[1] |
Ahn W. Y., Busemeyer J. R., Wagenmakers E. J., & Stout J. C. (2008). Comparison of decision learning models using the generalization criterion method. Cognitive Science, 32(8), 1376−1402. https://doi.org/10.1080/03640210802352992
|
[2] |
Ahn W. Y., Vasilev G., Lee S. H., Busemeyer J. R., Kruschke J. K., Bechara A., & Vassileva J. (2014). Decision-making in stimulant and opiate addicts in protracted abstinence: Evidence from computational modeling with pure users. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 849. https://doi.org/10.3389/ fpsyg.2014.00849
|
[3] |
Akaike H. (1974). A new look at the statistical model identification. IEEE Transactions on Automatic Control, 19(6), 716−723.
|
[4] |
Bechara A., Damasio A. R., Damasio H., & Anderson S. W. (1994). Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition, 50(1−3), 7−15. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(94)90018-3
|
[5] |
Bechara A., & Damasio H. (2002). Decision-making and addiction (part I): Impaired activation of somatic states in substance dependent individuals when pondering decisions with negative future consequences. Neuropsychologia, 40(10), 1675−1689. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0028-3932(02)00015-5
|
[6] |
Bechara A., Dolan S., Denburg N., Hindes A., Anderson S. W., & Nathan P. E. (2001). Decision-making deficits, linked to a dysfunctional ventromedial prefrontal cortex, revealed in alcohol and stimulant abusers. Neuropsychologia, 39(4), 376−389. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0028-3932(00)00136-6
|
[7] |
Bergert F. B., & Nosofsky R. M. (2007). A response-time approach to comparing generalized rational and take-the- best models of decision making. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 33, 107−129.
|
[8] |
Brandstätter E., Gigerenzer G., & Hertwig R. (2006). The priority heuristic: Making choices without trade-offs. Psychological Review, 113, 409−432.
|
[9] |
Bröder A., & Schiffer S. (2006). Adaptive flexibility and maladaptive routines in selecting fast and frugal decision strategies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 32, 904−918. https://doi.org/10.1037/ 0278-7393.32.4.904
|
[10] |
Broomell S. B., Budescu D. V., & Por H. H. (2011). Pair-wise comparisons of multiple models. Judgment & Decision Making, 6(8), 821−831.
|
[11] |
Burnham K. P., & Anderson D. R. (2004). Multimodel inference: Understanding AIC and BIC in model selection. Sociological Methods & Research, 33(2), 261−304. https://doi.org/10.1177/0049124104268644
|
[12] |
Busemeyer J. R., & Stout J. C. (2002). A contribution of cognitive decision models to clinical assessment: Decomposing performance on the Bechara gambling task. Psychological Assessment, 14(3), 253. https://doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590. 14.3.253
doi: 10.1037//1040-3590.14.3.253
URL
pmid: 12214432
|
[13] |
Clerc M. (2010). Particle swarm optimization (Vol. 93). John Wiley & Sons.
|
[14] |
Dai J., Kerestes R., Upton D. J., Busemeyer J. R., & Stout J. C. (2015). An improved cognitive model of the Iowa and Soochow Gambling Tasks with regard to model fitting performance and tests of parameter consistency. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 299. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00229
|
[15] |
Erev I., & Roth A. E. (1998). Predicting how people play games: Reinforcement learning in experimental games with unique, mixed strategy equilibria. American Economic Review, 88(4), 848−881.
|
[16] |
Fang J., Schooler L., & Shenghua L. (2023). Machine learning strategy identification: A paradigm to uncover decision strategies with high fidelity. Behavior Research Methods, 55(1), 263−284.
|
[17] |
Hochman G., Yechiam E., & Bechara A. (2010). Recency gets larger as lesions move from anterior to posterior locations within the ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Behavioural Brain Research, 213(1), 27−34. https:// doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2010.04.023
|
[18] |
Kahneman D., & Tversky A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47(2), 263−292.
|
[19] |
Lee M. D., & Gluck K. A. (2021). Modeling strategy switches in multi-attribute decision making. Computational Brain & Behavior, 4, 148−163. https://doi.org/10.1007/ s42113-020-00092-w
|
[20] |
Lee M. D., Gluck K. A., & Walsh M. M. (2019). Understanding the complexity of simple decisions: Modeling multiple behaviors and switching strategies. Decision, 6(4), 335−368. https://doi.org/10.1037/dec0000105
|
[21] |
Lee M. D., Newell B. R., & Vandekerckhove J. (2014). Modeling the adaptation of search termination in human decision making. Decision, 1(4), 223−251. https://doi.org/ 10.1037/dec0000019
|
[22] |
Li L., Zhang J. Q., Hou J. W., Li Y. L., Lu Y. J., & Guo Z. J. (2019). Decision-making characteristics assessed by the IOWA Gambling Task in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis. Journal of Qingdao University (Medical Sciences), 55(6), 688−691, 695.
|
|
[李蕾, 张俊青, 侯继文, 李亚铃, 鲁玉洁, 郭宗君. (2019). 爱荷华赌博任务评估精神分裂症决策特点Meta分析. 青岛大学学报(医学版), 55(6), 688−691, 695.]
|
[23] |
Luce R. D. (1959). Individual choice behavior: A theoretical analysis. New York: Wiley.
|
[24] |
Pachur T., & Galesic M. (2013). Strategy selection in risky choice: The impact of numeracy, affect, and cross-cultural differences. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 26, 260−271.
|
[25] |
Payne J. W., Bettman J. R., & Johnson.E. J. (1988). Adaptive strategy selection in decision making. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 14, 534−552.
|
[26] |
Rescorla R. A., & Wagner A. R. (1972). A theory of Pavlovian conditioning:Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In A. H. Black, & W. F. Prokasy (Eds.), Classical conditioning II: Current research and theory (pp. 64−99). Appleton-Century-Crofts.
|
[27] |
Rieskamp J., & Otto P. E. (2006). SSL: A theory of how people learn to select strategies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135(2), 207−236. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.135.2.207
|
[28] |
Schwarz G. (1978). Estimating the dimension of a model. The Annals of Statistics, 6(2), 461−464.
|
[29] |
Steingroever H., Fridberg D. J., Horstmann A., Kjome K. L., Kumari V., Lane S. D., … Wagenmakers E. J. (2015). Data from 617 healthy participants performing the Iowa Gambling Task: A “Many Labs” Collaboration. Journal of Open Psychology Data, 3(1), e5. http://doi.org/10.5334/ jopd.ak
|
[30] |
Stout J. C., Rodawalt W. C., & Siemers E. R. (2001). Risky decision making in Huntington's disease. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 7(1), 92−101. https://doi.org/10.1017/s1355617701711095
|
[31] |
Sugiura N. (1978). Further analysis of the data by Akaike’s information criterion and the finite corrections. Communications in Statistics, Theory and Methods, 7, 13−26. http://doi.org/10.1080/03610927808827599
|
[32] |
Sutton R. S., & Barto A. G. (1998). Reinforcement learning: An introduction. IEEE Transactions on Neural Networks, 9(5), 1054−1054. https://doi.org/10.1109/tnn.1998.712192
|
[33] |
Von Neumann J., & Morgenstern O. (1944). Theory of games and economic behavior. Princeton University Press.
|
[34] |
Wagenmakers E. J., Ratcliff R., Gomez P., & Iverson G. J. (2004). Assessing model mimicry using the parametric bootstrap. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 48, 28−50. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jmp.2003.11.004
|
[35] |
Walsh M. M., & Gluck K. A. (2016). Verbalization of decision strategies in multiple-cue probabilistic inference. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 29(1), 78−91. https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.1878
|
[36] |
Worthy D. A., Hawthorne M. J., & Otto A. R. (2012). Heterogeneity of strategy use in the Iowa gambling task: A comparison of win-stay/lose-shift and reinforcement learning models. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20(2), 364−371. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-012-0324-9
|
[37] |
Worthy D. A., Pang B., & Byrne K. A. (2013). Decomposing the roles of perseveration and expected value representation in models of the Iowa gambling task. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 640. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00640
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00640
URL
pmid: 24137137
|
[38] |
Xu S. H. (2012). Internet addicts’ behavior impulsivity: Evidence from the Iowa Gambling Task. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 44(11), 1523−1534.
|
|
[徐四华. (2012). 网络成瘾者的行为冲动性——来自爱荷华赌博任务的证据. 心理学报, 44(11), 1523−1534.]
|
[39] |
Yechiam E., Busemeyer J. R., Stout J. C., & Bechara A. (2005). Using cognitive models to map relations between neuropsychological disorders and human decision-making deficits. Psychological Science, 16, 973−978.
|
[40] |
Yechiam E., & Ert E. (2007). Evaluating the reliance on past choices in adaptive learning models. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 51(2), 75−84. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jmp.2006.11.002
|