心理学报 ›› 2021, Vol. 53 ›› Issue (2): 128-138.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00128
收稿日期:
2020-02-24
发布日期:
2020-12-29
出版日期:
2021-02-25
通讯作者:
陈安涛
E-mail:xscat@swu.edu.cn
基金资助:
ZHANG Mengke, LI Qing, YIN Shouhang, CHEN Antao()
Received:
2020-02-24
Online:
2020-12-29
Published:
2021-02-25
Contact:
CHEN Antao
E-mail:xscat@swu.edu.cn
摘要:
冲突适应是重要的认知控制现象, 反映了认知控制的动态过程。然而, 目前为止对于冲突水平上的差异能否诱发冲突适应尚不清楚。本研究采用字母Flanker的变式, 通过改变目标-分心物的一致性操纵冲突水平, 探究冲突水平的变化对认知控制调整的影响。结果发现被试的反应时间随冲突水平的提高而增加; 而且先前试次的一致性影响当前试次的干扰效应, 无冲突和低冲突、无冲突和高冲突、低冲突和高冲突条件之间均出现了典型的冲突适应。本研究结果表明, 除了冲突的有无, 冲突水平的变化也能诱发冲突适应, 支持更大的冲突驱动更强的认知控制, 而且冲突诱发的认知控制的功能可能通过注意聚焦实现。这一发现为冲突监测理论的注意调节机制提供了直接的支持证据, 对于促进冲突适应的相关研究具有重要意义。
中图分类号:
张孟可, 李晴, 尹首航, 陈安涛. (2021). 冲突水平的变化诱发冲突适应. 心理学报, 53(2), 128-138.
ZHANG Mengke, LI Qing, YIN Shouhang, CHEN Antao. (2021). Changes in the level of conflict trigger conflict adaptation. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 53(2), 128-138.
先前试次一致性 | 当前试次一致性 | ||
---|---|---|---|
无冲突 | 低冲突 | 高冲突 | |
无冲突 | 701.58 (65.86) | 763.27 (72.44) | 772.42 (78.31) |
3.36 (2.83) | 4.03 (2.96) | 3.68 (3.56) | |
低冲突 | 706.88 (67.91) | 722.55 (70.05) | 768.02 (71.22) |
2.64 (2.72) | 3.68 (4.94) | 3.76 (4.68) | |
高冲突 | 724.61 (69.82) | 755.61 (73.33) | 758.88 (65.31) |
3.09 (2.97) | 3.94 (4.64) | 3.77 (3.83) |
表1 先前试次一致性与当前试次一致性的反应时和错误率
先前试次一致性 | 当前试次一致性 | ||
---|---|---|---|
无冲突 | 低冲突 | 高冲突 | |
无冲突 | 701.58 (65.86) | 763.27 (72.44) | 772.42 (78.31) |
3.36 (2.83) | 4.03 (2.96) | 3.68 (3.56) | |
低冲突 | 706.88 (67.91) | 722.55 (70.05) | 768.02 (71.22) |
2.64 (2.72) | 3.68 (4.94) | 3.76 (4.68) | |
高冲突 | 724.61 (69.82) | 755.61 (73.33) | 758.88 (65.31) |
3.09 (2.97) | 3.94 (4.64) | 3.77 (3.83) |
交互作用 | df | F | p | ηp2 | 冲突适应模式 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
3 (无冲突、低冲突、高冲突) × 3 (无冲突、低冲突、高冲突) | 4, 120 | 13.06*** | <0.001 | 0.30 | |
3 (无冲突、低冲突、高冲突) × 2 (无冲突、低冲突) | 2, 60 | 19.70*** | <0.001 | 0.40 | |
2 (无冲突、低冲突) × 2 (无冲突、低冲突) | 1, 30 | 35.43*** | <0.001 | 0.54 | 经典 |
2 (无冲突、高冲突) × 2 (无冲突、低冲突) | 1, 30 | 22.86*** | <0.001 | 0.43 | 经典 |
2 (低冲突、高冲突) × 2 (无冲突、低冲突) | 1, 30 | 3.55 | 0.069 | 0.11 | 反转趋势 |
3 (无冲突、低冲突、高冲突) × 2 (无冲突、高冲突) | 2, 60 | 10.43*** | <0.001 | 0.26 | |
2 (无冲突、低冲突) × 2 (无冲突、高冲突) | 1, 30 | 1.17 | 0.287 | 0.04 | 无 |
2 (无冲突、高冲突) × 2 (无冲突、高冲突) | 1, 30 | 25.08*** | <0.001 | 0.46 | 经典 |
2 (低冲突、高冲突) × 2 (无冲突、高冲突) | 1, 30 | 9.90** | 0.004 | 0.25 | 经典 |
3 (无冲突、低冲突、高冲突) × 2 (低冲突、高冲突) | 2, 60 | 11.06*** | <0.001 | 0.27 | |
2 (无冲突、低冲突) × 2 (低冲突、高冲突) | 1, 30 | 11.97** | 0.002 | 0.29 | 反转 |
2 (无冲突、高冲突) × 2 (低冲突、高冲突) | 1, 30 | <1 | 0.529 | 0.01 | 无 |
2 (低冲突、高冲突) × 2 (低冲突、高冲突) | 1, 30 | 20.22*** | <0.001 | 0.40 | 经典 |
表2 先前试次一致性与当前试次一致性的反应时的统计分析结果
交互作用 | df | F | p | ηp2 | 冲突适应模式 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
3 (无冲突、低冲突、高冲突) × 3 (无冲突、低冲突、高冲突) | 4, 120 | 13.06*** | <0.001 | 0.30 | |
3 (无冲突、低冲突、高冲突) × 2 (无冲突、低冲突) | 2, 60 | 19.70*** | <0.001 | 0.40 | |
2 (无冲突、低冲突) × 2 (无冲突、低冲突) | 1, 30 | 35.43*** | <0.001 | 0.54 | 经典 |
2 (无冲突、高冲突) × 2 (无冲突、低冲突) | 1, 30 | 22.86*** | <0.001 | 0.43 | 经典 |
2 (低冲突、高冲突) × 2 (无冲突、低冲突) | 1, 30 | 3.55 | 0.069 | 0.11 | 反转趋势 |
3 (无冲突、低冲突、高冲突) × 2 (无冲突、高冲突) | 2, 60 | 10.43*** | <0.001 | 0.26 | |
2 (无冲突、低冲突) × 2 (无冲突、高冲突) | 1, 30 | 1.17 | 0.287 | 0.04 | 无 |
2 (无冲突、高冲突) × 2 (无冲突、高冲突) | 1, 30 | 25.08*** | <0.001 | 0.46 | 经典 |
2 (低冲突、高冲突) × 2 (无冲突、高冲突) | 1, 30 | 9.90** | 0.004 | 0.25 | 经典 |
3 (无冲突、低冲突、高冲突) × 2 (低冲突、高冲突) | 2, 60 | 11.06*** | <0.001 | 0.27 | |
2 (无冲突、低冲突) × 2 (低冲突、高冲突) | 1, 30 | 11.97** | 0.002 | 0.29 | 反转 |
2 (无冲突、高冲突) × 2 (低冲突、高冲突) | 1, 30 | <1 | 0.529 | 0.01 | 无 |
2 (低冲突、高冲突) × 2 (低冲突、高冲突) | 1, 30 | 20.22*** | <0.001 | 0.40 | 经典 |
[1] |
Algom, D., & Chajut, E. (2019). Reclaiming the Stroop effect back from control to input-driven attention and perception. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 1683.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01683 URL pmid: 31428008 |
[2] | Botvinick, M. M. (2007). Conflict monitoring and decision making: Reconciling two perspectives on anterior cingulate function. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 7(4), 356-366. |
[3] |
Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108(3), 624-652.
URL pmid: 11488380 |
[4] |
Botvinick, M. M., Cohen, J. D., & Carter, C. S. (2004). Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: An update. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(12), 539-546.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.10.003 URL pmid: 15556023 |
[5] |
Botvinick, M., Nystrom, L. E., Fissell, K., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D. (1999). Conflict monitoring versus selection-for-action in anterior cingulate cortex. Nature, 402(6758), 179-181.
doi: 10.1038/46035 URL pmid: 10647008 |
[6] |
Braem, S., Bugg, J. M., Schmidt, J. R., Crump, M. J. C., Weissman, D. H., Notebaert, W., & Egner, T. (2019). Measuring adaptive control in conflict tasks. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 23(9), 769-783.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2019.07.002 URL pmid: 31331794 |
[7] |
Chiu, Y. -C., & Egner, T. (2019). Cortical and subcortical contributions to context-control learning. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 99, 33-41.
doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.01.019 URL pmid: 30685484 |
[8] |
Clayson, P. E., & Larson, M. J. (2011). Conflict adaptation and sequential trial effects: Support for the conflict monitoring theory. Neuropsychologia, 49(7), 1953-1961.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.03.023 URL pmid: 21435347 |
[9] |
Danielmeier, C., Wessel, J. R., Steinhauser, M., & Ullsperger, M. (2009). Modulation of the error-related negativity by response conflict. Psychophysiology, 46(6), 1288-1298.
doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00860.x URL pmid: 19572907 |
[10] |
Dignath, D., Johannsen, L., Hommel, B., & Kiesel, A. (2019). Reconciling cognitive-control and episodic-retrieval accounts of sequential conflict modulation: Binding of control-states into event-files. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 45(9), 1265-1270.
doi: 10.1037/xhp0000673 URL pmid: 31380673 |
[11] |
Egner, T. (2008). Multiple conflict-driven control mechanisms in the human brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(10), 374-380.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.07.001 URL pmid: 18760657 |
[12] |
Egner, T. (2014). Creatures of habit (and control): A multi-level learning perspective on the modulation of congruency effects. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1247.
URL pmid: 25414679 |
[13] |
Egner, T., Delano, M., & Hirsch, J. (2007). Separate conflict-specific cognitive control mechanisms in the human brain. NeuroImage, 35(2), 940-948.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.11.061 URL pmid: 17276088 |
[14] |
Egner, T., Jamieson, G., & Gruzelier, J. (2005). Hypnosis decouples cognitive control from conflict monitoring processes of the frontal lobe. NeuroImage, 27(4), 969-978.
URL pmid: 15964211 |
[15] |
Eriksen, C. W., & Schultz, D. W. (1979). Information processing in visual search: A continuous flow conception and experimental results. Perception & Psychophysics, 25(4), 249-263.
URL pmid: 461085 |
[16] |
Eriksen, C. W., & ST., James, J., D. (1986). Visual attention within and around the field of focal attention: A zoom lens model. Perception & Psychophysics, 40(4), 225-240.
doi: 10.3758/bf03211502 URL pmid: 3786090 |
[17] | Flowers, J. H. (1980). Response priming effects in a digit naming task as a function of target-noise separation. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 16(6), 443-446. |
[18] |
Forster, S. E., Carter, C. S., Cohen, J. D., & Cho, R. Y. (2011). Parametric manipulation of the conflict signal and control-state adaptation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(4), 923-935.
doi: 10.1162/jocn.2010.21458 URL pmid: 20146615 |
[19] |
Funes, M. J., Lupiáñez, J., & Humphreys, G. (2010). Analyzing the generality of conflict adaptation effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36(1), 147-161.
doi: 10.1037/a0017598 URL pmid: 20121301 |
[20] |
Gratton, G., Coles, M. G. H., & Donchin, E. (1992). Optimizing the use of information: Strategic control of activation of responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 121(4), 480-506.
doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.121.4.480 URL |
[21] |
Grundy, J. G., & Shedden, J. M. (2014). A role for recency of response conflict in producing the bivalency effect. Psychological Research, 78(5), 679-691.
doi: 10.1007/s00426-013-0520-x URL pmid: 24146081 |
[22] |
Kerns, J. G. (2006). Anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex activity in an FMRI study of trial-to-trial adjustments on the Simon task. NeuroImage, 33(1), 399-405.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.06.012 URL pmid: 16876434 |
[23] |
Kerns, J. G., Cohen, J. D., MacDonald III, A. W., Cho, R. Y., Stenger, V. A., & Carter, C. S. (2004). Anterior cingulate conflict monitoring and adjustments in control. Science, 303(5660), 1023-1026.
URL pmid: 14963333 |
[24] |
Kim, C., Chung, C., & Kim, J. (2010). Multiple cognitive control mechanisms associated with the nature of conflict. Neuroscience Letters, 476(3), 156-160.
doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2010.04.019 URL pmid: 20399838 |
[25] |
Kim, C., Kroger, J. K., & Kim, J. (2011). A functional dissociation of conflict processing within anterior cingulate cortex. Human Brain Mapping, 32(2), 304-312.
doi: 10.1002/hbm.21020 URL pmid: 21229616 |
[26] |
Liu, P. D., Chen, A. T., Li, C., Li, H., & West, R. (2012). Conflict adaptation is reflected by response interference. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 24(4), 457-467.
doi: 10.1080/20445911.2011.651080 URL |
[27] | Liu, P. -D., Yang, W. -J., Tian, X., & Chen, A. -T. (2012). An overview of current studies about the conflict adaptation effect. Advances in Psychological Science, 20(4), 532-541. |
[ 刘培朵, 杨文静, 田夏, 陈安涛. (2012). 冲突适应效应研究述评. 心理科学进展, 20(4), 532-541.] | |
[28] |
Mayr, U., & Awh, E. (2009). The elusive link between conflict and conflict adaptation. Psychological Research, 73(6), 794-802.
doi: 10.1007/s00426-008-0191-1 URL pmid: 19034501 |
[29] |
Notebaert, W., Gevers, W., Verbruggen, F., & Liefooghe, B. (2006). Top-down and bottom-up sequential modulations of congruency effects. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13(1), 112-117.
doi: 10.3758/bf03193821 URL pmid: 16724777 |
[30] |
Notebaert, W., & Verguts, T. (2006). Stimulus conflict predicts conflict adaptation in a numerical flanker task. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13(6), 1078-1084.
doi: 10.3758/bf03213929 URL pmid: 17484439 |
[31] |
Notebaert, W., & Verguts, T. (2011). Conflict and error adaptation in the Simon task. Acta Psychologica, 136(2), 212-216.
doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.05.006 URL pmid: 21420518 |
[32] | Rajkowski, J., Kubiak, P., & Aston-Jones, G. (1993). Correlations between locus coeruleus (LC) neural activity, pupil diameter and behavior in monkey support a role of LC in attention. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts, 19, 974. |
[33] |
Rey-Mermet, A., & Meier, B. (2014). More conflict does not trigger more adjustment of cognitive control for subsequent events: A study of the bivalency effect. Acta Psychologica, 145, 111-117.
doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.11.005 URL pmid: 24333810 |
[34] |
Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18(6), 643-662.
doi: 10.1037/h0054651 URL |
[35] |
Takezawa, T., & Miyatani, M. (2005). Quantitative relation between conflict and response inhibition in the flanker task. Psychological Reports, 97(2), 515-526.
doi: 10.2466/pr0.97.2.515-526 URL pmid: 16342579 |
[36] | Ullsperger, M., Bylsma, L. M., & Botvinick, M. M. (2005). The conflict adaptation effect: It’s not just priming. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 5(4), 467-472. |
[37] |
Verbruggen, F., Notebaert, W., Liefooghe, B., & Vandierendonck, A. (2006). Stimulus- and response-conflict-induced cognitive control in the flanker task. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13(2), 328-333.
doi: 10.3758/bf03193852 URL pmid: 16893003 |
[38] |
Verguts, T., & Notebaert, W. (2008). Hebbian learning of cognitive control: Dealing with specific and nonspecific adaptation. Psychological Review, 115(2), 518-525.
doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.115.2.518 URL pmid: 18426302 |
[39] |
Verguts, T., & Notebaert, W. (2009). Adaptation by binding: A learning account of cognitive control. Trends in Cognitive Science, 13(6), 252-257.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.02.007 URL |
[40] |
Weissman, D. H., Egner, T., Hawks, Z., & Link, J. (2015). The congruency sequence effect emerges when the distracter precedes the target. Acta Psychologica, 156, 8-21.
doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2015.01.003 URL pmid: 25616120 |
[41] |
Weissman, D. H., Jiang, J., & Egner, T. (2014). Determinants of congruency sequence effects without learning and memory confounds. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40(5), 2022-2037.
doi: 10.1037/a0037454 URL pmid: 25089574 |
[42] |
Wendt, M., Kiesel, A., Geringswald, F., Purmann, S., & Fischer, R. (2014). Attentional adjustment to conflict strength: Evidence from the effects of manipulating flanker-target SOA on response times and prestimulus pupil size. Experimental Psychology, 61(1), 55-67.
doi: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000227 URL pmid: 24149239 |
[1] | 潘玥安, 姜云鹏, 郭茂杰, 吴瑕. 不确定性和预期有效性对运动方向感知决策的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2022, 54(6): 595-603. |
[2] | 陈莉, 石晓柯, 李维娜, 胡妍. 基于不同冲突水平的认知控制对性别刻板印象表达的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2022, 54(6): 628-645. |
[3] | 李建花, 解佳佳, 庄锦英. 生理周期对情景记忆的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2022, 54(5): 466-480. |
[4] | 吴建校, 曹碧华, 陈云, 李子夏, 李富洪. 认知控制的层级性:来自任务切换的脑电证据*[J]. 心理学报, 2022, 54(10): 1167-1180. |
[5] | 孙琳, 段涛, 刘伟, 陈宁. 特质正念对初中生学业情绪预测偏差的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(11): 1203-1214. |
[6] | 黄月胜, 张豹, 范兴华, 黄杰. 无关工作记忆表征的负性情绪信息能否捕获视觉注意?一项眼动研究[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(1): 26-37. |
[7] | 孙岩, 吕娇娇, 兰帆, 张丽娜. 自我关注重评和情境关注重评情绪调节策略及对随后认知控制的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(12): 1393-1406. |
[8] | 崔诣晨, 王沛, 崔亚娟. 知觉冲突印象形成的认知控制策略:以刻板化信息与反刻板化信息为例 *[J]. 心理学报, 2019, 51(10): 1157-1170. |
[9] | 王宴庆, 陈安涛, 胡学平, 尹首航. 奖赏通过增强信号监测提升认知控制[J]. 心理学报, 2019, 51(1): 48-57. |
[10] | 胡岑楼;张豹;黄赛. 无关长时记忆表征能否引导视觉注意选择?[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(5): 590-601. |
[11] | 张豹;胡岑楼;黄赛. 认知控制在工作记忆表征引导注意中的作用:来自眼动的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2016, 48(9): 1105-1118. |
[12] | 刘聪;焦鲁;孙逊;王瑞明. 语言转换对非熟练双语者不同认知控制成分的即时影响[J]. 心理学报, 2016, 48(5): 472-481. |
[13] | 王佳莹; 缴润凯; 张明. 任务设置影响负相容效应的机制 ——自上而下认知控制对阈下启动信息加工的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2016, 48(11): 1370-1378. |
[14] | 刘晓瑜;何朝丹;陈俊;邓沁丽. 熟练粤-普双言者的双言认知控制机制 ——来自双任务切换范式的行为研究证据[J]. 心理学报, 2015, 47(4): 439-454. |
[15] | 蒋军;向玲;张庆林;陈安涛. 冲突适应独立于意识:来自行为和ERP的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2014, 46(5): 581-592. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||