[1] |
Bates E., & Goodman J. (2001). On the inseparability of grammar and the lexicon:Evidence from acquisition. In M. Tomasello & E. Bates (Eds.), Language development: The essential readings(pp.134-162). Oxford: Blackwell Publishing.
|
[2] |
Brown R. W. (1957). Linguistic determinism and the part of speech. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 55(1), 1. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0041199
doi: 10.1037/h0041199
URL
|
[3] |
Caglar-Ryeng Ø., Eklund K., & Nergård-Nilssen T. (2019). Lexical and grammatical development in children at family risk of dyslexia from early childhood to school entry: A cross-lagged analysis. Journal of Child Language, 46(6), 1102-1126. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000919000333
doi: 10.1017/S0305000919000333
URL
pmid: 31317848
|
[4] |
Cao A., & Lewis M. (2022). Quantifying the syntactic bootstrapping effect in verb learning: A meta‐analytic synthesis. Developmental Science, 25(2), e13176. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.13176
doi: 10.1111/desc.v25.2
URL
|
[5] |
Chen Y. X., & Zhu L. Q. (2014). Associations of body parts and early-learned Mandarin verbs and their effect on AoA of these verbs. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 46(7), 912-921. https://doi.org/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2014.00912.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2014.00912
URL
|
|
[ 陈永香, 朱莉琪. (2014). 身体部位与早期习得的汉语动词的联结及其对动词习得年龄的影响. 心理学报, 46(7), 912-921. https://doi.org/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2014.00912 ]
|
[6] |
Chen Y. X., & Zhu L. Q. (2023). Two-year-olds use body part information to speed up verb comprehension. Journal of Psychological Science, 46(2), 355-362. http://www.psysci.org/CN/Y2023/V46/I2/355.
|
|
[ 陈永香, 朱莉琪. (2023). 两岁儿童利用身体部位信息快速识别动词. 心理科学, 46(2), 355-362. http://www.psysci.org/CN/Y2023/V46/I2/355]
|
[7] |
de Carvalho A., He A. X., Lidz J., & Christophe A. (2019). Prosody and function words cue the acquisition of word meanings in 18-month-old infants. Psychological Science, 30(3), 319-332. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797618814131
doi: 10.1177/0956797618814131
URL
pmid: 30668928
|
[8] |
Fisher C. (1996). Structural limits on verb mapping: The role of analogy in children's interpretations of sentences. Cognitive Psychology, 31(1), 41-81. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1996.0012
URL
pmid: 8812021
|
[9] |
Fisher C., & Gleitman L. R. (2002). Language acquisition. John Wiley & Sons, Inc. 10.1002/0471214426.pas0311
|
[10] |
Fisher C., Jin K. S., & Scott R. M. (2020). The developmental origins of syntactic bootstrapping. Topics in Cognitive Science, 12(1), 48-77. https://doi.org/10.1111/tops.12447
doi: 10.1111/tops.12447
URL
pmid: 31419084
|
[11] |
Gleitman L. (1990). The structural sources of verb meanings. Language Acquisition, 1(1), 3-55. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327817la0101_2
doi: 10.1207/s15327817la0101_2
URL
|
[12] |
Guasti M. T. (2017). Language acquisition: The growth of grammar (2nd ed.). MIT Press.
|
[13] |
Hao M., Liu Y., Shu H., Xing A., Jiang Y., & Li P. (2015). Developmental changes in the early child lexicon in Mandarin Chinese. Journal of Child Language, 42(3), 505-537. https://doi.org/10.1017/S030500091400018X
doi: 10.1017/S030500091400018X
URL
pmid: 24967509
|
[14] |
He A. X., & Lidz J. (2017). Verb learning in 14-and 18-month-old English-learning infants. Language Learning and Development, 13(3), 335-356. https://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2017.1285238
doi: 10.1080/15475441.2017.1285238
URL
|
[15] |
Huang R.(L.), Fletcher P., Zhang Z., Liang W.,Marchman, V., & Tardif, T.(2022). Early grammatical marking development in Mandarin-speaking toddlers. Developmental Psychology, 58(4), 631-645. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001316
doi: 10.1037/dev0001316
URL
pmid: 35343712
|
[16] |
Imai M., Haryu E., & Okada H. (2005). Mapping novel nouns and verbs onto dynamic action events: Are verb meanings easier to learn than noun meanings for Japanese children? Child Development, 76(2), 340-355. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00849.x
URL
pmid: 15784086
|
[17] |
Imai M., Li L., Haryu E., Okada H., Hirsh-Pasek K., Golinkoff R., & Shigematsu J. (2008). Novel Noun and Verb Learning in Chinese-, English-, and Japanese-Speaking Children. Child Development, 79(4), 979-1000. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01171.x
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01171.x
URL
pmid: 18717902
|
[18] |
Klein W., Li P., & Hendriks H. (2000). Aspect and assertion in Mandarin Chinese. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 18(4), 723-770. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1006411825993
doi: 10.1023/A:1006411825993
URL
|
[19] |
Lee J. N., & Naigles L. R. (2005). The input to verb learning in Mandarin Chinese: A role for syntactic bootstrapping. Developmental Psychology, 41(3), 529-540. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.41.3.529
URL
pmid: 15910160
|
[20] |
Li P., & Shirai Y. (2000). The acquisition of lexical and grammatical aspect(Vol.16). Walter de Gruyter. https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110800715
|
[21] |
Ma W., Zhou P., & Golinkoff R. (2020). Young Mandarin learners use function words to distinguish between nouns and verbs. Developmental Science, 23(5), e12927. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12927
doi: 10.1111/desc.v23.5
URL
|
[22] |
Ma W., Zhou P., Golinkoff R. M., Lee J., & Hirsh-Pasek K. (2019). Syntactic cues to the noun and verb distinction in Mandarin child-directed speech. First Language, 39(4), 433-461. https://doi.org/10.1177/0142723719845175
doi: 10.1177/0142723719845175
URL
|
[23] |
Mahon B. Z., & Caramazza A. (2009). Why does lexical selection have to be so hard? Comment on Abdel Rahman and Melinger's swinging lexical network proposal. Language and Cognitive Processes, 24(5), 735-748. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960802597276
doi: 10.1080/01690960802597276
URL
|
[24] |
Mahon B. Z., Costa A., Peterson R., Vargas K. A., & Caramazza A. (2007). Lexical selection is not by competition: A reinterpretation of semantic interference and facilitation effects in the picture-word interference paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33(3), 503-535. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.33.3.503
|
[25] |
Maouene J., Hidaka S., & Smith L. B. (2008). Body parts and early-learned verbs. Cognitive Science, 32(7), 1200-1216. https://doi.org/10.1080/03640210802019997
doi: 10.1080/03640210802019997
URL
pmid: 21585449
|
[26] |
Morgan E. U., van der Meer A., Vulchanova M., Blasi D. E., & Baggio G. (2020). Meaning before grammar: A review of ERP experiments on the neurodevelopmental origins of semantic processing. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 27, 441-464. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-019-01677-8
doi: 10.3758/s13423-019-01677-8
URL
|
[27] |
Moyle M. J., Weismer S. E., Evans J. L., & Lindstrom M. J. (2007). Longitudinal relationships between lexical and grammatical development in typical and late-talking children. Journal of Speech Language & Hearing Research, 50(2), 508-528. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2007/035)
|
[28] |
Oberecker R., & Friederici A. D. (2006). Syntactic event- related potential components in 24-month-olds’ sentence comprehension. Neuroreport, 17(10), 1017-1021. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.wnr.0000223397.12694.9a
URL
pmid: 16791095
|
[29] |
Oberecker R., Friedrich M., & Friederici A. D. (2005). Neural correlates of syntactic processing in two-year-olds. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(10), 1667-1678. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892905774597236
URL
pmid: 16269104
|
[30] |
Pinker S. (1984). Language learnability and language development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
|
[31] |
Shi R., & Lepage M. (2008). The effect of functional morphemes on word segmentation in preverbal infants. Developmental Science, 11(3), 407-413. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00685.x
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00685.x
URL
pmid: 18466374
|
[32] |
Shi R., & Melançon A. (2010). Syntactic Categorization in French-Learning Infants. Infancy, 15(5), 517-533. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7078.2009.00022.x
doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7078.2009.00022.x
URL
pmid: 32693508
|
[33] |
Skeide M. A., & Friederici A. D. (2016). The ontogeny of the cortical language network. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 17(5), 323-332. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2016.23
doi: 10.1038/nrn.2016.23
URL
pmid: 27040907
|
[34] |
Singh L., Tan A., & Quinn P. C. (2021). Infants recognize words spoken through opaque masks but not through clear masks. Developmental Science, 24(6), e13117. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.13117
doi: 10.1111/desc.v24.6
URL
|
[35] |
Tardif T. (2006). But are they really verbs? Chinese words for action. In K. Hirsh-Pasek, & Golinkoff R. M.(Eds.), Action meets word: How children learn verbs. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
|
[36] |
Tardif T. (2008). The Chinese Communicative Development Inventory: Manual, forms, and norms. Beijing: Peking University Medical Press.
|
|
[ 谭霞灵. (2008). 汉语沟通发展量表使用手册. 北京大学医学出版社.]
|
[37] |
Tomblin J. B., & Zhang X. (2006). The dimensionality of language ability in school-age children. Journal of Speech Language & Hearing Research, 49(6), 1193-1208. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2006/086)
|
[38] |
Wagley N., & Booth J. R. (2021). Neuro-cognitive development of semantic and syntactic bootstrapping in 6-to 7.5-year-old children. NeuroImage, 241, 118416. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118416
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118416
URL
|
[39] |
Waxman S. R. (1999). Specifying the scope of 13-month-olds' expectations for novel words. Cognition, 70(3), B35-B50. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00017-7
|
[40] |
Waxman S. R., & Booth A. E. (2001). Seeing pink elephants: Fourteen-month-olds’ interpretations of novel nouns and adjectives. Cognitive Psychology, 43(3), 217-242. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.2001.0764
URL
pmid: 11689022
|
[41] |
Waxman S. R., & Gelman S. A. (2009). Early word-learning entails reference, not merely associations. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(6), 258-263. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.03.006
|
[42] |
Zhang L. (2016). A character-based constructional approach to Chinese imperfective aspect markers 在 zai and 着 zhe. Acta Linguistica Asiatica, 6(1), 59-79. https://doi.org/10.4312/ala.6.1.59-79
doi: 10.4312/ala.6.1
URL
|