心理科学进展 ›› 2022, Vol. 30 ›› Issue (9): 2004-2019.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2022.02004
收稿日期:
2021-07-08
出版日期:
2022-09-15
发布日期:
2022-07-21
通讯作者:
李晶
E-mail:lij@psych.ac.cn
基金资助:
JIANG Danying, YANG Yunmei, LI Jing()
Received:
2021-07-08
Online:
2022-09-15
Published:
2022-07-21
Contact:
LI Jing
E-mail:lij@psych.ac.cn
摘要:
群体情境下儿童的资源分配决策受到群体身份、群体地位和群体规范的影响。在群体因素与公平分配之间, 儿童或坚持公平正义, 或偏向群体利益。这三种群体因素对儿童的分配公平性均造成了影响; 个体因素也会影响群体情境下儿童的分配决策。未来研究应对群体因素多加关注, 通过操控分配情境中的群体信息, 尝试探讨三种群体因素之间的相互作用, 以进一步了解复杂的群体情境下儿童的分配公平性。群体情境下影响分配公平性的个体因素也是需要未来研究逐一验证和讨论的问题。
中图分类号:
江丹莹, 杨运梅, 李晶. (2022). 群体情境下儿童的分配公平性. 心理科学进展 , 30(9), 2004-2019.
JIANG Danying, YANG Yunmei, LI Jing. (2022). Children's distributive justice in group context. Advances in Psychological Science, 30(9), 2004-2019.
模型 | 研究 | 被试年龄 | 情境 | 是否追问被试原因 |
---|---|---|---|---|
情感偏好模型 | Olson et al., | 5~7岁 | 幸运/不幸 | 否 |
Horwitz et al., | 4~5岁 | 贫穷/富有 | 否 | |
回报策略模型 | Ahl et al., | 4~10岁 | 贫穷/富有 | 否 |
Ahl & Dunham, | 4~5岁 7~8岁 | 贫穷/富有 | 否 | |
数值匹配模型 | Essler et al., | 3~5岁 | 贫穷/富有 必需品/奢侈品 | 是 |
Paulus & Rosal-Grifoll, | 3~6岁 | 贫穷/富有 | 否 |
表1 关于匹配倾向背后猜想的实证研究
模型 | 研究 | 被试年龄 | 情境 | 是否追问被试原因 |
---|---|---|---|---|
情感偏好模型 | Olson et al., | 5~7岁 | 幸运/不幸 | 否 |
Horwitz et al., | 4~5岁 | 贫穷/富有 | 否 | |
回报策略模型 | Ahl et al., | 4~10岁 | 贫穷/富有 | 否 |
Ahl & Dunham, | 4~5岁 7~8岁 | 贫穷/富有 | 否 | |
数值匹配模型 | Essler et al., | 3~5岁 | 贫穷/富有 必需品/奢侈品 | 是 |
Paulus & Rosal-Grifoll, | 3~6岁 | 贫穷/富有 | 否 |
[1] | 陈童, 伍珍. (2017). 儿童的分配公平性:心理理论的作用. 心理科学进展, 25(8), 1299-1309. |
[2] | 高凤阳, 仇小莉, 莫书亮. (2015). 儿童对分配公平性的理解: 发展特征、机制和影响因素. 心理研究, 8(5), 40-48. |
[3] | 侯玉波. (2013). 社会心理学. 北京: 北京大学出版社. |
[4] | 李晶, 朱莉琪. (2014). 高功能孤独症儿童的合作行为. 心理学报, 46(9), 1301-1316. |
[5] | 梁福成, 王心怡, 唐卫海. (2015). 不同分配情境下儿童公平行为的发展. 心理发展与教育, 31(6), 648-653. |
[6] | 林小霞, 刘秀丽. (2019). 3-8岁儿童在社会权力不平等情境下分配行为的发展特点: 心理理论的作用.见第二十二届全国心理学学术会议摘要集(pp.314-315). |
[7] | 刘国雄, 李红. (2013). 儿童对社会规则的认知发展研究述评. 华东师范大学学报(教育科学版), 31(3), 65-71. |
[8] | 刘璐, 肖雪, 刘丽莎, 徐良苑, 张旭然, 李燕芳. (2019). 儿童在不同卷入情境下基于资源价值的分配行为特点: 内群体偏爱的作用. 心理学报, 51(5), 584-597. |
[9] | 刘文, 张雪, 张玉, 俞睿玮. (2017). 4-8岁儿童公平认知与行为差距: 社会比较的作用. 心理学报, 49(12), 1504-1512. |
[10] | 刘文, 朱琳, 张雪, 张玉, 刘颖. (2015). 2-3岁儿童在分配情境下的公平敏感性. 心理学报, 47(11), 1341-1348. |
[11] | 聂瑞虹, 周楠, 张宇驰, 方晓义. (2017). 人际关系与高中生内外化问题的关系: 自尊的中介及性别的调节作用. 心理发展与教育, 33(6), 708-718. |
[12] | 王斯, 苏彦捷. (2011). 儿童面对不同对象的公平性选择: 心理理论的作用. 西南大学学报(社会科学版), 37(4), 26-32. |
[13] | 王斯, 苏彦捷. (2013). 从理解到使用: 心理理论与儿童不同情境中的分配公平性. 心理学报, 45(11), 1242-1512. |
[14] | 王笑楠, 郝洋, 苏彦捷. (2019). 竞争和合作对学龄前儿童分配模式的影响: 心理理论和抑制控制的作用. 心理发展与教育, 35(4), 385-392. |
[15] | 肖雪, 刘璐, 刘丽莎, 徐良苑, 张旭然, 李燕芳. (2021). 内群体偏爱与儿童基于贡献的分配公平性: 心理理论的调节作用. 心理发展与教育, 37(4), 498-507. |
[16] | 张亮, 张莉. (2014). 5-6岁幼儿公平分配行为及其认知研究. 教育研究与实验, (6), 92-96. |
[17] | 张雪, 刘文, 朱琳, 张玉. (2014). 基于贡献原则的幼儿分配公平性. 心理科学进展, 22(11), 1740-1746. |
[18] | 朱丽雯, 刘惠军. (2005). 儿童心理理论与社会性发展的相关研究. 见 第十届全国心理学学术大会论文摘要集. (p. 427). |
[19] | Abrams, D., Hogg, M. A., & Marques, J. M. (2005). A social psychological framework for understanding social inclusion and exclusion. New York: Psychology Press. |
[20] |
Abrams, D., Rutland, A., & Cameron, L. (2003). The development of subjective group dynamics: Children's judgments of normative and deviant in-group and out-group individuals. Child Development, 74(6), 1840-1856.
doi: 10.1046/j.1467-8624.2003.00641.x URL |
[21] |
Abrams, D., Rutland, A., Cameron, L., & Marques, J. (2003). The development of subjective group dynamics: When in- group bias gets specific. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 21(2), 155-176.
doi: 10.1348/026151003765264020 URL |
[22] |
Ahl, R. E., & Dunham, Y. (2019). “Wealth makes many friends”: Children expect more giving from resource-rich than resource-poor individuals. Child Development, 90(2), 524-543.
doi: 10.1111/cdev.12922 URL |
[23] |
Ahl, R. E., Duong, S., & Dunham, Y. (2019). Children employ wealth cues when predicting others' sharing. Developmental Psychology, 55(2), 303-314.
doi: 10.1037/dev0000643 URL |
[24] |
An, J., Yu, J., & Zhu, L. (2020). The origins of intergroup resource inequality influence children's decision to perpetuate or rectify inequality. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 571570.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.571570 URL |
[25] |
Bernhard, H., Fischbacher, U., & Fehr, E. (2006). Parochial altruism in humans. Nature, 442(7105), 912-915.
doi: 10.1038/nature04981 URL |
[26] | Bigler, R. S., & Liben, L. S. (2006). A developmental intergroup theory of social stereotypes and prejudice. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 34, 39-89. |
[27] |
Bindra, P. C., Glätzle-Rützler, D., & Lergetporer, P. (2020). Discrimination at young age: Experimental evidence from preschool children. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 175, 55-70.
doi: 10.1016/j.jebo.2019.12.026 URL |
[28] | Bloom, P. (2013). Just Babies: The Origins of Good and Evil. New York: Crown Publishers. |
[29] |
Breathett, K., Yee, E., Pool, N., Hebdon, M., Crist, J. D., Yee, R. H.,... Sweitzer, N. K. (2020). Association of gender and race with allocation of advanced heart failure therapies. JAMA Network Open, 3(7), e2011044.
doi: 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2020.11044 URL |
[30] |
Brown, C. S., & Bigler, R. S. (2004). Children's perceptions of gender discrimination. Developmental Psychology, 40(5), 714-726.
doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.40.5.714 URL |
[31] |
Brown, C. S., & Bigler, R. S. (2005). Children's perceptions of discrimination: A developmental model. Child Development, 76(3), 533-553.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00862.x URL |
[32] |
Buffington, C., Cerf, B., Jones, C., & Weinberg, B. A. (2016). STEM training and early career outcomes of female and male graduate students: Evidence from UMETRICS data linked to the 2010 census. American Economic Review, 106(5), 333-338.
pmid: 27231399 |
[33] |
Chai, Q., & He, J. (2017). Chinese preschoolers' resource allocation in the face of existing inequality under collaborative and noncollaborative contexts. Developmental Psychology, 53(8), 1494-1500.
doi: 10.1037/dev0000359 URL |
[34] |
Chalik, L., Rivera, C., & Rhodes, M. (2014). Children's use of categories and mental states to predict social behavior. Developmental Psychology, 50(10), 2360-2367.
doi: 10.1037/a0037729 pmid: 25151249 |
[35] |
Charafeddine, R., Mercier, H., Clément, F., Kaufmann, L., Reboul, A., & Van der Henst, J.-B. (2016). Children's allocation of resources in social dominance situations. Developmental Psychology, 52(11), 1843-1857.
pmid: 27668661 |
[36] |
Chernyak, N., Harris, P. L., & Cordes, S. (2019). Explaining early moral hypocrisy: Numerical cognition promotes equal sharing behavior in preschool-aged children. Developmental Science, 22(1), e12695.
doi: 10.1111/desc.12695 URL |
[37] |
Chevallier, C., Xu, J., Adachi, K., van der Henst, J., & Baumard, N. (2015). Preschoolers' understanding of merit in two Asian societies. Plos One, 10(5), e0114717.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0114717 URL |
[38] |
Conry-Murray, C. (2017). Children's distributive justice: the role of gender norms in different settings. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 14(1), 62-74.
doi: 10.1080/17405629.2016.1152176 URL |
[39] |
Cooley, S., & Killen, M. (2015). Children's evaluations of resource allocation in the context of group norms. Developmental Psychology, 51(4), 554-563.
doi: 10.1037/a0038796 URL |
[40] | Corsaro, W. A. (2015). The sociology of childhood. London: SAGE Publications, Inc. |
[41] |
Cowell, J. M., Samek, A., List, J., Decety, J., & Yu, R. (2015). The curious relation between theory of mind and sharing in preschool age children. Plos One, 10(2), e0117947.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0117947 URL |
[42] |
Damon, W. (1975). Early conceptions of positive justice as related to the development of logical operations. Child Development, 46(2), 301-312.
doi: 10.2307/1128122 URL |
[43] |
Damon, W. (1980). Patterns of change in children's social reasoning: A two-year longitudinal study. Child Development, 51(4), 1010-1017.
doi: 10.2307/1129538 URL |
[44] | Deutsch, M. (1975). Equity, equality, and need: What determines which value will be used as the basis of distributive justice? Journal of Social Issues, 31(3), 137-149. |
[45] |
Ding, X. P., Wellman, H. M., Wang, Y., Fu, G., & Lee, K. (2015). Theory-of-mind training causes honest young children to lie. Psychological Science, 26(11), 1812-1821.
doi: 10.1177/0956797615604628 URL |
[46] |
Dunham, Y., Baron, A. S., & Carey, S. (2011). Consequences of “minimal” group affiliations in children. Child Development, 82(3), 793-811.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01577.x URL |
[47] |
Elenbaas, L. (2019a). Interwealth contact and young children's concern for equity. Child Development, 90(1), 108-116.
doi: 10.1111/cdev.13157 URL |
[48] |
Elenbaas, L. (2019b). Perceptions of economic inequality are related to children's judgments about access to opportunities. Developmental Psychology, 55(3), 471-481.
doi: 10.1037/dev0000550 URL |
[49] |
Elenbaas, L., & Killen, M. (2016). Children rectify inequalities for disadvantaged groups. Developmental Psychology, 52(8), 1318-1329.
doi: 10.1037/dev0000154 pmid: 27455190 |
[50] |
Elenbaas, L., Rizzo, M. T., & Killen, M. (2020). A developmental- science perspective on social inequality. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 29(6), 610-616.
doi: 10.1177/0963721420964147 pmid: 33758480 |
[51] |
Ellemers, N. (2018). Gender stereotypes. Annual Review of Psychology, 69, 275-298.
doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-122216-011719 pmid: 28961059 |
[52] |
Engelmann, J. M., Herrmann, E., & Tomasello, M. (2018). Concern for group reputation increases prosociality in young children. Psychological Science, 29(2), 181-190.
doi: 10.1177/0956797617733830 pmid: 29095665 |
[53] |
Essler, S., Lepach, A. C., Petermann, F., & Paulus, M. (2020). Equality, equity, or inequality duplication? How preschoolers distribute necessary and luxury resources between rich and poor others. Social Development, 29(1), 110-125.
doi: 10.1111/sode.12390 URL |
[54] |
Essler, S., & Paulus, M. (2021). Robin hood or matthew? Children's reasoning about redistributive justice in the context of economic inequalities. Child Development, 92(4), 1254-1273.
doi: 10.1111/cdev.13482 URL |
[55] |
Fehr, E., Bernhard, H., & Rockenbach, B. (2008). Egalitarianism in young children. Nature, 454(7208), 1079-1083.
doi: 10.1038/nature07155 URL |
[56] |
Flavell, J. H. (1999). Cognitive development: Children's knowledge about the mind. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 21-45.
pmid: 10074674 |
[57] |
Gaither, S. E., Perlin, J. D., & Doan, S. N. (2020). Race, gender, and the development of cross-race egalitarianism. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 1525.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01525 pmid: 32754090 |
[58] |
Gummerum, M., Hanoch, Y., & Keller, M. (2008). When child development meets economic game theory: An interdisciplinary approach to investigating social development. Human Development, 51(4), 235-261.
doi: 10.1159/000151494 URL |
[59] | Hackman, J. R. (1976). Group influences on individuals. In M. Dunnette (Ed.), Handbook of industrial and organizational psychology (pp. 1455-1525). Chicago: Rand McNally. |
[60] |
Hamann, K., Bender, J., & Tomasello, M. (2014). Meritocratic sharing is based on collaboration in 3-year-olds. Developmental Psychology, 50(1), 121-128.
doi: 10.1037/a0032965 URL |
[61] |
Haslanger, S. (2016). What is a (social) structural explanation? Philosophical Studies, 173, 113-130.
doi: 10.1007/s11098-014-0434-5 URL |
[62] |
Hitti, A., Mulvey, K. L., Rutland, A., Abrams, D., & Killen, M. (2014). When is it okay to exclude a member of the ingroup? Children's and adolescents' social reasoning. Social Development, 23(3), 451-469.
doi: 10.1111/sode.12047 URL |
[63] |
Holopainen, A., Veld, D., Hoddenbach, E., & Begeer, S. (2019). Does theory of mind training enhance empathy in autism? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 49(10), 3965-3972.
doi: 10.1007/s10803-018-3671-1 pmid: 30074122 |
[64] |
Horwitz, S., Shutts, K., & Olson, K. (2014). Social class differences produce social group preferences. Developmental Science, 17(6), 991-1002.
doi: 10.1111/desc.12181 URL |
[65] |
Hsu, M., Anen, C., & Quartz, S. R. (2008). The right and the good: Distributive justice and neural encoding of equity and efficiency. Science, 320(5879), 1092-1095.
doi: 10.1126/science.1153651 URL |
[66] |
Jones, C., Simonoff, E., Baird, G., Pickles, A., Marsden, A., Tregay, J., Happé, F., & Charman, T. (2018). The association between theory of mind, executive function, and the symptoms of autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research: Official Journal of the International Society for Autism Research, 11(1), 95-109.
doi: 10.1002/aur.1873 URL |
[67] | Jordan, J. J., Mcauliffe, K., & Warneken, F. (2014). Development of in-group favoritism in children's third-party punishment of selfishness. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 111(35), 12710-12715. |
[68] |
Jost, J. T., & Banaji, M. R. (1994). The role of stereotyping in system-justification and the production of false consciousness. British Journal of Social Psychology, 33(1), 1-27.
doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8309.1994.tb01008.x URL |
[69] |
Kanngiesser, P., & Warneken, F. (2012). Young children consider merit when sharing resources with others. PloS One, 7(8), e43979.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0043979 URL |
[70] |
Kenward, B., Hellmer, K., Winter, L. S., & Eriksson, M. (2015). Four-year-olds' strategic allocation of resources: Attempts to elicit reciprocation correlate negatively with spontaneous helping. Cognition, 136, 1-8.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.11.035 pmid: 25490123 |
[71] |
Killen, M., Elenbaas, L., & Rizzo, M. (2018). Young children's ability to recognize and challenge unfair treatment of others in group contexts. Human Development, 61(4-5), 281-296.
doi: 10.1159/000492804 URL |
[72] |
Knowles, E. D., Lowery, B. S., Chow, R. M., & Unzueta, M. M. (2014). Deny, distance, or dismantle? How white Americans manage a privileged identity. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 9(6), 594-609.
doi: 10.1177/1745691614554658 pmid: 26186110 |
[73] |
Kofler, M. J., Sarver, D. E., Austin, K. E., Schaefer, H. S., Holland, E., Aduen, P. A.,... Lonigan, C. J. (2018). Can working memory training work for ADHD? Development of central executive training and comparison with behavioral parent training. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 86(12), 964-979.
doi: 10.1037/ccp0000308 URL |
[74] | Lam, C. B., Solmeyer, A. R., & Mchale, S. M. (2012). Sibling relationships and empathy across the transition to adolescence. Journal of Youth & Adolescence, 41(12), 1657-1670. |
[75] |
Lee, K., Esposito, G., & Setoh, P. (2018). Preschoolers favor their ingroup when resources are limited. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1752.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01752 URL |
[76] |
Leslie, S. J., Cimpian, A., Meyer, M., & Freeland, E. (2015). Expectations of brilliance underlie gender distributions across academic disciplines. Science, 347(6219), 262-265.
doi: 10.1126/science.1261375 URL |
[77] | Li, J., Wang, W., Yu, J., & Zhu, L. (2016). Young children's development of fairness preference. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1274. |
[78] |
Li, V., Spitzer, B., & Olson, K. R. (2014). Preschoolers reduce inequality while favoring individuals with more. Child Development, 85(3), 1123-1133.
doi: 10.1111/cdev.12198 URL |
[79] |
Li, W., Curtis, R., Moore, C., Wang, Y., & Zeng, X. (2019). To be fair, generous, or selfish: The effect of relationship on Chinese children's distributive allocation and procedural application. Social Development, 28(2), 449-464.
doi: 10.1111/sode.12341 URL |
[80] |
Lisi, A. V. M.-D., de Lisi, R., & van Gulik, K. (2008). The effects of grade level, context, and family type on male and female adolescents' distributive justice reasoning. Journal of Adolescence, 31(1), 107-124.
doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2007.05.003 URL |
[81] |
Lobue, V., Nishida, T., Chiong, C., Deloache, J. S., & Haidt, J. (2011). When getting something good is bad: Even three-year-olds react to inequality. Social Development, 20(1), 154-170.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2009.00560.x URL |
[82] |
Macnell, L., Driscoll, A., & Hunt, A. (2014). What's in a name: Exposing gender bias in student ratings of teaching. Innovative Higher Education, 40(4), 291-303.
doi: 10.1007/s10755-014-9313-4 URL |
[83] |
Mahajan, N., & Wynn, K. (2012). Origins of “Us” versus “Them”: Prelinguistic infants prefer similar others. Cognition, 124(2), 227-233.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.05.003 pmid: 22668879 |
[84] |
Mandalaywala, T. M., Benitez, J., Sagar, K., & Rhodes, M. (2021). Why do children show racial biases in their resource allocation decisions? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 211(1), 105224.
doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2021.105224 URL |
[85] |
McAuliffe, K., & Dunham, Y. (2017). Fairness overrides group bias in children's second-party punishment. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 146(4), 485-494.
doi: 10.1037/xge0000244 URL |
[86] | Mcdonald, M. M., Navarrete, C. D., & Vugt, M. V. (2012). Evolution and the psychology of intergroup conflict: The male warrior hypothesis. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 367(1589), 670-679. |
[87] | McGuire, L. (2018). The development of fair resource allocation: Social norms and group processes (Unpublished doctorial dissertation). University of London. |
[88] |
McGuire, L., Manstead, A. S. R., & Rutland, A. (2017). Group norms, intergroup resource allocation, and social reasoning among children and adolescents. Developmental Psychology, 53(12), 2333-2339.
doi: 10.1037/dev0000392 pmid: 28933878 |
[89] |
McGuire, L., Rizzo, M. T., Killen, M., & Rutland, A. (2018). The development of intergroup resource allocation: The role of cooperative and competitive in-group norms. Developmental Psychology, 54(8), 1499-1506.
doi: 10.1037/dev0000535 pmid: 29952601 |
[90] |
McGuire, L., & Rutland, A. (2020). Children and adolescents coordinate group and moral concerns within different goal contexts when allocating resources. The British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 38(4), 529-542.
doi: 10.1111/bjdp.12342 URL |
[91] |
Mckown, C., & Weinstein, R. S. (2003). The development and consequences of stereotype consciousness in middle childhood. Child Development, 74(2), 498-515.
doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.7402012 URL |
[92] |
Misch, A., Over, H., & Carpenter, M. (2018). The whistleblower's dilemma in young children: when loyalty trumps other moral concerns. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 250.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00250 URL |
[93] |
Moore, C. (2009). Fairness in children's resource allocation depends on the recipient. Psychological Science, 20(8), 944-948.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02378.x URL |
[94] |
Mulvey, K. L. (2016). Children's reasoning about social exclusion: Balancing many factors. Child Development Perspectives, 10(1), 22-27.
doi: 10.1111/cdep.12157 URL |
[95] |
Mulvey, K. L., Buchheister, K., & McGrath, K. (2016). Evaluations of intergroup resource allocations: The role of theory of mind. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 142, 203-211.
doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.10.002 pmid: 26525855 |
[96] |
Mulvey, K. L., Hitti, A., Rutland, A., Abrams, D., & Killen, M. (2014). When do children dislike ingroup members? Resource allocation from individual and group perspectives. Journal of Social Issues, 70(1), 29-46.
doi: 10.1111/josi.12045 URL |
[97] |
Mulvey, K. L., Rizzo, M. T., & Killen, M. (2016). Challenging gender stereotypes: Theory of mind and peer group dynamics. Developmental Science, 19(6), 999-1010.
doi: 10.1111/desc.12345 pmid: 26395753 |
[98] |
Nesdale, D., & Lawson, M. J. (2011). Social groups and children's intergroup attitudes: Can school norms moderate the effects of social group norms? Child Development, 82(5), 1594-1606.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01637.x pmid: 21883158 |
[99] |
Olson, K., Banaji, M., Dweck, C., & Spelke, E. (2006). Children's biased evaluations of lucky versus unlucky people and their social groups. Psychological Science, 17(10), 845-846.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01792.x URL |
[100] |
Olson, K. R., Dweck, C. S., Spelke, E. S., & Banaji, M. R. (2011). Children's responses to group-based inequalities: Perpetuation and rectification. Social Cognition, 29(3), 270-287.
doi: 10.1521/soco.2011.29.3.270 URL |
[101] |
Olson, K. R., & Spelke, E. S. (2008). Foundations of cooperation in young children. Cognition, 108(1), 222-231.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.12.003 URL |
[102] |
Paluck, E. L., & Green, D. P. (2009). Prejudice reduction: What works? A review and assessment of research and practice. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 339-367.
doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163607 URL |
[103] |
Pappert, A.-T., Williams, A., & Moore, C. (2017). The influence of competition on resource allocation in preschool children. Social Development, 26(2), 367-381.
doi: 10.1111/sode.12202 URL |
[104] |
Paulus, M. (2014). The emergence of prosocial behavior: Why do infants and toddlers help, comfort, and share? Child Development Perspectives, 8(2), 77-81.
doi: 10.1111/cdep.12066 URL |
[105] |
Paulus, M. (2020). Is young children's helping affected by helpees' need? Preschoolers, but not infants selectively help needy others. Psychological Research, 84(5), 1440-1450.
doi: 10.1007/s00426-019-01148-8 URL |
[106] |
Paulus, M., & Leitherer, M. (2017). Preschoolers' social experiences and empathy-based responding relate to their fair resource allocation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 161, 202-210.
doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2017.03.005 URL |
[107] |
Paulus, M., & Rosal-Grifoll, B. (2017). Helping and sharing in preschool children with autism. Experimental Brain Research, 235(7), 2081-2088.
doi: 10.1007/s00221-017-4947-y URL |
[108] |
Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1(4), 515-526.
doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00076512 URL |
[109] |
Proudfoot, D., Kay, A. C., & Koval, C. Z. (2015). A gender bias in the attribution of creativity: Archival and experimental evidence for the perceived association between masculinity and creative thinking. Psychological Science, 26(11), 1751-1761.
doi: 10.1177/0956797615598739 pmid: 26386015 |
[110] |
Qiao, C., & Jie, H. (2017). Chinese preschoolers' resource allocation in the face of existing inequality under collaborative and noncollaborative contexts. Developmental Psychology, 53(8), 1494-1500.
doi: 10.1037/dev0000359 pmid: 28530436 |
[111] |
Raabe, T., & Beelmann, A. (2011). Development of ethnic, racial, and national prejudice in childhood and adolescence: A multinational meta-analysis of age differences. Child Development, 82(6), 1715-1737.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01668.x URL |
[112] |
Rakoczy, H., Kaufmann, M., & Lohse, K. (2016). Young children understand the normative force of standards of equal resource distribution. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 150, 396-403.
doi: S0022-0965(16)30046-7 pmid: 27329180 |
[113] |
Rapp, D. J., Engelmann, J. M., Herrmann, E., & Tomasello, M. (2019). Young children's reputational strategies in a peer group context. Developmental Psychology, 55(2), 329-336.
doi: 10.1037/dev0000639 URL |
[114] |
Renno, M. P., & Shutts, K. (2015). Children's social category- based giving and its correlates: Expectations and preferences. Developmental Psychology, 51(4), 533-543.
doi: 10.1037/a0038819 URL |
[115] |
Rhodes, M., & Baron, A. (2019). The development of social categorization. Annual Review of Developmental Psychology, 1, 359-386.
doi: 10.1146/annurev-devpsych-121318-084824 URL |
[116] |
Ridgeway, C. (2013). Framed by gender: How gender inequality persists in the modern world. European Journal of Population, 29(2), 131-133.
doi: 10.1007/s10680-013-9284-7 URL |
[117] |
Rizzo, M. T., Cooley, S., Elenbaas, L., & Killen, M. (2017). Young children's inclusion decisions in moral and social-conventional group norm contexts. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 165, 19-36.
doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2017.05.006 URL |
[118] |
Rizzo, M. T., Elenbaas, L., Cooley, S., & Killen, M. (2016). Children's recognition of fairness and others' welfare in a resource allocation task: Age related changes. Developmental Psychology, 52(8), 1307-1317.
doi: 10.1037/dev0000134 URL |
[119] |
Rizzo, M. T., Elenbaas, L., & Vanderbilt, K. E. (2020). Do children distinguish between resource inequalities with individual versus structural origins? Child Development, 91(2), 439-455.
doi: 10.1111/cdev.13181 URL |
[120] |
Rizzo, M. T., & Killen, M. (2016). Children's understanding of equity in the context of inequality. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 34(4), 569-581.
doi: 10.1111/bjdp.12150 URL |
[121] |
Rizzo, M. T., & Killen, M. (2018). Theory of Mind is related to children's resource allocations in gender stereotypic contexts. Developmental Psychology, 54(3), 510-520.
doi: 10.1037/dev0000439 URL |
[122] |
Rizzo, M. T., & Killen, M. (2020). Children's evaluations of individually and structurally based inequalities: The role of status. Developmental Psychology, 56(12), 2223-2235.
doi: 10.1037/dev0001118 URL |
[123] |
Roberts, S. O., Gelman, S. A., & Ho, A. K. (2017). So it is, so it shall be: Group regularities license children's prescriptive judgments. Cognitive Science, 41(S3), 576-600.
doi: 10.1111/cogs.12443 URL |
[124] |
Roberts, S. O., Guo, C., Ho, A. K., & Gelman, S. A. (2018). Children's descriptive-to-prescriptive tendency replicates (and varies) cross-culturally: Evidence from China. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 165, 148-160.
doi: S0022-0965(17)30207-2 pmid: 28552389 |
[125] |
Rochat, P., Dias, M. D. G., Liping, G., Broesch, T., Passos- Ferreira, C., Winning, A., & Berg, B. (2009). Fairness in distributive justice by 3- and 5-year-olds across seven cultures. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 40(3), 416-442.
doi: 10.1177/0022022109332844 URL |
[126] |
Rose, A. J., & Rudolph, K. D. (2006). A review of sex differences in peer relationship processes: Potential trade-offs for the emotional and behavioral development of girls and boys. Psychological Bulletin, 132(1), 98-131.
doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.1.98 URL |
[127] |
Rutland, A., & Killen, M. (2017). Fair resource allocation among children and adolescents: The role of group and developmental processes. Child Development Perspectives, 11(1), 56-62.
doi: 10.1111/cdep.12211 URL |
[128] |
Rutland, A., Killen, M., & Abrams, D. (2010). A new social-cognitive developmental perspective on prejudice: The interplay between morality and group identity. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5(3), 279-291.
doi: 10.1177/1745691610369468 pmid: 26162160 |
[129] |
Schmidt, M. F. H., & Sommerville, J. A. (2011). Fairness expectations and altruistic sharing in 15-month-old human infants. Plos One, 6(10), e23223.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0023223 URL |
[130] |
Schmidt, M. F. H., Svetlova, M., Johe, J., & Tomasello, M. (2016). Children's developing understanding of legitimate reasons for allocating resources unequally. Cognitive Development, 37, 42-52.
doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2015.11.001 URL |
[131] | Shaw, A., & Olson, K. (2013). All inequality is not equal: Children correct inequalities using resource value. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 393. |
[132] |
Sigelman, C. K. (2013). Age differences in perceptions of rich and poor people: Is it skill or luck? Social Development, 22(1), 1-18.
doi: 10.1111/sode.12000 URL |
[133] |
Sloane, S., Baillargeon, R., & Premack, D. (2012). Do infants have a sense of fairness? Psychological Science, 23(2), 196-204.
doi: 10.1177/0956797611422072 URL |
[134] |
Starmans, C., Sheskin, M., & Bloom, P. (2017). Why people prefer unequal societies. Nature Human Behaviour, 1(4), 0082.
doi: 10.1038/s41562-017-0082 URL |
[135] |
Vonk, J., Jett, S. E., Tomeny, T. S., Mercer, S. H., & Cwikla, J. (2020). Young children's theory of mind predicts more sharing with friends over time. Child Development, 91(1), 63-77.
doi: 10.1111/cdev.13112 URL |
[136] |
Weckstein, S. M., Weckstein, E. J., Parker, C. D., & Westerman, M. W. (2017). A retrospective chart analysis with follow-up of Cogmed working memory training in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Medical Science Monitor Basic Research, 23, 336-343.
pmid: 29033450 |
[137] |
Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development, 72(3), 655-684.
pmid: 11405571 |
[138] |
Wörle, M., & Paulus, M. (2018). Normative expectations about fairness: The development of a charity norm in preschoolers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 165, 66-84.
doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2017.03.016 URL |
[139] |
Wu, Z., & Gao, X. (2018). Preschoolers' group bias in punishing selfishness in the Ultimatum Game. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 166, 280-292.
doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2017.08.015 URL |
[140] |
Xiao, X., Liu, L., Xu, L., Liu, L., Chen, C. S., & Li, Y. (2019). Group bias in children's merit-based resource allocation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 188, 104660.
doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2019.06.013 URL |
[141] |
Xie, W., Ho, B., Meier, S., & Zhou, X. (2017). Rank reversal aversion inhibits redistribution across societies. Nature Human Behaviour, 1, 0142.
doi: 10.1038/s41562-017-0142 URL |
[142] |
Yu, J., Zhu, L., & Leslie, A. M. (2016). Children's sharing behavior in mini-dictator games: The role of in-group favoritism and Theory of Mind. Child Development, 87(6), 1747-1757.
doi: 10.1111/cdev.12635 URL |
[143] |
Zhang, Z. (2020). The impact of social relationships on children's distributive justice. Developmental Psychology, 56(1), 103-116.
doi: 10.1037/dev0000855 pmid: 31763865 |
[1] | 张李斌, 张其文, 王晨旭, 张云运. 社会网络视角下儿童青少年同伴关系网络与欺凌相关行为的共同变化关系[J]. 心理科学进展, 2023, 31(3): 416-427. |
[2] | 蒋涵. 注意缺陷多动障碍儿童教育康复线上模式的构建[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(8): 1747-1758. |
[3] | 康丹, 李佳佳, 蔡术. 学前儿童睡眠问题与语言障碍的关系[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(6): 1270-1281. |
[4] | 曹馨允, 童钰, 王福兴, 李卉. 来自屏幕的纽带:视频聊天与儿童发展[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(6): 1282-1293. |
[5] | 曾昭携, 白洁, 郭永玉, 张跃, 顾玉婷. 越富有越不支持再分配?——社会阶层与再分配偏向的关系及其心理机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(6): 1336-1349. |
[6] | 蒋路远, 曹李梅, 秦昕, 谭玲, 陈晨, 彭小斐. 人工智能决策的公平感知[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(5): 1078-1092. |
[7] | 刘笑晗, 陈明隆, 郭静. 机器学习在儿童创伤后应激障碍识别及转归预测中的应用[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(4): 851-862. |
[8] | 陈静, 冉光明, 张琪, 牛湘. 儿童和青少年同伴侵害与攻击行为关系的三水平元分析[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(2): 275-290. |
[9] | 杨建锋, 郭晓虹, 明晓东. 工作场所中的助人决策过程[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(1): 15-31. |
[10] | 王辉, 李广政. 儿童手势及其与学习的关系[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(9): 1617-1627. |
[11] | 尚俊辰, 刘智慧, 王笑雨, 迟智超, 李卫君. 两人和三人最后通牒博弈任务中嗓音吸引力对决策的影响[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(8): 1402-1409. |
[12] | 王琳, 王志丹, 王泓婧. 孤独症儿童动作发展障碍的神经机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(7): 1239-1250. |
[13] | 王学思, 李静雅, 王美芳. 父母婚姻冲突对儿童发展的影响及其机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(5): 875-884. |
[14] | 王祯. 儿童的性别刻板印象威胁及其干预[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(2): 276-285. |
[15] | 张振, 李海文, 熊建萍, 赵辉, 刘瑞雪, 齐春辉. 公平规范执行中内群体偏爱的心理发展机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(12): 2091-2104. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||