心理学报 ›› 2023, Vol. 55 ›› Issue (6): 954-967.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2023.00954
兰泽波1,2, 郭梅华1,3, 姜琨4, 吴俊杰1, 闫国利1()
收稿日期:
2022-07-28
发布日期:
2023-03-06
出版日期:
2023-06-25
通讯作者:
闫国利
E-mail:psyygl@163.com
基金资助:
LAN Zebo1,2, GUO Meihua1,3, JIANG Kun4, WU Junjie1, YAN Guoli1()
Received:
2022-07-28
Online:
2023-03-06
Published:
2023-06-25
摘要:
听障者词汇识别过程是否表现出特异性一直是研究者关注的焦点问题, 然而当前观点并不一致。本研究采用语义相关性判断任务, 通过两个实验探讨语言经验和阅读能力对听障大学生词汇识别中字形、语音和手语表征激活的影响。实验1比较不同语言经验和阅读能力的听障大学生在形似干扰字、同音干扰字和无关干扰字条件下的表现, 实验2比较他们在手语相关和无关条件下的表现。实验1结果显示, 在正确率和反应时指标上, 不同语言经验和阅读能力的听障大学生表现出相似的字形干扰效应(与无关干扰字相比, 形似干扰字条件下正确率更低、反应时更长), 均未出现语音干扰效应。实验2结果显示, 在正确率指标上, 当控制听障大学生阅读能力时, 阅读能力高的手语组表现出显著的手语干扰效应(与无关条件相比, 手语相关条件下正确率更低), 阅读能力高的口语组未出现手语干扰效应。当控制听障大学生语言经验, 阅读能力高的手语组和阅读能力低的手语组表现出相似的手语干扰效应。综合两个实验的结果可以发现, 语言经验影响听障大学生的词汇表征, 阅读能力不影响听障大学生的词汇表征。在此基础上, 本研究尝试提出听障大学生中文词汇识别的认知加工模型。
中图分类号:
兰泽波, 郭梅华, 姜琨, 吴俊杰, 闫国利. (2023). 听障大学生词汇识别过程的特异性:语言经验和阅读能力的影响. 心理学报, 55(6), 954-967.
LAN Zebo, GUO Meihua, JIANG Kun, WU Junjie, YAN Guoli. (2023). The distinctness of visual word recognition in hearing-impaired college readers: The effects of language experience and reading ability. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 55(6), 954-967.
变量 | SOD组(n = 24) | SSD组(n = 24) | LSSD组(n = 16) | F |
---|---|---|---|---|
年龄 | 22.34 (0.30) | 22.76 (0.26) | 22.38 (0.35) | 0.66 |
口语使用比例 | 0.77 (0.03) | 0.17 (0.02) | 0.10 (0.03) | 182.90*** |
手语使用比例 | 0.23 (0.03) | 0.83 (0.02) | 0.90 (0.03) | 182.90*** |
口语水平 | 0.66 (0.03) | 0.31 (0.03) | 0.14 (0.05) | 58.90*** |
手语水平 | 0.50 (0.05) | 0.71 (0.03) | 0.64 (0.03) | 9.90*** |
智力(IQ) | 114.48 (2.37) | 112.19 (2.78) | 111.16 (4.07) | 0.32 |
阅读流畅性(字/分) | 462.65 (22.41) | 449.25 (19.32) | 262.50 (12.14) | 26.53*** |
听力损失程度(dB) | 94.10 (2.83) | 101.46 (2.57) | 106.44 (3.21) | 4.48* |
听障时间 | 1.16 (0.21) | 1.04 (0.17) | 0.84 (0.26) | 0.52 |
表1 听障大学生人口学信息比较[M (SE)]
变量 | SOD组(n = 24) | SSD组(n = 24) | LSSD组(n = 16) | F |
---|---|---|---|---|
年龄 | 22.34 (0.30) | 22.76 (0.26) | 22.38 (0.35) | 0.66 |
口语使用比例 | 0.77 (0.03) | 0.17 (0.02) | 0.10 (0.03) | 182.90*** |
手语使用比例 | 0.23 (0.03) | 0.83 (0.02) | 0.90 (0.03) | 182.90*** |
口语水平 | 0.66 (0.03) | 0.31 (0.03) | 0.14 (0.05) | 58.90*** |
手语水平 | 0.50 (0.05) | 0.71 (0.03) | 0.64 (0.03) | 9.90*** |
智力(IQ) | 114.48 (2.37) | 112.19 (2.78) | 111.16 (4.07) | 0.32 |
阅读流畅性(字/分) | 462.65 (22.41) | 449.25 (19.32) | 262.50 (12.14) | 26.53*** |
听力损失程度(dB) | 94.10 (2.83) | 101.46 (2.57) | 106.44 (3.21) | 4.48* |
听障时间 | 1.16 (0.21) | 1.04 (0.17) | 0.84 (0.26) | 0.52 |
变量 | 线索词 | 目标字 | 形似干扰字 | 同音干扰字 | 无关干扰字 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
示例 | 舒展 | 伸 | 坤 | 深 | 娴 |
词频/字频a | 96.80 (59.1) | 21.20 (11) | 21.20 (4.45) | 16.50 (3.61) | 15.10 (2.13) |
笔画数 | 17.10 (0.51) | 10 (0.30) | 9.96 (0.30) | 9.50 (0.25) | 9.73 (0.21) |
字形相似性 | - | - | 4.04 (0.04) | 1.21 (0.02) | 1.18 (0.02) |
表2 实验1的实验材料基本属性M (SE)
变量 | 线索词 | 目标字 | 形似干扰字 | 同音干扰字 | 无关干扰字 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
示例 | 舒展 | 伸 | 坤 | 深 | 娴 |
词频/字频a | 96.80 (59.1) | 21.20 (11) | 21.20 (4.45) | 16.50 (3.61) | 15.10 (2.13) |
笔画数 | 17.10 (0.51) | 10 (0.30) | 9.96 (0.30) | 9.50 (0.25) | 9.73 (0.21) |
字形相似性 | - | - | 4.04 (0.04) | 1.21 (0.02) | 1.18 (0.02) |
干扰字类型 | Hearing | SOD | SSD | LSSD | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
正确率 | 反应时 | 正确率 | 反应时 | 正确率 | 反应时 | 正确率 | 反应时 | |
形似干扰字 | 0.81 (0.01) | 717 (8) | 0.76 (0.02) | 754 (9) | 0.73 (0.02) | 776 (11) | 0.72 (0.02) | 756 (14) |
同音干扰字 | 0.90 (0.01) | 694 (8) | 0.90 (0.01) | 739 (9) | 0.89 (0.01) | 740 (10) | 0.84 (0.02) | 728 (13) |
无关干扰字 | 0.94 (0.01) | 689 (7) | 0.93 (0.01) | 721 (9) | 0.90 (0.01) | 736 (9) | 0.87 (0.02) | 704 (10) |
表3 实验1结果的平均值(标准误)
干扰字类型 | Hearing | SOD | SSD | LSSD | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
正确率 | 反应时 | 正确率 | 反应时 | 正确率 | 反应时 | 正确率 | 反应时 | |
形似干扰字 | 0.81 (0.01) | 717 (8) | 0.76 (0.02) | 754 (9) | 0.73 (0.02) | 776 (11) | 0.72 (0.02) | 756 (14) |
同音干扰字 | 0.90 (0.01) | 694 (8) | 0.90 (0.01) | 739 (9) | 0.89 (0.01) | 740 (10) | 0.84 (0.02) | 728 (13) |
无关干扰字 | 0.94 (0.01) | 689 (7) | 0.93 (0.01) | 721 (9) | 0.90 (0.01) | 736 (9) | 0.87 (0.02) | 704 (10) |
效应 | 正确率 | 反应时 | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
b | SE | z | p | 95% CI | b | SE | t | p | 95% CI | |
截距 | 2.09 | 0.13 | 15.68 | < 0.001 | [1.83, 2.35] | 6.59 | 0.02 | 336.07 | < 0.001 | [6.55, 6.63] |
SSD vs. SOD | −0.28 | 0.24 | −1.17 | 0.24 | [−0.74, 0.19] | 0.03 | 0.04 | 0.61 | 0.54 | [−0.06, 0.11] |
LSSD vs. SSD | −0.20 | 0.26 | −0.76 | 0.45 | [−0.70, 0.31] | −0.04 | 0.05 | −0.76 | 0.45 | [−0.13, 0.06] |
形似字 vs. 无关字 | 1.40 | 0.12 | 12.02 | < 0.001 | [1.17, 1.62] | −0.05 | 0.01 | −4.27 | < 0.001 | [−0.08, −0.03] |
同音字 vs. 无关字 | 0.28 | 0.13 | 2.19 | 0.03 | [0.03, 0.53] | −0.02 | 0.01 | −1.73 | 0.09 | [−0.04, 0.00] |
(SSD vs. SOD) × (形似字vs.无关字) | −0.22 | 0.28 | −0.80 | 0.43 | [−0.77, 0.32] | 0.01 | 0.02 | 0.36 | 0.72 | [−0.03, 0.05] |
(SSD vs. SOD) × (同音字vs.无关字) | −0.24 | 0.31 | −0.78 | 0.44 | [−0.85, 0.37] | 0.02 | 0.02 | 0.85 | 0.40 | [−0.02, 0.05] |
(LSSD vs. SSD) × (形似字vs.无关字) | −0.24 | 0.28 | −0.85 | 0.39 | [−0.79, 0.31] | −0.02 | 0.02 | −0.91 | 0.36 | [−0.07, 0.02] |
(LSSD vs. SSD) × (同音字vs.无关字) | 0.06 | 0.31 | 0.19 | 0.88 | [−0.55, 0.67] | −0.01 | 0.02 | −0.53 | 0.60 | [−0.05, 0.03] |
表4 实验1中线性混合模型分析结果
效应 | 正确率 | 反应时 | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
b | SE | z | p | 95% CI | b | SE | t | p | 95% CI | |
截距 | 2.09 | 0.13 | 15.68 | < 0.001 | [1.83, 2.35] | 6.59 | 0.02 | 336.07 | < 0.001 | [6.55, 6.63] |
SSD vs. SOD | −0.28 | 0.24 | −1.17 | 0.24 | [−0.74, 0.19] | 0.03 | 0.04 | 0.61 | 0.54 | [−0.06, 0.11] |
LSSD vs. SSD | −0.20 | 0.26 | −0.76 | 0.45 | [−0.70, 0.31] | −0.04 | 0.05 | −0.76 | 0.45 | [−0.13, 0.06] |
形似字 vs. 无关字 | 1.40 | 0.12 | 12.02 | < 0.001 | [1.17, 1.62] | −0.05 | 0.01 | −4.27 | < 0.001 | [−0.08, −0.03] |
同音字 vs. 无关字 | 0.28 | 0.13 | 2.19 | 0.03 | [0.03, 0.53] | −0.02 | 0.01 | −1.73 | 0.09 | [−0.04, 0.00] |
(SSD vs. SOD) × (形似字vs.无关字) | −0.22 | 0.28 | −0.80 | 0.43 | [−0.77, 0.32] | 0.01 | 0.02 | 0.36 | 0.72 | [−0.03, 0.05] |
(SSD vs. SOD) × (同音字vs.无关字) | −0.24 | 0.31 | −0.78 | 0.44 | [−0.85, 0.37] | 0.02 | 0.02 | 0.85 | 0.40 | [−0.02, 0.05] |
(LSSD vs. SSD) × (形似字vs.无关字) | −0.24 | 0.28 | −0.85 | 0.39 | [−0.79, 0.31] | −0.02 | 0.02 | −0.91 | 0.36 | [−0.07, 0.02] |
(LSSD vs. SSD) × (同音字vs.无关字) | 0.06 | 0.31 | 0.19 | 0.88 | [−0.55, 0.67] | −0.01 | 0.02 | −0.53 | 0.60 | [−0.05, 0.03] |
词对 类型 | 语义相关性 M (SE) | 启动词及 对应手语 | 目标词及 对应手语 |
---|---|---|---|
语义无关−手语相关 | 1.94 (0.07) | 花 | 电灯 |
语义无关−手语无关 | 1.91 (0.02) | 蜗牛 | 插头 |
表5 实验2材料示例
词对 类型 | 语义相关性 M (SE) | 启动词及 对应手语 | 目标词及 对应手语 |
---|---|---|---|
语义无关−手语相关 | 1.94 (0.07) | 花 | 电灯 |
语义无关−手语无关 | 1.91 (0.02) | 蜗牛 | 插头 |
手语相关性 | Hearing | SOD | SSD | LSSD | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
正确率 | 反应时 | 正确率 | 反应时 | 正确率 | 反应时 | 正确率 | 反应时 | |
手语相关 | 0.92 (0.01) | 736 (9) | 0.89 (0.01) | 820 (12) | 0.89 (0.01) | 888 (17) | 0.85 (0.02) | 803 (15) |
手语无关 | 0.96 (0.01) | 713 (7) | 0.93 (0.01) | 774 (9) | 0.97 (0.01) | 831 (12) | 0.94 (0.01) | 746 (11) |
表6 实验2中结果的平均值(标准误)
手语相关性 | Hearing | SOD | SSD | LSSD | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
正确率 | 反应时 | 正确率 | 反应时 | 正确率 | 反应时 | 正确率 | 反应时 | |
手语相关 | 0.92 (0.01) | 736 (9) | 0.89 (0.01) | 820 (12) | 0.89 (0.01) | 888 (17) | 0.85 (0.02) | 803 (15) |
手语无关 | 0.96 (0.01) | 713 (7) | 0.93 (0.01) | 774 (9) | 0.97 (0.01) | 831 (12) | 0.94 (0.01) | 746 (11) |
效应 | 正确率 | 反应时 | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
b | SE | z | p | 95% CI | b | SE | t | p | 95% CI | |
截距 | 3.15 | 0.21 | 15.17 | < 0.001 | [2.75, 3.56] | 6.65 | 0.03 | 264.53 | < 0.001 | [6.60, 6.70] |
SSD vs. SOD | 0.58 | 0.28 | 2.06 | 0.04 | [0.03, 1.12] | 0.06 | 0.05 | 1.22 | 0.23 | [−0.04, 0.16] |
LSSD vs. SSD | −0.62 | 0.32 | −1.95 | 0.05 | [−1.25, 0.00] | −0.08 | 0.06 | −1.41 | 0.16 | [−0.20, 0.03] |
相关 vs. 无关 | 1.07 | 0.34 | 3.13 | < 0.01 | [0.40, 1.74] | −0.06 | 0.02 | −2.91 | < 0.01 | [−0.10, −0.02] |
(SSD vs. SOD) × (相关vs.无关) | 1.05 | 0.36 | 2.94 | < 0.01 | [0.35, 1.74] | −0.01 | 0.02 | −0.29 | 0.77 | [−0.05, 0.04] |
(LSSD vs. SSD) × (相关vs.无关) | −0.50 | 0.40 | −1.26 | 0.21 | [−1.28, 0.28] | −0.01 | 0.03 | 0.30 | 0.76 | [−0.06, 0.04] |
表7 实验2中线性混合模型分析结果
效应 | 正确率 | 反应时 | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
b | SE | z | p | 95% CI | b | SE | t | p | 95% CI | |
截距 | 3.15 | 0.21 | 15.17 | < 0.001 | [2.75, 3.56] | 6.65 | 0.03 | 264.53 | < 0.001 | [6.60, 6.70] |
SSD vs. SOD | 0.58 | 0.28 | 2.06 | 0.04 | [0.03, 1.12] | 0.06 | 0.05 | 1.22 | 0.23 | [−0.04, 0.16] |
LSSD vs. SSD | −0.62 | 0.32 | −1.95 | 0.05 | [−1.25, 0.00] | −0.08 | 0.06 | −1.41 | 0.16 | [−0.20, 0.03] |
相关 vs. 无关 | 1.07 | 0.34 | 3.13 | < 0.01 | [0.40, 1.74] | −0.06 | 0.02 | −2.91 | < 0.01 | [−0.10, −0.02] |
(SSD vs. SOD) × (相关vs.无关) | 1.05 | 0.36 | 2.94 | < 0.01 | [0.35, 1.74] | −0.01 | 0.02 | −0.29 | 0.77 | [−0.05, 0.04] |
(LSSD vs. SSD) × (相关vs.无关) | −0.50 | 0.40 | −1.26 | 0.21 | [−1.28, 0.28] | −0.01 | 0.03 | 0.30 | 0.76 | [−0.06, 0.04] |
[1] | Bai X. J., & Yan G. L. (2017). Psychology of reading. Shanghai: East China Normal University Press. |
[白学军, 闫国利. (2017). 阅读心理学. 上海: 华东师范大学出版社.] | |
[2] | Bates D., Kliegl R., Vasishth S., & Baayen H. (2015). Parsimonious mixed models. arXiv preprint arXiv: 1506. 04967. |
[3] |
Bélanger N. N., Baum S. R., & Mayberry R. I. (2012). Reading difficulties in adult deaf readers of French: Phonological codes, not guilty! Scientific Studies of Reading, 16(3), 263-285.
doi: 10.1080/10888438.2011.568555 URL |
[4] |
Bélanger N. N., Mayberry R. I., & Rayner K. (2013). Orthographic and phonological preview benefits: Parafoveal processing in skilled and less-skilled deaf readers. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66(11), 2237-2252.
doi: 10.1080/17470218.2013.780085 URL |
[5] | Blythe H. I., Dickins J. H., Kennedy C. R., & Liversedge S. P. (2018). Phonological processing during silent reading in teenagers who are deaf/hard of hearing: An eye movement investigation. Developmental Science, 21(5), e12643. |
[6] | Cai Q., & Brysbaert M. (2010). SUBTLEX-CH: Chinese word and character frequencies based on film subtitles. PLoS ONE, 5(6), Article e10729. |
[7] | Cardin V., Orfanidou E., Rönnberg J., Capek C. M., Rudner M., & Woll B. (2013). Dissociating cognitive and sensory neural plasticity in human superior temporal cortex. Nature Communications, 4(1), 1473. |
[8] | Chen C. Y., Liu Z. F., Su Y. Q., & Cheng Y. H. (2018). The prediction effects for skill and less-skill deaf readers in Chinese reading: Evidence from eye movement. Psychological Development and Education, 34(6), 692-699. |
[陈朝阳, 刘志方, 苏永强, 程亚华. (2018). 高低阅读技能聋生词汇加工的语境预测性效应特点: 眼动证据. 心理发展与教育, 34(6), 692-699.] | |
[9] | Cheng Y. H., & Wu X. C. (2018). Reading fluency of first-graders predicted reading comprehension of second- and third-graders. Psychological Development and Education, 34(3), 314-321. |
[程亚华, 伍新春. (2018). 小学一年级阅读流畅性对二、三年级阅读理解的预测. 心理发展与教育, 34(3), 314-321.] | |
[10] |
Coltheart M., Rastle K., Perry C., Langdon R., & Ziegler J. (2001). DRC: A dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review, 108(1), 204-256.
doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.108.1.204 pmid: 11212628 |
[11] | Dehaene S. (2009). Reading in the brain: The new science of how we read. New York: Penguin. |
[12] |
Elliott E. A., Braun M., Kuhlmann M., & Jacobs A. M. (2012). A dual-route cascaded model of reading by deaf adults: Evidence for grapheme to viseme conversion. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 17(2), 227-243.
doi: 10.1093/deafed/enr047 pmid: 22159409 |
[13] | Emmorey K., & Lee B. (2021). The neurocognitive basis of skilled reading in prelingually and profoundly deaf adults. Language and Linguistics Compass, 15(2), e12407. |
[14] |
Fariña N., Duñabeitia J. A., & Carreiras M. (2017). Phonological and orthographic coding in deaf skilled readers. Cognition, 168, 27-33.
doi: S0010-0277(17)30177-4 pmid: 28646750 |
[15] | Feng M., Han Y., & Guo Q. (2017). An experimental study of the activation of sign language in hearing-impaired persons' Chinese characters recognition. Chinese Journal of Special Education, (11), 25-31. |
[冯敏, 韩媛, 郭强. (2017). 聋生汉语词汇识别过程中手语激活的实验研究. 中国特殊教育, (11), 25-31.] | |
[16] |
Friesen D. C., & Joanisse M. F. (2012). Homophone effects in deaf readers: Evidence from lexical decision. Reading and Writing, 25(2), 375-388.
doi: 10.1007/s11145-010-9275-6 URL |
[17] |
Hermans D., Knoors H., Ormel E., & Verhoeven L. (2008). Modeling reading vocabulary learning in deaf children in bilingual education programs. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 13(2), 155-174.
pmid: 18048369 |
[18] |
Hirshorn E. A., Dye M. W. G., Hauser P. C., Supalla T. R., & Bavelier D. (2014). Neural networks mediating sentence reading in the deaf. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 394.
doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00394 pmid: 24959127 |
[19] | Koo D., Crain K., LaSasso C., & Eden G. F. (2008). Phonological awareness and short-term memory in hearing and deaf individuals of different communication backgrounds. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1145(1), 83-99. |
[20] |
Kubus O., Villwock A., Morford J. P., & Rathmann C. (2014). Word recognition in deaf readers: Cross-language activation of German Sign Language and German. Applied Psycholinguistics, 36(4), 831-854.
doi: 10.1017/S0142716413000520 URL |
[21] | Lan Z. B., Liang X. W., Wang Z. G., Jiang K., Meng Z., & Yan G. L. (2020). Phonological processing during sentence reading in deaf college students: An eye-tracking study. Journal of Psychological Science, 43(4), 997-1003. |
[兰泽波, 梁晓伟, 王正光, 姜琨, 孟珠, 闫国利. (2020). 听障大学生句子阅读中语音加工的眼动研究. 心理科学, 43(4), 997-1003.] | |
[22] | Lan Z. B., Lin M., Song Z. M., Meng Z., Jiang K., & Yan G. L. (2022). Phonological activation during reading in deaf college students: Evidence from the tongue-twister effect. Journal of Psychological Science, 45(2), 491-497. |
[兰泽波, 林梅, 宋子明, 孟珠, 姜琨, 闫国利. (2022). 听障大学生阅读中的语音激活:来自绕口令效应的证据. 心理科学, 45(2), 491-497.] | |
[23] |
Lederberg A. R., Schick B., & Spencer P. E. (2012). Language and literacy development of deaf and hard-of-hearing children: Successes and challenges. Developmental Psychology, 49(1), 15-30.
doi: 10.1037/a0029558 URL |
[24] | Li D., Hu K. D., Chen G. P., Jin Y., & Li M. (1988). Test report of Raven's Progressive Matrices (CRT) of Shanghai city. Journal of Psychological Science, (4), 29-33. |
[李丹, 胡克定, 陈国鹏, 金瑜, 李眉. (1988). 瑞文测验联合型(CRT)上海市区试测报告. 心理科学通讯, (4), 29-33.] | |
[25] |
Li P., Zhang F., Yu A., & Zhao X. (2020). Language History Questionnaire (LHQ3): An enhanced tool for assessing multilingual experience. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 23(5), 938-944.
doi: 10.1017/S1366728918001153 URL |
[26] |
Li X., Huang L., Yao P., & Hyönä J. (2022). Universal and specific reading mechanisms across different writing systems. Nature Reviews Psychology, 1, 133-144.
doi: 10.1038/s44159-022-00022-6 |
[27] | Marian V., Blumenfeld H. K., & Kaushanskaya M. (2007). The Language Experience and Proficiency Questionnaire (LEAP-Q): Assessing language profiles in bilinguals and multilinguals. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50(4), 940-967. |
[28] |
Mayberry R. I., del Giudice A. A., & Lieberman A. M. (2011). Reading achievement in relation to phonological coding and awareness in deaf readers: A meta-analysis. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 16(2), 164-188.
doi: 10.1093/deafed/enq049 pmid: 21071623 |
[29] | Morey R. D., Rouder J. N., Jamil T., Urbanek S., Forner K., & Ly A. (2018). BayesFactor: Computation of Bayes factors for common designs. Version 0.9.12. from https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=BayesFactor |
[30] |
Morford J. P., Kroll J. F., Piñar P., & Wilkinson E. (2014). Bilingual word recognition in deaf and hearing signers: Effects of proficiency and language dominance on cross- language activation. Second Language Research, 30(2), 251-271.
doi: 10.1177/0267658313503467 pmid: 32982006 |
[31] |
Morford J. P., Occhino-Kehoe C., Piñar P., Wilkinson E., & Kroll J. F. (2017). The time course of cross-language activation in deaf ASL-English bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 20(2), 337-350.
doi: 10.1017/S136672891500067X URL |
[32] |
Morford J. P., Wilkinson E., Villwock A., Piñar P., & Kroll J. F. (2011). When deaf signers read English: Do written words activate their sign translations? Cognition, 118(2), 286-292.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2010.11.006 pmid: 21145047 |
[33] | Ormel E. (2008). Visual word recognition in bilingual deaf children (Unpublished doctorial dissertation). Radboud University, Nijmegen. |
[34] | Ormel E., & Giezen M. (2014). Bimodal bilingual cross- language interaction:Pieces of the puzzle. In M. Marschark, G. Tang, & H. Knoors (Eds.), Bilingualism and bilingual deaf education (pp. 74-101). Oxford University Press. |
[35] |
Ormel E., Hermans D., Knoors H., & Verhoeven L. (2012). Cross-language effects in written word recognition: The case of bilingual deaf children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15(2), 288-303.
doi: 10.1017/S1366728911000319 URL |
[36] | Pavani F., & Bottari D. (2012). Visual abilities in individuals with profound deafness:A critical review. In Murray, M. M., & Wallace, M. T. (Ed.), The neural bases of multisensory processes (pp. 421-445). CRC Press/Taylor & Francis. |
[37] |
Perfetti C. (2007). Reading ability: Lexical quality to comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11(4), 357-383.
doi: 10.1080/10888430701530730 URL |
[38] | Ren G. Q., Han Y. C., & Yu Z. (2012). The activation of orthography and phonology during Chinese sentence reading: Evidence from eye movements. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 44(4), 427-434. |
[任桂琴, 韩玉昌, 于泽. (2012). 句子语境中汉语词汇形、音作用的眼动研究. 心理学报, 44(4), 427-434.] | |
[39] | Sterne A., & Goswami U. (2000). Phonological awareness of syllables, rhymes, and phonemes in deaf children. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 41(5), 609-625. |
[40] |
Thierfelder P., Wigglesworth G., & Tang G. (2020a). Sign phonological parameters modulate parafoveal preview effects in deaf readers. Cognition, 201, 104286.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104286 URL |
[41] |
Thierfelder P., Wigglesworth G., & Tang G. (2020b). Orthographic and phonological activation in Hong Kong deaf readers: An eye-tracking study. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 73(12), 2217-2235.
doi: 10.1177/1747021820940223 URL |
[42] |
Traxler C. B. (2000). The Stanford Achievement Test: National norming and performance standards for deaf and hard-of-hearing students. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 5(4), 337-348.
pmid: 15454499 |
[43] |
Villwock A., Wilkinson E., Piñar P., & Morford J. P. (2021). Language development in deaf bilinguals: Deaf middle school students co-activate written English and American sign language during lexical processing. Cognition, 211, 104642.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104642 URL |
[44] | Xu Y., Pollatsek A., & Potter M. C. (1999). The activation of phonology during silent Chinese word reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 25(4), 838-857. |
[45] |
Yao P., Staub A., & Li X. (2022). Predictability eliminates neighborhood effects during Chinese sentence reading. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 29(1), 243-252.
doi: 10.3758/s13423-021-01966-1 |
[46] | Zhou X. L. (1997). The limitation of the phonology in semantic activation. In Peng, D. L., Shu, H., & Chen, H. -C. (Eds.), Cognitive Research on Chinese Language. Jinan, China: Shandong Education Press. |
[周晓林. (1997). 语义激活中语音的有限作用. 见:彭聃龄, 舒华, 陈烜之 (编). 汉语认知研究. 济南: 山东教育出版社..] |
[1] | 朱一鸣, 赵阳, 唐宁, 周吉帆, 沈模卫. 笔画节点在手写体汉字识别中的作用[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(12): 1903-1916. |
[2] | 尹华站, 张丽, 刘鹏玉, 李丹. 负性情绪的动机维度对时距知觉的影响:注意控制和注意偏向的中介作用[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(12): 1917-1931. |
[3] | 钟毅平, 牛娜娜, 范伟, 任梦梦, 李梅. 动作自主性与社会距离对主动控制感的影响:来自行为与ERPs的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(12): 1932-1948. |
[4] | 徐楚言, 朱麟, 王芸萍, 王瑞冰, 刘聪慧. 外语口语焦虑对言语互动质量的影响:fNIRS超扫描研究[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(12): 1949-1965. |
[5] | 汪海玲, 陈恩光, 连玉净, 李晶晶, 王丽薇. 面孔宽高比的自动加工[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(11): 1745-1761. |
[6] | 张航, 王婷, 冯晓慧, 韦义平, 张积家. 铜鼓经验对壮族鼓手的节奏知觉和执行功能的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(11): 1762-1779. |
[7] | 孙亚茹, 刘泽军, 段亚杰, 陈宁, 刘伟. 协作如何减少记忆错误:一项元分析研究[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(11): 1780-1792. |
[8] | 胡馨允, 沈悦, 戴俊毅. 系列决策任务中的策略转换:来自爱荷华赌博任务的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(11): 1793-1805. |
[9] | 邱慧燕, 吕勇. 情绪显著性对情绪诱发视盲的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(11): 1806-1814. |
[10] | 李梅, 李琎, 张冠斐, 钟毅平, 李红. 承诺水平与社会距离对信任投资的影响:来自行为与ERPs的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(11): 1859-1871. |
[11] | 王阳, 张琳爽, 崔楠楠, 吴岩. 4~6岁幼儿口语产生中句法结构和动词重复的作用:来自句法启动的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(10): 1608-1619. |
[12] | 李庆功, 方溦, 胡超, 石德君, 胡晓晴, 傅根跃, 王乾东. 灰姑娘能变成白雪公主吗?感知到的信任对他人面孔表征的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(9): 1518-1528. |
[13] | 庞超, 陈颜璋, 王莉, 杨喜端, 贺雅, 李芷莹, 欧阳小钰, 傅世敏, 南威治. 客体信息在视觉工作记忆编码和维持阶段的不同注意选择模式[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(9): 1397-1410. |
[14] | 蔡文琦, 张向阳, 王小娟, 杨剑峰. 汉语复合词语素意义与整词语义整合加工的时间进程[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(8): 1207-1219. |
[15] | 祖光耀, 李舒淇, 张天阳, 王爱君, 张明. 返回抑制对视听跨通道对应的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(8): 1220-1233. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||