心理学报 ›› 2021, Vol. 53 ›› Issue (2): 113-127.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00113
• 研究报告 • 下一篇
收稿日期:
2020-03-05
发布日期:
2020-12-29
出版日期:
2021-02-25
通讯作者:
王慧媛,张明
E-mail:wanghuiyuan@ccsfu.edu.cn;psyzm@suda.edu.cn
基金资助:
WANG Hui-Yuan1(), CHEN Ai-Rui2, ZHANG Ming3,4()
Received:
2020-03-05
Online:
2020-12-29
Published:
2021-02-25
Contact:
WANG Hui-Yuan,ZHANG Ming
E-mail:wanghuiyuan@ccsfu.edu.cn;psyzm@suda.edu.cn
摘要:
采用线索化范式, 通过3个实验建立线索和靶子的意义关联, 考察了意义性线索在不同空间位置的注意定向效应。结果表明, 在下视野出现抑制效应, 且位置越下抑制效应越显著; 在上视野出现捕获效应, 且位置越上捕获效应越显著; 同时注意定向效应的程度受引导线索性质的影响。这些结果说明:(1)客体间的意义关联能够引导视觉空间注意, 表现出不同空间位置的注意定向效应; (2)客体性质能够影响意义关联的注意定向, 表现为客体的生动性越高, 调节能力越强, 抽象性越高, 调节能力越弱; (3)意义关联的注意定向具有规律性变化, 表现为基于空间位置的抑制和捕获效应。
中图分类号:
王慧媛, 陈艾睿, 张明. (2021). 意义关联的注意定向效应:基于空间位置的抑制和捕获. 心理学报, 53(2), 113-127.
WANG Hui-Yuan, CHEN Ai-Rui, ZHANG Ming. (2021). Meaningful contingent attentional orienting effects: Spatial location-based inhibition and capture. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 53(2), 113-127.
线索靶子语义一致性 | 线索位置 | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
上 | 右 | 下 | 左 | |
语义一致 | ||||
线索无效 | 523.76 ± 40.26 92.58 ± 6.94 | 519.42 ± 44.22 92.58 ± 7.63 | 510.76 ± 40.28 93.42 ± 5.20 | 516.21 ± 37.88 93.83 ± 5.97 |
线索有效 | 497.61 ± 35.19 96.50 ± 6.77 | 503.24 ± 43.28 95.50 ± 5.85 | 524.96 ± 50.30 96.25 ± 6.99 | 497.87 ± 44.26 90.75 ± 8.49 |
语义不一致 | ||||
线索无效 | 524.81 ± 35.75 93.33 ± 6.28 | 521.99 ± 35.59 93.25 ± 6.81 | 510.06 ± 32.23 92.75 ± 7.24 | 520.28 ± 33.78 94.67 ± 6.45 |
线索有效 | 496.43 ± 34.03 96.50 ± 7.67 | 503.84 ± 44.52 95.00 ± 5.71 | 518.35 ± 37.04 94.75 ± 5.90 | 504.62 ± 36.53 94.75 ± 5.61 |
表1 实验1中线索靶子语义一致性、线索有效性和线索位置各条件结合下的平均反应时和正确率(M ± SD)
线索靶子语义一致性 | 线索位置 | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
上 | 右 | 下 | 左 | |
语义一致 | ||||
线索无效 | 523.76 ± 40.26 92.58 ± 6.94 | 519.42 ± 44.22 92.58 ± 7.63 | 510.76 ± 40.28 93.42 ± 5.20 | 516.21 ± 37.88 93.83 ± 5.97 |
线索有效 | 497.61 ± 35.19 96.50 ± 6.77 | 503.24 ± 43.28 95.50 ± 5.85 | 524.96 ± 50.30 96.25 ± 6.99 | 497.87 ± 44.26 90.75 ± 8.49 |
语义不一致 | ||||
线索无效 | 524.81 ± 35.75 93.33 ± 6.28 | 521.99 ± 35.59 93.25 ± 6.81 | 510.06 ± 32.23 92.75 ± 7.24 | 520.28 ± 33.78 94.67 ± 6.45 |
线索有效 | 496.43 ± 34.03 96.50 ± 7.67 | 503.84 ± 44.52 95.00 ± 5.71 | 518.35 ± 37.04 94.75 ± 5.90 | 504.62 ± 36.53 94.75 ± 5.61 |
线索靶子语义 一致性 | 线索位置 | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
上方 | 右上 | 右下 | 下方 | 左下 | 左上 | |
语义一致 | ||||||
线索无效 | 536.88 ± 42.35 97.95 ± 3.33 | 537.75 ± 44.63 96.59 ± 4.97 | 536.77 ± 45.04 95.45 ± 6.71 | 531.48 ± 36.46 96.82 ± 4.51 | 536.44 ± 40.01 98.86 ± 2.14 | 533.55 ± 42.43 97.05 ± 3.67 |
线索有效 | 519.41 ± 45.78 97.73 ± 7.36 | 519.72 ± 43.55 95.45 ± 12.53 | 545.26 ± 60.33 95.45 ± 9.87 | 545.51 ± 62.54 96.59 ± 11.69 | 550.27 ± 63.40 89.77 ± 14.76 | 536.36 ± 53.61 93.18 ± 11.40 |
语义不一致 | ||||||
线索无效 | 540.97 ± 36.93 96.36 ± 4.92 | 536.90 ± 42.92 96.36 ± 4.68 | 540.14 ± 42.30 96.59 ± 5.21 | 534.26 ± 32.20 96.82 ± 3.95 | 536.42 ± 44.58 97.50 ± 3.36 | 530.14 ± 35.28 97.05 ± 3.67 |
线索有效 | 532.07 ± 53.20 98.96 ± 5.33 | 533.43 ± 46.86 98.86 ± 5.33 | 553.02 ± 53.33 98.86 ± 5.33 | 568.89 ± 55.31 96.59 ± 8.78 | 563.85 ± 62.08 93.18 ± 13.76 | 535.43 ± 66.59 96.59 ± 8.78 |
表2 实验2中线索靶子语义一致性、线索有效性和线索位置各条件结合下的平均反应时和正确率(M ± SD)
线索靶子语义 一致性 | 线索位置 | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
上方 | 右上 | 右下 | 下方 | 左下 | 左上 | |
语义一致 | ||||||
线索无效 | 536.88 ± 42.35 97.95 ± 3.33 | 537.75 ± 44.63 96.59 ± 4.97 | 536.77 ± 45.04 95.45 ± 6.71 | 531.48 ± 36.46 96.82 ± 4.51 | 536.44 ± 40.01 98.86 ± 2.14 | 533.55 ± 42.43 97.05 ± 3.67 |
线索有效 | 519.41 ± 45.78 97.73 ± 7.36 | 519.72 ± 43.55 95.45 ± 12.53 | 545.26 ± 60.33 95.45 ± 9.87 | 545.51 ± 62.54 96.59 ± 11.69 | 550.27 ± 63.40 89.77 ± 14.76 | 536.36 ± 53.61 93.18 ± 11.40 |
语义不一致 | ||||||
线索无效 | 540.97 ± 36.93 96.36 ± 4.92 | 536.90 ± 42.92 96.36 ± 4.68 | 540.14 ± 42.30 96.59 ± 5.21 | 534.26 ± 32.20 96.82 ± 3.95 | 536.42 ± 44.58 97.50 ± 3.36 | 530.14 ± 35.28 97.05 ± 3.67 |
线索有效 | 532.07 ± 53.20 98.96 ± 5.33 | 533.43 ± 46.86 98.86 ± 5.33 | 553.02 ± 53.33 98.86 ± 5.33 | 568.89 ± 55.31 96.59 ± 8.78 | 563.85 ± 62.08 93.18 ± 13.76 | 535.43 ± 66.59 96.59 ± 8.78 |
线索靶子语义 一致性 | 线索位置 | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
上方 | 右上 | 右下 | 下方 | 左下 | 左上 | |
语义一致 | ||||||
线索无效 | 936.77 ± 93.65 92.50 ± 4.89 | 935.36 ± 77.04 91.96 ± 5.89 | 915.47 ± 75.92 93.48 ± 4.69 | 890.01 ± 88.04 92.17 ± 4.79 | 916.90 ± 101.56 93.04 ± 5.22 | 956.92 ± 95.82 94.13 ± 5.36 |
线索有效 | 811.39 ± 187.07 94.02 ± 8.32 | 855.84 ± 194.22 95.65 ± 8.09 | 1017.24 ± 195.81 92.93 ± 9.85 | 1057.90 ± 198.98 96.20 ± 5.88 | 958.87 ± 145.62 89.67 ± 12.30 | 779.27 ± 150.16 94.02 ± 9.88 |
语义不一致 | ||||||
线索无效 | 970.18 ± 85.43 92.17 ± 3.72 | 950.88 ± 89.25 91.41 ± 6.21 | 923.04 ± 82.25 91.41 ± 4.70 | 928.99 ± 97.65 91.09 ± 5.37 | 933.36 ± 83.62 91.41 ± 5.43 | 949.07 ± 79.12 91.20 ± 7.34 |
线索有效 | 865.73 ± 165.24 94.02 ± 9.13 | 861.82 ± 184.36 93.48 ± 9.13 | 1014.82 ± 182.12 91.85 ± 11.06 | 1065.98 ± 158.78 85.87 ± 16.12 | 981.85 ± 165.56 89.67 ± 11.71 | 777.12 ± 173.19 94.02 ± 9.13 |
表3 实验3中线索靶子语义一致性、线索有效性和线索位置各条件结合下的平均反应时和正确率(M ± SD)
线索靶子语义 一致性 | 线索位置 | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
上方 | 右上 | 右下 | 下方 | 左下 | 左上 | |
语义一致 | ||||||
线索无效 | 936.77 ± 93.65 92.50 ± 4.89 | 935.36 ± 77.04 91.96 ± 5.89 | 915.47 ± 75.92 93.48 ± 4.69 | 890.01 ± 88.04 92.17 ± 4.79 | 916.90 ± 101.56 93.04 ± 5.22 | 956.92 ± 95.82 94.13 ± 5.36 |
线索有效 | 811.39 ± 187.07 94.02 ± 8.32 | 855.84 ± 194.22 95.65 ± 8.09 | 1017.24 ± 195.81 92.93 ± 9.85 | 1057.90 ± 198.98 96.20 ± 5.88 | 958.87 ± 145.62 89.67 ± 12.30 | 779.27 ± 150.16 94.02 ± 9.88 |
语义不一致 | ||||||
线索无效 | 970.18 ± 85.43 92.17 ± 3.72 | 950.88 ± 89.25 91.41 ± 6.21 | 923.04 ± 82.25 91.41 ± 4.70 | 928.99 ± 97.65 91.09 ± 5.37 | 933.36 ± 83.62 91.41 ± 5.43 | 949.07 ± 79.12 91.20 ± 7.34 |
线索有效 | 865.73 ± 165.24 94.02 ± 9.13 | 861.82 ± 184.36 93.48 ± 9.13 | 1014.82 ± 182.12 91.85 ± 11.06 | 1065.98 ± 158.78 85.87 ± 16.12 | 981.85 ± 165.56 89.67 ± 11.71 | 777.12 ± 173.19 94.02 ± 9.13 |
[1] |
Bao, Y., Lei, Q., Fang, Y., Tong, Y., Schill, K., Pöppel, E., & Strasburger, H. (2013). Inhibition of return in the visual field: The eccentricity effect is independent of cortical magnification. Experimental Psychology, 60(6), 425-431.
doi: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000215 URL |
[2] | Bergerbest, D., Shilkrot, O., Joseph, M., & Salti, M. (2017). Right visual-field advantage in the attentional blink: Asymmetry in attentional gating across time and space. Attention Perception & Psychophysics, 79(7), 1979-1992. |
[3] |
Binder, J. R., Conant, L. L., Humphries, C. J., Fernandino, L., Simons, S. B., Aguilar, M., & Desai, R. H. (2016). Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 33(3-4), 130-174.
doi: 10.1080/02643294.2016.1147426 URL pmid: 27310469 |
[4] |
Burnham, B. R. (2007). Displaywide visual features associated with a search display’s appearance can mediate attentional capture. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14(3), 392-422.
doi: 10.3758/bf03194082 URL pmid: 17874581 |
[5] |
Carlei, C., Framorando, D., Burra, N., & Kerzel, D. (2017). Face processing is enhanced in the left and upper visual hemi-fields. Visual Cognition, 25(7-8), 749-761.
doi: 10.1080/13506285.2017.1327466 URL |
[6] |
Chee, M. W. L., Weekes, B., Lee, K. M., Soon, C. S., Schreiber, A., Hoon, J. J., & Chee, M. (2000). Overlap and dissociation of semantic processing of Chinese characters, English words, and pictures: Evidence from fMRI. Neuroimage, 12(4), 392-403.
doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0631 URL pmid: 10988033 |
[7] | Colegatef, R. L., Hoffman, J. E., & Eriksen, C. W. (1973). Selective encoding from multielement visual displays. Perception & Psychophysics, 14(2), 217-224. |
[8] | Dampuré, J., Ros, C., Rouet, J.-F., & Vibert, N. (2014). Task-dependent sensitisation of perceptual and semantic processing during visual search for words. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 26(5), 530-549. |
[9] |
Eriksen, C. W., & Murphy, T. D. (1987). Movement of attentional focus across the visual field: A critical look at the evidence. Perception & Psychophysics, 42(3), 299-305.
doi: 10.3758/bf03203082 URL pmid: 3671056 |
[10] |
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175-191.
doi: 10.3758/BF03193146 URL |
[11] |
Folk, C. L., & Anderson, B. A. (2010). Target-uncertainty effects in attentional capture: Color-singleton set or multiple attentional control settings? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17(3), 421-426.
doi: 10.3758/PBR.17.3.421 URL pmid: 20551369 |
[12] |
Folk, C. L., & Remington, R. (1998). Selectivity in distraction by irrelevant featural singletons: Evidence for two forms of attentional capture. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 24(3), 847-858.
doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.24.3.847 URL pmid: 9627420 |
[13] |
Folk, C. L., Remington, R. W., & Johnston, J. C. (1992). Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 18(4), 1030-1044.
URL pmid: 1431742 |
[14] |
Folk, C. L., Remington, R. W., & Wright, J. H. (1994). The structure of attentional control: Contingent attentional capture by apparent motion, abrupt onset, and color. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20(2), 317-329.
doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.20.2.317 URL pmid: 8189195 |
[15] |
Gibson, B. S., & Kelsey, E. M. (1998). Stimulus-driven attentional capture is contingent on attentional set for displaywide visual features. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 24(3), 699-706.
doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.24.3.699 URL pmid: 9627409 |
[16] | Goodhew, S. C., Kendall, W., Ferber, S., & Pratt, J. (2014). Setting semantics: Conceptual set can determine the physical properties that capture attention. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 76(6), 1577-1589. |
[17] |
Greene, D. J., Mooshagian, E., Kaplan, J. T., Zaidel, E., & Iacoboni, M. (2009). The neural correlates of social attention: Automatic orienting to social and nonsocial cues. Psychological Research, 73(4), 499-511.
doi: 10.1007/s00426-009-0233-3 URL |
[18] |
Guo, Y. N., You, X. Q., & Li, Y. (2016). The role of stimulus type and semantic category-level attentional set in sustained inattentional blindness: Stimulus type and attentional set in inattentional blindness. Japanese Psychological Research, 58(4), 332-341.
doi: 10.1111/jpr.12127 URL |
[19] |
He, S., Cavanagh, P., & Intriligator, J. (1997). Attentional resolution. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 1(3), 115-121.
doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(97)89058-4 URL pmid: 21223875 |
[20] |
Henderson, J. M., & Hayes, T. R. (2017). Meaning-based guidance of attention in scenes as revealed by meaning maps. Nature Human Behaviour, 1(10), 743-747.
doi: 10.1038/s41562-017-0208-0 URL pmid: 31024101 |
[21] |
Hollingworth, A., Maxcey-Richard, A. M., & Vecera, S. P. (2012). The spatial distribution of attention within and across objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 38(1), 135-151.
doi: 10.1037/a0024463 URL pmid: 21728455 |
[22] |
Huang, W. Y., Su, Y. L., Zhen, Y. F., & Zhe, Q. (2016). The role of top-down spatial attention in contingent attentional capture. Psychophysiology, 53(5), 650-662.
doi: 10.1111/psyp.12615 URL pmid: 26879628 |
[23] | Jonides, J. (1981). Voluntary vs. automatic control over the mind’s eye’s movement. In J.B. Long and A.D. Baddeley (Eds.) Attention and performance IX: (pp.187-203). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. |
[24] |
Joseph, R. M., Fricker, Z., & Keehn, B. (2015). Activation of frontoparietal attention networks by non-predictive gaze and arrow cues. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 10(2), 294-301.
doi: 10.1093/scan/nsu054 URL pmid: 24748545 |
[25] |
Klein, R. M. (2000). Inhibition of return. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(4), 138-147.
doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01452-2 URL pmid: 10740278 |
[26] |
Liu, T., Heeger, D. J., & Carrasco, M. (2006). Neural correlates of the visual vertical meridian asymmetry. Journal of Vision, 6(11), 1294-1306.
doi: 10.1167/6.11.12 URL pmid: 17209736 |
[27] |
Lu, J. C., Tian, L. L., Zhang, J. F., Wang, J., Ye, C. X., & Liu, Q. (2017). Strategic inhibition of distractors with visual working memory contents after involuntary attention capture. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 16314.
doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-16305-5 URL pmid: 29176675 |
[28] |
Maxfield, L. (1997). Attention and semantic priming: A review of prime task effects. Consciousness & Cognition, 6(2-3), 204-218.
URL pmid: 9262409 |
[29] |
Meyer, D. E., & Schvaneveldt, R. W. (1971). Facilitation in recognizing pairs of words: Evidence of a dependence between retrieval operations. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 90(2), 227-234.
URL pmid: 5134329 |
[30] | Palanica, A., & Itier, R. J. (2015). Eye gaze and head orientation modulate the inhibition of return for faces. Attention Perception & Psychophysics, 77(8), 2589-2600. |
[31] |
Palanica, A., & Itier, R. J. (2017). Asymmetry in gaze direction discrimination between the upper and lower visual fields. Perception, 46(8), 941-955.
URL pmid: 28056652 |
[32] | Posner, M. I., & Snyder, C. R. R. (1975). Attention and cognitive control. In R. L. Solso (Ed.). Information processing and cognition: The Loyola symposium (pp. 55-85). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. |
[33] | Posner, M. I., & Cohen, Y. (1984). Components of visual orienting. In H. Bouma & D. G. Bouwhuis (Eds.), Attention and performance X: Control of language processes (pp. 531-556). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. |
[34] | Quek, G. L., & Finkbeiner, M. (2016). The upper-hemifield advantage for masked face processing: Not just an attentional bias. Attention Perception & Psychophysics, 78(1), 52-68. |
[35] |
Sato, W., Kochiyama, T., Uono, S., & Yoshikawa, S. (2009). Commonalities in the neural mechanisms underlying automatic attentional shifts by gaze, gestures, and symbols. Neuroimage, 45(3), 984-992.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.12.052 URL pmid: 19167506 |
[36] | Schoeberl, T., Fuchs, I., Theeuwes, J., & Ansorge, U. (2015). Stimulus-driven attentional capture by subliminal onset cues. Attention Perception & Psychophysics, 77(3), 737-748. |
[37] | Seidl-Rathkopf, K. N., Turk-Browne, N. B., & Kastner, S. (2015). Automatic guidance of attention during real-world visual search. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 77(6), 1881-1895. |
[38] | Shen, W., Qu, Q. Q., & Li, X. S. (2016). Semantic information mediates visual attention during spoken word recognition in Chinese: Evidence from the printed-word version of the visual-world paradigm. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 78(5), 1267-1284. |
[39] | Sun, S. Z., Shen, J., Shaw, M., Cant, J. S., & Ferber, S. (2015). Automatic capture of attention by conceptually generated working memory templates. Attention Perception & Psychophysics, 77(6), 1841-1847. |
[40] |
Thomas, N. A., Castine, B. R., Loetscher, T., & Nicholls, M. E. R. (2015). Upper visual field distractors preferentially bias attention to the left. Cortex, 64, 179-193.
doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2014.10.018 URL pmid: 25437374 |
[41] |
Wang, B. C., Cao, X. H., Theeuwes, J., Olivers, C. N. L., & Wang, Z. G. (2017). Separate capacities for storing different features in visual working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 43(2), 226-236.
doi: 10.1037/xlm0000295 URL pmid: 27399920 |
[42] | Wang, H. Y., Sui, J., & Zhang, M. (2016). The effect of cue-target relevance and search strategies on attentional capture: Evidence from meaning cues. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 48(7), 783-793. |
[ 王慧媛, 隋洁, 张明. (2016). 线索靶子关联和搜索策略对注意捕获的作用——来自意义线索的证据. 心理学报, 48(7), 783-793.] | |
[43] | Wang, H. Y., Sui, J., & Zhang, M. (2018). Attentional capture is contingent on attentional control setting for semantic meaning: Evidence from modified spatial cueing paradigm. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 50(10), 1071-1082. |
[ 王慧媛, 隋洁, 张明. (2018). 语义关联的注意捕获——来自线索化范式的证据. 心理学报, 50(10), 1071-1082.] | |
[44] | Wang, H. Y., Zhang, M., & Sui, J. (2014). The effect of cue-target relevance and search strategies on attentional capture. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 46(2), 185-195. |
[ 王慧媛, 张明, 隋洁. (2014). 线索靶子关联和搜索策略对注意捕获的作用. 心理学报, 46(2), 185-195.] | |
[45] |
Wurm, L. H., Legge, G. E., Isenberg, L. M., & Luebker, A. (1993). Color improves object recognition in normal and low vision. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception & Performance, 19(4), 899-911.
doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.19.4.899 URL pmid: 8409865 |
[46] |
Wyble, B., Folk, C., & Potter, M. C. (2013). Contingent attentional capture by conceptually relevant images. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 39(3), 861-871.
doi: 10.1037/a0030517 URL pmid: 23163786 |
[47] | Yang, M., Liu, D. W., Cai, A. N., & Zhou, R. L. (2012). Attentional bias of spatial location in viewing scenes: Evidence from eye tracking research. Journal of Psychological Science, 35(2), 258-263. |
[ 杨萌, 刘丹玮, 蔡安妮, 周仁来. (2012). 自由观看实景图片时注意的空间偏向. 心理科学, 35(2), 258-263.] | |
[48] |
Yantis, S., & Jonides, J. (1984). Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: Evidence from visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10(5), 601-621.
URL pmid: 6238122 |
[49] |
Zhao, S., Li, C. L., Uono, S., Yoshimura, S., & Toichi, M. (2017). Human cortical activity evoked by contextual processing in attentional orienting. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 2962.
URL pmid: 28592863 |
[1] | 祖光耀, 李舒淇, 张天阳, 王爱君, 张明. 返回抑制对视听跨通道对应的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(8): 1220-1233. |
[2] | 郭晓栋, 郑泓, 阮盾, 胡丁鼎, 王毅, 王艳郁, 陈楚侨. 认知和情感共情与负性情绪:情绪调节的作用机制[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(6): 892-904. |
[3] | 孙亚茹, 刘泽军, 段亚杰, 陈宁, 刘伟. 协作如何减少记忆错误:一项元分析研究[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(11): 1780-1792. |
[4] | 赵佳旭, 鲍敏. 基于伪装原理研究无意识视觉运动加工的方法[J]. 心理学报, 2022, 54(7): 725-735. |
[5] | 周雨汐, 刘宇颢, 张清芳. 图词间隔时间影响口语词汇产生中语义效应的机制:图词干扰范式研究[J]. 心理学报, 2022, 54(5): 453-465. |
[6] | 张明, 王婷婷, 吴晓刚, 张月娥, 王爱君. 面孔表情和声音情绪信息整合对返回抑制的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2022, 54(4): 331-342. |
[7] | 金花, 贾丽娜, 阴晓娟, 严世振, 魏士琳, 陈俊涛. 错误信息持续影响效应的神经基础[J]. 心理学报, 2022, 54(4): 343-354. |
[8] | 林静, 黄量杰成, 何耘丰, 段继鹏, 尹军. 基于成本最小化信息的社会性意图识别:来自脑电和行为的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2022, 54(1): 12-24. |
[9] | 张明, 桑汉斌, 鲁柯, 王爱君. 试次历史对跨通道非空间返回抑制的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(7): 681-693. |
[10] | 车晓玮, 徐慧云, 王凯旋, 张倩, 李寿欣. 工作记忆表征精度加工需求对注意引导的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(7): 694-713. |
[11] | 杨群, 张积家, 范丛慧. 维吾尔族与汉族的大学生在汉语歧义词消解中的语境促进效应及反应抑制效应[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(7): 746-757. |
[12] | 张帆, 王爱君, 张明. 基于干扰物特征的统计规则对注意抑制效应的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(6): 555-564. |
[13] | 黄月胜, 张豹, 范兴华, 黄杰. 无关工作记忆表征的负性情绪信息能否捕获视觉注意?一项眼动研究[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(1): 26-37. |
[14] | 宋晓蕾, 王丹, 张欣欣, 贾筱倩. 基于客体的一致性效应的产生机制[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(6): 669-681. |
[15] | 杨亦松, 林静, 何晓燕, 尹军. 动作理解因境而异: 动作加工中情境信息的自动整合[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(6): 682-693. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||