心理学报 ›› 2024, Vol. 56 ›› Issue (1): 61-69.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2024.00061 cstr: 32110.14.2024.00061
收稿日期:
2023-04-04
发布日期:
2023-11-23
出版日期:
2024-01-25
通讯作者:
陈永香, E-mail: chenyongxiang_236@163.com
CHEN Yongxiang1,2(), PEI Feifei2, HUANG Jiali2
Received:
2023-04-04
Online:
2023-11-23
Published:
2024-01-25
摘要:
口语词汇习得过程中存在语法促进和语义促进效应, 而语法又包含了句法和词法。本研究采用优先指向范式考察了语法和语义线索对汉语儿童动词习得的影响。实验1发现, 5岁儿童可以利用单个句法线索词习得动词。实验2发现, 4岁儿童可以在增加句法线索词或语义线索词条件下习得动词; 实验2a发现, 4岁儿童更倾向于将单音节词判定为动词, 实验2b发现, 在增加语义线索词条件下, 3岁儿童更倾向于将双音节词判定为名词。结果表明, 5岁儿童可以利用单个句法线索词习得动词, 4岁儿童可以通过多重线索习得动词。本研究拓展了以往关于汉语儿童动词习得的结果, 提示语言习得研究应考虑语法线索的独特性和可靠性。
中图分类号:
陈永香, 裴斐斐, 黄佳丽. (2024). 语法和语义线索对儿童动词习得的影响. 心理学报, 56(1), 61-69.
CHEN Yongxiang, PEI Feifei, HUANG Jiali. (2024). Effects of grammatical and semantic clues on verb acquisition in Chinese-speaking children. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 56(1), 61-69.
学习阶段 | 测试阶段 | |
---|---|---|
视频→ | | |
音频→ | “看, 她在X。看, 她在X啊!” | “谁在X呢?哪个视频里的人在X呢?” |
表1 实验1单试次流程
学习阶段 | 测试阶段 | |
---|---|---|
视频→ | | |
音频→ | “看, 她在X。看, 她在X啊!” | “谁在X呢?哪个视频里的人在X呢?” |
[1] | Bates E., & Goodman J. (2001). On the inseparability of grammar and the lexicon:Evidence from acquisition. In M. Tomasello & E. Bates (Eds.), Language development: The essential readings(pp.134-162). Oxford: Blackwell Publishing. |
[2] |
Brown R. W. (1957). Linguistic determinism and the part of speech. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 55(1), 1. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0041199
doi: 10.1037/h0041199 URL |
[3] |
Caglar-Ryeng Ø., Eklund K., & Nergård-Nilssen T. (2019). Lexical and grammatical development in children at family risk of dyslexia from early childhood to school entry: A cross-lagged analysis. Journal of Child Language, 46(6), 1102-1126. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000919000333
doi: 10.1017/S0305000919000333 URL pmid: 31317848 |
[4] |
Cao A., & Lewis M. (2022). Quantifying the syntactic bootstrapping effect in verb learning: A meta‐analytic synthesis. Developmental Science, 25(2), e13176. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.13176
doi: 10.1111/desc.v25.2 URL |
[5] |
Chen Y. X., & Zhu L. Q. (2014). Associations of body parts and early-learned Mandarin verbs and their effect on AoA of these verbs. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 46(7), 912-921. https://doi.org/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2014.00912.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2014.00912 URL |
[ 陈永香, 朱莉琪. (2014). 身体部位与早期习得的汉语动词的联结及其对动词习得年龄的影响. 心理学报, 46(7), 912-921. https://doi.org/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2014.00912 ] | |
[6] | Chen Y. X., & Zhu L. Q. (2023). Two-year-olds use body part information to speed up verb comprehension. Journal of Psychological Science, 46(2), 355-362. http://www.psysci.org/CN/Y2023/V46/I2/355. |
[ 陈永香, 朱莉琪. (2023). 两岁儿童利用身体部位信息快速识别动词. 心理科学, 46(2), 355-362. http://www.psysci.org/CN/Y2023/V46/I2/355] | |
[7] |
de Carvalho A., He A. X., Lidz J., & Christophe A. (2019). Prosody and function words cue the acquisition of word meanings in 18-month-old infants. Psychological Science, 30(3), 319-332. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797618814131
doi: 10.1177/0956797618814131 URL pmid: 30668928 |
[8] |
Fisher C. (1996). Structural limits on verb mapping: The role of analogy in children's interpretations of sentences. Cognitive Psychology, 31(1), 41-81. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1996.0012
URL pmid: 8812021 |
[9] | Fisher C., & Gleitman L. R. (2002). Language acquisition. John Wiley & Sons, Inc. 10.1002/0471214426.pas0311 |
[10] |
Fisher C., Jin K. S., & Scott R. M. (2020). The developmental origins of syntactic bootstrapping. Topics in Cognitive Science, 12(1), 48-77. https://doi.org/10.1111/tops.12447
doi: 10.1111/tops.12447 URL pmid: 31419084 |
[11] |
Gleitman L. (1990). The structural sources of verb meanings. Language Acquisition, 1(1), 3-55. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327817la0101_2
doi: 10.1207/s15327817la0101_2 URL |
[12] | Guasti M. T. (2017). Language acquisition: The growth of grammar (2nd ed.). MIT Press. |
[13] |
Hao M., Liu Y., Shu H., Xing A., Jiang Y., & Li P. (2015). Developmental changes in the early child lexicon in Mandarin Chinese. Journal of Child Language, 42(3), 505-537. https://doi.org/10.1017/S030500091400018X
doi: 10.1017/S030500091400018X URL pmid: 24967509 |
[14] |
He A. X., & Lidz J. (2017). Verb learning in 14-and 18-month-old English-learning infants. Language Learning and Development, 13(3), 335-356. https://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2017.1285238
doi: 10.1080/15475441.2017.1285238 URL |
[15] |
Huang R.(L.), Fletcher P., Zhang Z., Liang W.,Marchman, V., & Tardif, T.(2022). Early grammatical marking development in Mandarin-speaking toddlers. Developmental Psychology, 58(4), 631-645. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001316
doi: 10.1037/dev0001316 URL pmid: 35343712 |
[16] |
Imai M., Haryu E., & Okada H. (2005). Mapping novel nouns and verbs onto dynamic action events: Are verb meanings easier to learn than noun meanings for Japanese children? Child Development, 76(2), 340-355. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00849.x
URL pmid: 15784086 |
[17] |
Imai M., Li L., Haryu E., Okada H., Hirsh-Pasek K., Golinkoff R., & Shigematsu J. (2008). Novel Noun and Verb Learning in Chinese-, English-, and Japanese-Speaking Children. Child Development, 79(4), 979-1000. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01171.x
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01171.x URL pmid: 18717902 |
[18] |
Klein W., Li P., & Hendriks H. (2000). Aspect and assertion in Mandarin Chinese. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 18(4), 723-770. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1006411825993
doi: 10.1023/A:1006411825993 URL |
[19] |
Lee J. N., & Naigles L. R. (2005). The input to verb learning in Mandarin Chinese: A role for syntactic bootstrapping. Developmental Psychology, 41(3), 529-540. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.41.3.529
URL pmid: 15910160 |
[20] | Li P., & Shirai Y. (2000). The acquisition of lexical and grammatical aspect(Vol.16). Walter de Gruyter. https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110800715 |
[21] |
Ma W., Zhou P., & Golinkoff R. (2020). Young Mandarin learners use function words to distinguish between nouns and verbs. Developmental Science, 23(5), e12927. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12927
doi: 10.1111/desc.v23.5 URL |
[22] |
Ma W., Zhou P., Golinkoff R. M., Lee J., & Hirsh-Pasek K. (2019). Syntactic cues to the noun and verb distinction in Mandarin child-directed speech. First Language, 39(4), 433-461. https://doi.org/10.1177/0142723719845175
doi: 10.1177/0142723719845175 URL |
[23] |
Mahon B. Z., & Caramazza A. (2009). Why does lexical selection have to be so hard? Comment on Abdel Rahman and Melinger's swinging lexical network proposal. Language and Cognitive Processes, 24(5), 735-748. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960802597276
doi: 10.1080/01690960802597276 URL |
[24] | Mahon B. Z., Costa A., Peterson R., Vargas K. A., & Caramazza A. (2007). Lexical selection is not by competition: A reinterpretation of semantic interference and facilitation effects in the picture-word interference paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33(3), 503-535. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.33.3.503 |
[25] |
Maouene J., Hidaka S., & Smith L. B. (2008). Body parts and early-learned verbs. Cognitive Science, 32(7), 1200-1216. https://doi.org/10.1080/03640210802019997
doi: 10.1080/03640210802019997 URL pmid: 21585449 |
[26] |
Morgan E. U., van der Meer A., Vulchanova M., Blasi D. E., & Baggio G. (2020). Meaning before grammar: A review of ERP experiments on the neurodevelopmental origins of semantic processing. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 27, 441-464. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-019-01677-8
doi: 10.3758/s13423-019-01677-8 URL |
[27] | Moyle M. J., Weismer S. E., Evans J. L., & Lindstrom M. J. (2007). Longitudinal relationships between lexical and grammatical development in typical and late-talking children. Journal of Speech Language & Hearing Research, 50(2), 508-528. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2007/035) |
[28] |
Oberecker R., & Friederici A. D. (2006). Syntactic event- related potential components in 24-month-olds’ sentence comprehension. Neuroreport, 17(10), 1017-1021. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.wnr.0000223397.12694.9a
URL pmid: 16791095 |
[29] |
Oberecker R., Friedrich M., & Friederici A. D. (2005). Neural correlates of syntactic processing in two-year-olds. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(10), 1667-1678. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892905774597236
URL pmid: 16269104 |
[30] | Pinker S. (1984). Language learnability and language development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. |
[31] |
Shi R., & Lepage M. (2008). The effect of functional morphemes on word segmentation in preverbal infants. Developmental Science, 11(3), 407-413. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00685.x
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00685.x URL pmid: 18466374 |
[32] |
Shi R., & Melançon A. (2010). Syntactic Categorization in French-Learning Infants. Infancy, 15(5), 517-533. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7078.2009.00022.x
doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7078.2009.00022.x URL pmid: 32693508 |
[33] |
Skeide M. A., & Friederici A. D. (2016). The ontogeny of the cortical language network. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 17(5), 323-332. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2016.23
doi: 10.1038/nrn.2016.23 URL pmid: 27040907 |
[34] |
Singh L., Tan A., & Quinn P. C. (2021). Infants recognize words spoken through opaque masks but not through clear masks. Developmental Science, 24(6), e13117. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.13117
doi: 10.1111/desc.v24.6 URL |
[35] | Tardif T. (2006). But are they really verbs? Chinese words for action. In K. Hirsh-Pasek, & Golinkoff R. M.(Eds.), Action meets word: How children learn verbs. Oxford: Oxford University Press. |
[36] | Tardif T. (2008). The Chinese Communicative Development Inventory: Manual, forms, and norms. Beijing: Peking University Medical Press. |
[ 谭霞灵. (2008). 汉语沟通发展量表使用手册. 北京大学医学出版社.] | |
[37] | Tomblin J. B., & Zhang X. (2006). The dimensionality of language ability in school-age children. Journal of Speech Language & Hearing Research, 49(6), 1193-1208. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2006/086) |
[38] |
Wagley N., & Booth J. R. (2021). Neuro-cognitive development of semantic and syntactic bootstrapping in 6-to 7.5-year-old children. NeuroImage, 241, 118416. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118416
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118416 URL |
[39] | Waxman S. R. (1999). Specifying the scope of 13-month-olds' expectations for novel words. Cognition, 70(3), B35-B50. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00017-7 |
[40] |
Waxman S. R., & Booth A. E. (2001). Seeing pink elephants: Fourteen-month-olds’ interpretations of novel nouns and adjectives. Cognitive Psychology, 43(3), 217-242. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.2001.0764
URL pmid: 11689022 |
[41] | Waxman S. R., & Gelman S. A. (2009). Early word-learning entails reference, not merely associations. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(6), 258-263. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.03.006 |
[42] |
Zhang L. (2016). A character-based constructional approach to Chinese imperfective aspect markers 在 zai and 着 zhe. Acta Linguistica Asiatica, 6(1), 59-79. https://doi.org/10.4312/ala.6.1.59-79
doi: 10.4312/ala.6.1 URL |
[1] | 梁菲菲, 刘瑛, 贺斐, 冯琳琳, 王峥, 白学军. 中文阅读伴随词汇学习中的视觉复杂性效应:基于笔画数和词长的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(12): 1734-1750. |
[2] | 郭存, 谢瑞波, 喻艳玲, 夏月, 王振梁, 伍新春. 小学中高年级儿童复合语素意识、词语结构意识与词汇知识的关系:交叉滞后研究[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(11): 1488-1498. |
[3] | 夏月, 谢瑞波, 王振梁, 阮世芳, 伍新春. 小学低年级汉语儿童语素意识、汉字识别和词汇知识的发展关系——交叉滞后研究[J]. 心理学报, 2022, 54(8): 905-916. |
[4] | 于文勃, 王璐, 瞿邢芳, 王天琳, 张晶晶, 梁丹丹. 转换概率和词长期待对语音统计学习的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(6): 565-574. |
[5] | 程亚华, 王健, 伍新春. 小学低年级儿童汉语语素意识在阅读理解中的 作用:字词阅读流畅性的中介效应[J]. 心理学报, 2018, 50(4): 413-425. |
[6] | 赵英;程亚华;伍新春;阮氏芳. 汉语儿童语素意识与词汇知识的双向关系:一项追踪研究[J]. 心理学报, 2016, 48(11): 1434-1444. |
[7] | 李利平; 伍新春;周宁宁;程亚华; 阮氏芳. 汉语儿童读词者的认知特征及其影响因素[J]. 心理学报, 2016, 48(10): 1270-1281. |
[8] | 章玉祉; 张积家. 汉语言语产生中的词长效应[J]. 心理学报, 2014, 46(9): 1232-1241. |
[9] | 徐展,李毕琴. 工作记忆中汉语反词长效应机制[J]. 心理学报, 2009, 41(09): 802-811. |
[10] | 宁宁,卢春明,彭聃龄,马振玲,丁国盛. 口吃者的言语计划缺陷 ——来自词长效应的证据 [J]. 心理学报, 2007, 39(2): 215-224. |
[11] | 刘霞,陶沙. 汉语儿童英语口语词汇与阅读学习的关系[J]. 心理学报, 2007, 39(01): 118-128. |
[12] | 庄捷,周晓林. 言语产生中的词长效应[J]. 心理学报, 2001, 33(3): 23-27. |
阅读次数 | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
全文 3294
|
|
|||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
摘要 1740
|
|
|||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||