelink, C. J. (2024). Addressing dependency in meta-analysis: A companion to Assink and Wibbelink (2016). The Quantitative Methods for Psychology, 20(1), 1-16. [4] Balduzzi S., Rücker G., Nikolakopoulou A., Papakonstantinou T., Salanti G., Efthimiou O., & Schwarzer G. (2023). netmeta: An R package for network meta-analysis using frequentist methods. Journal of Statistical Software, 106(2), 1-40. [5] *Bar-Haim Y., Kerem A., Lamy D., & Zakay D. (2010). When time slows down: The influence of threat on time perception in anxiety. Cognition & Emotion, 24(2), 255-263. [6] Borenstein M., Hedges L. V., Higgins J. P. T., & Rothstein H. R. (2009). Introduction to meta-analysis. Wiley. [7] Borenstein M., Hedges L. V., Higgins J. P. T., & Rothstein H. R. (2010). A basic introduction to fixed-effect and random-effects models for meta-analysis. Research Synthesis Methods, 1(2), 97-111. [8] Borenstein M., Hedges L., Higgins J., & Rothstein H. R. (2014). Comprehensive meta-analysis (version 3.3) [computer software]. Englewood, NJ: Biostat. [9] Bradley M. M., Codispoti M., Cuthbert B. N., & Lang P. J. (2001). Emotion and motivation I: Defensive and appetitive reactions in picture processing. Emotion, 1(3), 276-298. [10] Brendel E., DeLucia P. R., Hecht H., Stacy R. L., & Larsen J. T. (2012). Threatening pictures induce shortened time-to-contact estimates. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74(5), 979-987. [11] Buhusi, C. V., & Meck, W. H. (2005). What makes us tick? Functional and neural mechanisms of interval timing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6(10), 755-765. [12] Burenkova O. V., Dolgorukova T. A., An I., Kustova T. A., Podturkin A. A., Shurdova E. M., ... Grigorenko E. L. (2023). Endogenous oxytocin and human social interactions: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 149(9-10), 549-579. [13] Cocenas-Silva R., Bueno J. L. O., & Droit-Volet S. (2013). Emotion and long-term memory for duration: Resistance against interference. Behavioural Processes, 97, 6-10. [14] Cochran, W. G. (1954). The combination of estimates from different experiments. Biometrics, 10, 101-129. [15] Cacioppo, J. T., & Gardner, W. L. (1999). Emotion. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 191-214. [16] Cheung, M. W.-L. (2014). Modeling dependent effect sizes with three-level meta-analyses: A structural equation modeling approach. Psychological Methods, 19(2), 211-229. [17] *Coy, A. L., & Hutton, S. B. (2013). The influence of hallucination proneness and social threat on time perception. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 18(6), 463-476. [18] *Cruz J. F., Vidaud-Laperrière K., Brechet C., & Charras P. (2020). Emotional context distorts both time and space in children. Journal of Behavioral and Brain Science, 10(9), 371-385. [19] Cui Q., Zhao K., & Fu X. L. (2018). The mode of action and cognitive neural mechanisms in emotional modulation of interval timing. Progress in Biochemistry and Biophysics, 45(4), 409-421. [崔倩, 赵科, 傅小兰. (2018). 情绪调节时间知觉的作用方式及认知神经机制. 生物化学与生物物理进展, 45(4), 409-421.] [20] Cui X., Ding Q., Yu S., Zhang S., & Li X. (2024). The deficit in cognitive reappraisal capacity in individuals with anxiety or depressive disorders: Meta-analyses of behavioral and neuroimaging studies. Clinical Psychology Review, 114, 102480. [21] Cui X., Tian Y., Zhang L., Chen Y., Bai Y., Li D., Liu J., Gable P., & Yin H. (2023). The role of valence, arousal, stimulus type, and temporal paradigm in the effect of emotion on time perception: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 30(1), 1-21. [22] Cui X., Zhang S., Yu S., Ding Q., & Li X. (2024). Does working memory training improve emotion regulation and reduce internalizing symptoms? A pair of three-level meta-analyses. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 179, 104549. [23] Curtis V., de Barra M., & Aunger R. (2011). Disgust as an adaptive system for disease avoidance behaviour. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366(1563), 389-401. [24] DeLucia P. R., Brendel E., Hecht H., Stacy R. L., & Larsen J. T. (2014). Threatening scenes but not threatening faces shorten time-to-contact estimates. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 76(6), 1698-1708. [25] Diano M., Celeghin A., Bagnis A., & Tamietto M. (2017). Amygdala response to emotional stimuli without awareness: Facts and interpretations. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 2029. [26] Droit-Volet, S. (2016). Emotion and implicit timing. PLOS ONE, 11(7), e0158474. [27] *Droit-Volet S., Brunot S., & Niedenthal P. (2004). Perception of the duration of emotional events. Cognition & Emotion, 18(6), 849-858. [28] *Droit-Volet S., Fayolle S., & Gil S. (2016). Emotion and time perception in children and adults: The effect of task difficulty. Timing & Time Perception, 4(1), 7-29. [29] Droit-Volet, S., & Gil, S. (2009). The time-emotion paradox. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364(1525), 1943-1953. [30] *Droit-Volet S., Lamotte M., & Izaute M. (2015). The conscious awareness of time distortions regulates the effect of emotion on the perception of time. Consciousness and Cognition, 38, 155-164. [31] Duval, S., & Tweedie, R. (2000). A nonparametric “trim and fill” method of accounting for publication bias in meta- analysis. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 95(449), 89-98. [32] *Eberhardt, L. V, Pittino, F., Scheins A., Huckauf A., Kiefer M., & Kliegl K. M. (2020). Duration estimation of angry and neutral faces: Behavioral and electrophysiological correlates. Timing & Time Perception, 8(3-4), 254-278. [33] *Effron D. A., Niedenthal P. M., Gil S., & Droit-Volet S. (2006). Embodied temporal perception of emotion. Emotion, 6(1), 1-9. [34] Egger M., Smith G. D., Schneider M., & Minder C. (1997). Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ, 315(7109), 629-634. [35] Ekman, P. (1992). An argument for basic emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 6(3-4), 169-200. [36] Ekman, P. (1993). Facial expression and emotion. American Psychologist, 48(4), 384-392. [37] Ekman, P. (1999). Basic Emotions. In T. Dalgleish & M.J. Power (Eds.), Handbook of cognition and emotion (pp. 45-60). Wiley. [38] Ekman, P., & Cordaro, D. (2011). What is meant by calling emotions basic. Emotion Review, 3(4), 364-370. [39] Emmer C., Dorn J., & Mata J. (2024). The immediate effect of discrimination on mental health: A meta-analytic review of the causal evidence. Psychological Bulletin, 150(3), 215-252. [40] Erdoğan, Ş., & Baran, Z. (2019). Effect of basic emotional facial expressions on time perception. Psikiyatride Guncel Yaklasimlar-Current Approaches in Psychiatry, 11(Suppl 1), 176-191. [41] *Fayolle, S. L., & Droit-Volet, S. (2014). Time perception and dynamics of facial expressions of emotions. PLoS ONE, 9(5), e97944. [42] Fernandes, A. C., & Garcia-Marques, T. (2020). A meta- analytical review of the familiarity temporal effect: Testing assumptions of the attentional and the fluency-attributional accounts. Psychological Bulletin, 146(3), 187-217. [43] Field, A. P., & Gillett, R. (2010). How to do a meta‐analysis. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 63(3), 665-694. [44] Fraisse, P. (1984). Perception and estimation of time. Annual Review of Psychology, 35(1), 1-36. [45] Fu X. L.(Ed.). (2015). Psychology of emotion. East China Normal University Press. [傅小兰. (主编). (2015). 情绪心理学. 华东师范大学出版社.] [46] Gan T., Luo Y. J., & Zhang Z. J. (2009). The influence of emotion on time perception. Journal of Psychological Science, 32(4), 836-839. [甘甜, 罗跃嘉, 张志杰. (2009). 情绪对时间知觉的影响. 心理科学, 32(4), 836-839.] [47] Gibbon J., Church R. M., & Meck W. H. (1984). Scalar timing in memory. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 423, 52-77. [48] *Gil, S., & Droit-Volet, S. (2011). “Time flies in the presence of angry faces”… depending on the temporal task used! Acta Psychologica, 136(3), 354-362. [49] *Gil S., Niedenthal P. M., & Droit-Volet S. (2007). Anger and time perception in children. Emotion, 7(1), 219-225. [50] *Grondin S., Laflamme V., Bienvenue P., Labonté K., & Roy M.-L. (2015). Sex effect in the temporal perception of faces expressing anger and shame. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 28, 0-12. [51] Grondin S., Laflamme V., & Gontier É. (2014). Effect on perceived duration and sensitivity to time when observing disgusted faces and disgusting mutilation pictures. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 76(6), 1522-1534. [52] *Guo X. J., Ren W. C., & Zhang Z. J. (2023). The effect of threat-related emotions on time perception: Based on the emotional motivation dimension. Studies of Psychology and Behavior, 21(03), 289-295. *[郭秀娟, 任维聪, 张志杰. (2023). 威胁性情绪对时间知觉的影响——基于情绪动机理论的视角. 心理与行为研究, 21(03), 289-295.] [53] Harrington D. L., Castillo G. N., Fong C. H., & Reed J. D. (2011). Neural underpinnings of distortions in the experience of time across senses. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 5, 32. [54] Higgins J. P. T., Thompson S. G., Deeks J. J., & Altman D. G. (2003). Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. BMJ, 327(7414), 557-560. [55] *Hosseini Houripasand M., Sabaghypour S., Farkhondeh Tale Navi F., & Nazari M. A. (2023). Time distortions induced by high-arousing emotional compared to low-arousing neutral faces: An event-related potential study. Psychological Research, 87(6), 1836-1847. [56] Huang S. H., Liu P. D., Li Q. Q., Chen Y. G., & Huang X. T. (2018). The influence of facial expressions of pain on subsecond and suprasecond time perception. Journal of Psychological Science, 41(2), 278-284. [黄顺航, 刘培朵, 李庆庆, 陈有国, 黄希庭. (2018). 疼痛表情对秒下及秒上时距知觉的影响. 心理科学, 41(2), 278-284.] [57] Huang X. T., Li B. Y., & Zhang Z. J. (2003). The research of the range-synthetic model of temporal cognition. Journal of Southwest China Normal University (Humanities and Social Sciences Edition), 29(2), 5-9. [黄希庭, 李伯约, 张志杰. (2003). 时间认知分段综合模型的探讨. 西南师范大学学报(人文社会科学版), 29(2), 5-9.] [58] Jia L., Shi Z., Zang X., & Müller H. J. (2013). Concurrent emotional pictures modulate temporal order judgments of spatially separated audio-tactile stimuli. Brain Research, 1537, 156-163. [59] *Jones C. R. G., Lambrechts A., & Gaigg S. B. (2017). Using time perception to explore implicit sensitivity to emotional stimuli in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 47(7), 2054-2066. [60] *Kliegl K. M., Limbrecht-Ecklundt K., Dürr L., Traue H. C., & Huckauf A. (2015). The complex duration perception of emotional faces: Effects of face direction. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 262. [61] Knapp, G., & Hartung, J. (2003). Improved tests for a random effects meta‐regression with a single covariate. Statistics in Medicine, 22(17), 2693-2710. [62] Lake J. I., LaBar K. S., & Meck W. H. (2016). Emotional modulation of interval timing and time perception. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 64, 403-420. [63] Lang P. J., Greenwald M. K., Bradley M. M., & Hamm A. O. (1993). Looking at pictures: Affective, facial, visceral, and behavioral reactions. Psychophysiology, 30(3), 261-273. [64] *Lee K.-H., Seelam K., & O’Brien T. (2011). The relativity of time perception produced by facial emotion stimuli. Cognition & Emotion, 25(8), 1471-1480. [65] Leppänen, J. M., & Nelson, C. A. (2012). Early development of fear processing. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(3), 200-204. [66] Levenson, R. W. (2011). Basic emotion questions. Emotion Review, 3(4), 379-386. [67] Li C., Xuan Y., Bruns P., & Fu X. (2024). The role of arousal in the estimation of time-to-collision of threatening stimuli. PsyCh Journal, 13(3), 376-386. https://doi.org/10.1002/pchj.762 [68] *Li D., Liu S. G., Bai Y. L., Cui X. B., Shi Y., & Yin H. Z. (2021). Effects of facial expressions of fear on perception of time of individuals of different ages: Moderating effect of task difficulty. Chinese Journal of Clinical Psychology, 29(6), 1119-1126. *[李丹, 刘思格, 白幼玲, 崔晓冰, 史焱, 尹华站. (2021). 恐惧面孔影响不同年龄个体时距知觉: 任务难度的调节作用. 中国临床心理学杂志, 29(6), 1119-1126.] [69] *Li, D., & Yin, H. Z. (2019). The influence of fear faces on the perception of duration among different age groups. Journal of Psychological Science, 42(5), 1061-1068. *[李丹, 尹华站. (2019). 恐惧情绪面孔影响不同年龄个体时距知觉的研究. 心理科学, 42(5), 1061-1068.] [70] Li, L., & Tian, Y. (2020). Aesthetic preference and time: Preferred painting dilates time perception. SAGE Open, 10(3), 215824402093990. [71] *Li, W. O., & Yuen, K. S. L. (2015). The perception of time while perceiving dynamic emotional faces. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1248. [72] Lin C. D.(Ed.). (2018). Psychology of development. People's Education Press. [林崇德. (主编). (2018). 发展心理学. 人民教育出版社.] [73] Liu, J. Y., & Li, H. (2019). How state anxiety influences time perception: Moderated mediating effect of cognitive appraisal and attentional bias. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 51(7), 747-758. [刘静远, 李虹. (2019). 状态焦虑对时距知觉的影响: 认知评价和注意偏向有调节的中介作用. 心理学报, 51(7), 747-758.] [74] Liu Y. Z., Zhang D. D., & Luo Y. J. (2013). Early development of the social and emotional brain in infancy. Chinese Science Bulletin, 58(9), 753-761. [柳昀哲, 张丹丹, 罗跃嘉. (2013). 婴儿社会和情绪脑机制的早期发展. 科学通报, 58(9), 753-761.] [75] Ma X., Tao Y., & Hu W. Q. (2009). Emotion effects on interval timing. Advances in Psychological Science, 17(1), 29-36. [马谐, 陶云, 胡文钦. (2009). 时距知觉中的情绪效应. 心理科学进展, 17(1), 29-36.] [76] Matthews, W. J., & Meck, W. H. (2014). Time perception: The bad news and the good. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 5(4), 429-446. [77] *Micillo L., Stablum F., & Mioni G. (2021). Do the young and the old perceive emotional intervals differently when shown on a younger or older face? Cognitive Processing, 22(4), 691-699. [78] *Min, Y., & Kim, S.-H. (2023). How do looming and receding emotional faces modulate duration perception? Perceptual and Motor Skills, 130(1), 54-79. [79] *Mioni G., Grondin S., Meligrana L., Perini F., Bartolomei L., & Stablum F. (2018). Effects of happy and sad facial expressions on the perception of time in Parkinson’s disease patients with mild cognitive impairment. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 40(2), 123-138. [80] *Mioni G., Grondin S., & Stablum F. (2021). Do I dislike what you dislike? Investigating the effect of disgust on time processing. Psychological Research, 85(7), 2742-2754. [81] *Mondillon L., Niedenthal P. M., Gil S., & Droit-Volet S. (2007). Imitation of in-group versus out-group members’ facial expressions of anger: A test with a time perception task. Social Neuroscience, 2(3-4), 223-237. [82] Mroczek, D. K., & Kolarz, C. M. (1998). The effect of age on positive and negative affect: A developmental perspective on happiness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(5), 1333-1349. [83] Nesse, R. M. (1990). Evolutionary explanations of emotions. Human Nature, 1(3), 261-289. [84] *Nicol J. R., Tanner J., & Clarke K. (2013). Perceived duration of emotional events: Evidence for a positivity effect in older adults. Experimental Aging Research, 39(5), 565-578. [85] Oaten M., Stevenson R. J., & Case T. I. (2009). Disgust as a disease-avoidance mechanism. Psychological Bulletin, 135(2), 303-321. [86] Palermo, R. (2017). The function of moods and emotions: Comment on “can sadness be good for you? On the cognitive, motivational and interpersonal benefits of mild negative affect” (forgas, 2017). Australian Psychologist, 52(1), 14-17. [87] Qian Y., Jiang S., Jing X., Shi Y., Qin H., Xin B., Chi L., & Wu B. (2021). Effects of 15-day head-down bed rest on emotional time perception. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 732362. [88] *Qu F., Shi X., Dai J., Gao T., Wang H., & Gu C. (2023). Dynamic and static angry faces influence time perception differently—Evidence from ERPs. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 17, 1124929. [89] Qu F., Shi X., Zhang A., & Gu C. (2021). Development of young children’s time perception: Effect of age and emotional localization. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 688165. [90] Ren W. C., Ma J., & Zhang Z. J. (2019). Age-related changes in time perception and the underlying neurobiological mechanism. Progress in Biochemistry and Biophysics, 46(1), 63-72. [任维聪, 马将, 张志杰. (2019). 时间认知的年老化及其神经机制. 生物化学与生物物理进展, 46(1), 63-72.] [91] Rodgers, M. A., & Pustejovsky, J. E. (2021). Evaluating meta-analytic methods to detect selective reporting in the presence of dependent effect sizes. Psychological Methods, 26(2), 141-160. [92] Rücker, G. (2012). Network meta‐analysis, electrical networks and graph theory. Research Synthesis Methods, 3(4), 312-324. [93] Russell, J. A. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39(6), 1161-1178. [94] Russell, J. A. (2014). Four perspectives on the psychology of emotion: An introduction. Emotion Review, 6(4), 291-291. [95] Schindler, J., & Richter, T. (2023). Text generation benefits learning: A meta-analytic review. Educational Psychology Review, 35(2), 44. [96] Schirmer A., Ng T., Escoffier N., & Penney T. B. (2016). Emotional voices distort time: Behavioral and neural correlates. Timing & Time Perception, 4(1), 79-98. [97] Shim S. R., Kim S.-J., Lee J., & Rücker G. (2019). Network meta-analysis: Application and practice using R software. Epidemiology and Health, 41, e2019013. [98] Silvers J. A., Shu J., Hubbard A. D., Weber J., & Ochsner K. N. (2015). Concurrent and lasting effects of emotion regulation on amygdala response in adolescence and young adulthood. Developmental Science, 18(5), 771-784. [99] Smith S. D., McIver T. A., Di Nella, M. S. J., & Crease M. L. (2011). The effects of valence and arousal on the emotional modulation of time perception: Evidence for multiple stages of processing. Emotion, 11(6), 1305-1313. [100] Sterne, J. A., & Egger, M. (2001). Funnel plots for detecting bias in meta-analysis: Guidelines on choice of axis. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 54(10), 1046-1055. [101] Stevenson R. J., Case T. I., Oaten M. J., Stafford L., & Saluja S. (2019). A proximal perspective on disgust. Emotion Review, 11(3), 209-225. [102] *Tao, Y., & Ma, X. (2010). Facial emotion and interval perception in children aged 3 to 8 years. Psychological Development and Education, 3, 225-232. *[陶云, 马谐. (2010). 面孔情绪下3~8岁儿童时距知觉的实验研究. 心理发展与教育, 3, 225-232.] [103] Thoenes, S., & Oberfeld, D. (2017). Meta-analysis of time perception and temporal processing in schizophrenia: Differential effects on precision and accuracy. Clinical Psychology Review, 54, 44-64. [104] Tian Y., Liu P., & Huang X. (2018). The role of emotion regulation in reducing emotional distortions of duration perception. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 347. [105] *Tipples, J. (2008). Negative emotionality influences the effects of emotion on time perception. Emotion, 8(1), 127-131. [106] *Tipples, J. (2011). When time stands still: Fear-specific modulation of temporal bias due to threat. Emotion, 11(1), 74-80. [107] *Tipples J., Brattan V., & Johnston P. (2015). Facial emotion modulates the neural mechanisms responsible for short interval time perception. Brain Topography, 28(1), 104-112. [108] *Tipples J., Lupton M., & George D. (2023). Temporal distortion for angry faces: Testing visual attention and action preparation accounts. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 77(9), 1800-1812. [109] *Tomas, J., & Španić, A. M. (2016). Angry and beautiful: The interactive effect of facial expression and attractiveness on time perception. Psychological Topics, 25(2), 299-315. [110] Tracy, J. L. (2014). An evolutionary approach to understanding distinct emotions. Emotion Review, 6(4), 308-312. [111] Treisman, M. (1963). Temporal discrimination and the indifference interval: Implications for a model of the “internal clock”. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 77(13), 1-31. [112] Viechtbauer, W. (2010). Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package. Journal of Statistical Software, 36(3), 1-48. [113] Viechtbauer, W., & Cheung, M. W. L. (2010). Outlier and influence diagnostics for meta-analysis. Research synthesis methods, 1(2), 112-125. [114] Visalli A., Begliomini C., & Mioni G. (2023). The effect of emotion intensity on time perception: A study with transcranial random noise stimulation. Experimental Brain Research, 241(8), 2179-2190. [115] Wang N., Wang J. Y., & L F. (2016). Neurophysiological mechanisms and effects of emotional regulation on time perception. Acta Physiologica Sinica, 68(4), 464-474. [王宁, 王锦琰, 罗非. (2016). 情绪对时间知觉的影响及其神经生理学机制. 生理学报, 68(4), 464-474.] [116] Wearden, J. H. (2003). Applying the scalar timing model to human time psychology: Progress and challenges. In H. Helfrich (Ed.), Time and Mind II: Information Processing Perspectives (pp. 21-39). Hogrefe & Huber Publishers. [117] Wearden, J. H., & Lejeune, H. (2008). Scalar properties in human timing: Conformity and violations. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61(4), 569-587. [118] Weng, C. C., & Wang, N. (2020). Animal research paradigm and related neural mechanism of interval timing. Advances in Psychological Science, 28(9), 1478-1492. [翁纯纯, 王宁. (2020). 时距知觉的动物研究范式及相关神经机制. 心理科学进展, 28(9), 1478-1492.] [119] Wu Z. L., He Y. Y., & Li H. (2009). What catastrophe leaved to our mind?Reviewing the roots causes psychological trauma and the brain mechanisms of post-traumatic stress reaction. Advances in Psychological Science, 17(3), 639-644. [伍泽莲, 何媛媛, 李红. (2009). 灾难给我们的心理留下了什么?——创伤心理的根源及创伤后应激反应的脑机制. 心理科学进展, 17(3), 639-644.] [120] *Yin H., Cui X., Bai Y., Cao G., Zhang L., Ou Y., Li D., & Liu J. (2021). The effects of angry expressions and fearful expressions on duration perception: An ERP study. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 570497. [121] *Young, L. N., & Cordes, S. (2012). Time and number under the influence of emotion. Visual Cognition, 20(9), 1048-1051. [122] Yuan J., Tian Y., Huang X., Fan H., & Wei X. (2019). Emotional bias varies with stimulus type, arousal and task setting: Meta-analytic evidences. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 107, 461-472. [123] Zakay, D., & Block, R. A. (1997). Temporal cognition. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 6(1), 12-16. [124] *Zhang D., Liu Y., Wang X., Chen Y., & Luo Y. (2014). The duration of disgusted and fearful faces is judged longer and shorter than that of neutral faces: The attention-related time distortions as revealed by behavioral and electrophysiological measurements. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 8, 293. [125] Zhang, F., & Zhao, G. X. (2019). The effect of emotion on duration perception among deaf undergraduates. Journal of Psychological Science, 42(4), 861-867. [张锋, 赵国祥. (2019). 情绪对聋哑大学生时距知觉的影响. 心理科学, 42(4), 861-867.] [126] Zhang M., Zhao D., Zhang Z., Cao X., Yin L., Liu Y., Yuan T.-F., & Luo W. (2019). Time perception deficits and its dose-dependent effect in methamphetamine dependents with short-term abstinence. Science Advances, 5(10), eaax6916. [127] Zheng W. Q., Zhang Y. C., Ma J. X., Zhao H., Ren Z. Y., & Zhang Y. (2021). Effects of movement on duration perception and the neural mechanism. Progress in Biochemistry and Biophysics, 48(7), 758-767. [郑玮琦, 张亦晨, 马佳欣, 赵晗, 任子媛, 张禹. (2021). 运动对时距知觉的影响及其神经机制. 生物化学与生物物理进展, 48(7), 758-767.] [128] Zhou S., Li L., Wang F., & Tian Y. (2021). How facial attractiveness affects time perception: Increased arousal results in temporal dilation of attractive faces. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 784099. |
[1] | 陆嘉琦, 李雨斯, 何贵兵. 双相障碍患者的风险决策偏好:来自三水平元分析的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2025, 57(1): 100-124. |
[2] | 苑明亮, 伍俊辉, 金淑娴, 林靓, 寇彧, Paul A. M. Van Lange. 中国社会陌生人之间合作行为的变迁:基于社会困境研究的元分析(1999~2019)[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(9): 1159-1175. |
[3] | 王祥坤, 辛自强, 侯友. 我国大中学生道德推脱水平的变迁及宏观成因[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(7): 859-875. |
[4] | 侯娟, 贾可可, 方晓义. 近20年中国夫妻婚姻满意度发展趋势与社会变迁[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(7): 895-910. |
[5] | 王碧瑶, 陈晨, 胡晓斐, 王迪, 李宝林. 视觉时距知觉序列依赖效应的空间迁移性[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(4): 394-411. |
[6] | 孟现鑫, 俞德霖, 陈怡静, 张玲, 傅小兰. 儿童期创伤与共情的关系:一项三水平元分析[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(8): 1285-1300. |
[7] | 李超平, 孟雪, 胥彦, 蓝媛美. 家庭支持型主管行为对员工的影响与作用机制:基于元分析的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(2): 257-271. |
[8] | 尹华站, 张丽, 刘鹏玉, 李丹. 负性情绪的动机维度对时距知觉的影响:注意控制和注意偏向的中介作用[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(12): 1917-1931. |
[9] | 靳娟娟, 邵蕾, 黄潇潇, 张亚利, 俞国良. 社会排斥与攻击的关系:一项元分析[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(12): 1979-1996. |
[10] | 孙亚茹, 刘泽军, 段亚杰, 陈宁, 刘伟. 协作如何减少记忆错误:一项元分析研究[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(11): 1780-1792. |
[11] | 陈必忠, 黄璇, 牛更枫, 孙晓军, 蔡志慧. 学步期至青年期社交焦虑的发展轨迹和稳定性:一项基于纵向研究的三水平元分析[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(10): 1637-1652. |
[12] | 廖友国, 陈建文, 张妍, 彭聪. 儿童青少年同伴侵害与内化问题的双向关系: 纵向研究的元分析[J]. 心理学报, 2022, 54(7): 828-849. |
[13] | 蓝媛美, 李超平, 王佳燕, 孟雪. 员工跨界行为的收益与代价:元分析的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2022, 54(6): 665-683. |
[14] | 辛素飞, 梁鑫, 盛靓, 赵智睿. 我国内地教师主观幸福感的变迁(2002~2019):横断历史研究的视角[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(8): 875-889. |
[15] | 张文芸, 李晓云, 姚俊杰, 叶倩, 彭微微. 自闭症谱系障碍个体的疼痛敏感性异常:来自元分析的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(6): 613-628. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||