心理学报 ›› 2023, Vol. 55 ›› Issue (3): 481-495.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2023.00481
收稿日期:
2021-12-20
发布日期:
2022-12-22
出版日期:
2023-03-25
通讯作者:
李健,倪荫梅
E-mail:li.jian@pku.edu.cn;niyinmei@pku.edu.cn
基金资助:
HUANG Xinru1, LI Jian1,2(), NI Yinmei1()
Received:
2021-12-20
Online:
2022-12-22
Published:
2023-03-25
Contact:
LI Jian, NI Yinmei
E-mail:li.jian@pku.edu.cn;niyinmei@pku.edu.cn
摘要:
在不同社会偏好类型中, 研究者较为关注利他偏好及其信号功能。本研究探究在独裁者游戏中, 决策者的利他偏好如何受到分配方案对接受者可见性的影响。实验1采用行为实验结合计算建模的方法, 发现无论在选择或评分条件下, 相比于行为不可见, 当分配者的行为能够为接受者所见时, 分配者都表现出更大程度的利他偏好。此外, 相比于评分条件, 在选择时人们更加在意分配效率。实验2仅使用选择条件, 并操纵社会规范, 发现行为可见增加利他偏好的作用依赖于利他的社会规范, 当存在非利他社会规范时, 行为可见的影响减小。本研究结果表明, 在利他社会规范下, 当行为对接受者可见时, 人们将表现出更多利他偏好。
黄馨茹, 李健, 倪荫梅. (2023). 行为可见增加利他偏好及其社会规范机制. 心理学报, 55(3), 481-495.
HUANG Xinru, LI Jian, NI Yinmei. (2023). Social norm modulates the enhancement effect of behavioral visibility on altruistic preference. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 55(3), 481-495.
图4 实验1基于模型的结果(a) (b)模型估计的AIA和DIA; (c)选择和评分条件下, 可见与不可见条件之间的AIA之差与DIA之差; (d)可见和不可条件下, 选择与评分条件之间的AIA之差与DIA之差。(a) (b)中, 每个箱图上方的显著性表示该条件下参数显著大于零, 下方的显著性表示该条件下参数显著小于零。图像最上方的显著性表示可见与不可见情况下, 选择和评分条件的差值的比较。箱图内部的水平实线代表中位数, 水平虚线代表均值。 * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001。(c) (d)中, 浅色图案为个体层面模型拟合结果, 深色图案为各个条件下的均值。
图6 实验2结果 (a) (b)优势和劣势情况下, 自我他人收益之差; (c)平均分给他人的点数; (d) (e)分层贝叶斯模型拟合的AIA和DIA。图中, 箱图内部的水平实线代表中位数, 水平虚线代表均值。(d) (e)中, 每个箱图上方的显著性表示该条件下参数显著大于零, 下方的显著性表示该条件下参数显著小于零。 * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001。
[1] |
Agerström J., Carlsson R., Nicklasson L., & Guntell L. (2016). Using descriptive social norms to increase charitable giving: The power of local norms. Journal of Economic Psychology, 52, 147-153.
doi: 10.1016/j.joep.2015.12.007 URL |
[2] |
Andreoni J., & Bernheim B. D. (2009). Social image and the 50-50 norm: A theoretical and experimental analysis of audience effects. Econometrica, 77(5), 1607-1636.
doi: 10.3982/ECTA7384 URL |
[3] |
Andreoni J., & Petrie R. (2004). Public goods experiments without confidentiality: A glimpse into fund-raising. Journal of Public Economics, 88(7), 1605-1623.
doi: 10.1016/S0047-2727(03)00040-9 URL |
[4] |
Bateson M., Nettle D., & Roberts G. (2006). Cues of being watched enhance cooperation in a real-world setting. Biology Letters, 2(3), 412-414.
doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2006.0509 pmid: 17148417 |
[5] |
Böckler A., Tusche A., & Singer T. (2016). The structure of human prosociality: Differentiating altruistically motivated, norm motivated, strategically motivated, and self-reported prosocial behavior. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 7(6), 530-541.
doi: 10.1177/1948550616639650 URL |
[6] | Bradley A., Lawrence C., & Ferguson E. (2018). Does observability affect prosociality? Proceedings of the Royal Society. B, Biological Sciences, 285(1875), 20180116. |
[7] |
Brüne M., & Brüne-Cohrs U. (2006). Theory of mind— Evolution, ontogeny, brain mechanisms and psychopathology. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 30(4), 437-455.
doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.08.001 URL |
[8] |
Burnham T. C., & Hare B. (2007). Engineering human cooperation: Does involuntary neural activation increase public goods contributions? Human Nature, 18(2), 88-108.
doi: 10.1007/s12110-007-9012-2 URL |
[9] |
Choshen-Hillel S., & Yaniv I. (2011). Agency and the construction of social preference: Between inequality aversion and prosocial behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101(6), 1253-1261.
doi: 10.1037/a0024557 pmid: 21767033 |
[10] |
Choshen-Hillel S., & Yaniv I. (2012). Social preferences shaped by conflicting motives: When enhancing social welfare creates unfavorable comparisons for the self. Judgment and Decision Making, 7(5), 618-627.
doi: 10.1017/S1930297500006331 URL |
[11] |
Cialdini R. B., Reno R. R., & Kallgren C. A. (1990). A focus theory of normative conduct: Recycling the concept of norms to reduce littering in public places. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58(6), 1015-1026.
doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.58.6.1015 URL |
[12] |
Dana J., Cain D. M., & Dawes R. M. (2006). What you don’t know won’t hurt me: Costly (but quiet) exit in dictator games. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 100(2), 193-201.
doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2005.10.001 URL |
[13] |
Dempsey R. C., McAlaney J., & Bewick B. M. (2018). A critical appraisal of the social norms approach as an interventional strategy for health-related behavior and attitude change. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 2180.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02180 pmid: 30459694 |
[14] |
Elliott R., Bohart A. C., Watson J. C., & Greenberg L. S. (2011). Empathy. Psychotherapy, 48(1), 43-49.
doi: 10.1037/a0022187 pmid: 21401273 |
[15] | Elster J. (1989). Social norms and economic theory. The Journal of Economic Perspectives, 3(4), 99-117. |
[16] |
Fathi M., Bateson M., & Nettle D. (2014). Effects of watching eyes and norm cues on charitable giving in a surreptitious behavioral experiment. Evolutionary Psychology, 12(5), 878-887.
pmid: 25331033 |
[17] |
Fehr E., & Fischbacher U. (2004). Third-party punishment and social norms. Evolution and Human Behavior, 25(2), 63-87.
doi: 10.1016/S1090-5138(04)00005-4 URL |
[18] |
Fehr E., & Schmidt K. M. (1999). A theory of fairness, competition, and cooperation. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 114(3), 817-868.
doi: 10.1162/003355399556151 URL |
[19] |
Fox J., & Guyer M. (1978). "Public" choice and cooperation in n-person prisoner's dilemma. The Journal of Conflict Resolution, 22(3), 469-481.
doi: 10.1177/002200277802200307 URL |
[20] | Gao X., Yu H., Sáez I., Blue P. R., Zhu L., Hsu M., & Zhou X. (2018). Distinguishing neural correlates of context-dependent advantageous- and disadvantageous- inequity aversion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(33), E7680-E7689. |
[21] |
Gintis H., Smith E. A., & Bowles S. (2001). Costly signaling and cooperation. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 213(1), 103-119.
pmid: 11708857 |
[22] | Glimcher P. W., & Fehr E. (2013). Neuroeconomics: Decision making and the brain: Second edition. Academic Press. |
[23] |
Haley K. J., & Fessler D. M. T. (2005). Nobody's watching? Subtle cues affect generosity in an anonymous economic game. Evolution and Human Behavior, 26(3), 245-256.
doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2005.01.002 URL |
[24] |
Hamilton A., & Lind F. (2016). Audience effects: What can they tell us about social neuroscience, theory of mind and autism? Culture and Brain, 4(2), 159-177.
doi: 10.1007/s40167-016-0044-5 pmid: 27867833 |
[25] |
Hardy C. L., & van Vugt M. (2006). Nice guys finish first: The competitive altruism hypothesis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32(10), 1402-1413.
pmid: 16963610 |
[26] | Hesslinger V. M., Carbon C., & Hecht H. (2017). The sense of being watched is modulated by arousal and duration of the perceptual episode. I-Perception, 8(6), 2041669517742179. |
[27] |
Hu Y., He L., Zhang L., Wölk T., Dreher J., & Weber B. (2018). Spreading inequality: Neural computations underlying paying-it-forward reciprocity. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 13(6), 578-589.
doi: 10.1093/scan/nsy040 pmid: 29897606 |
[28] |
Jerdee T. H., & Rosen B. (1974). Effects of opportunity to communicate and visibility of individual decisions on behavior in the common interest. Journal of Applied Psychology, 59(6), 712-716.
doi: 10.1037/h0037450 URL |
[29] |
Kawamura Y., & Kusumi T. (2017). The norm-dependent effect of watching eyes on donation. Evolution and Human Behavior, 38(5), 659-666.
doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2017.05.003 URL |
[30] |
Kimbrough E. O., & Vostroknutov A. (2016). Norms make preferences social. Journal of the European Economic Association, 14(3), 608-638.
doi: 10.1111/jeea.12152 URL |
[31] |
Lacetera N., & Macis M. (2010). Social image concerns and prosocial behavior: Field evidence from a nonlinear incentive scheme. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 76(2), 225-237.
doi: 10.1016/j.jebo.2010.08.007 URL |
[32] |
Li O., Xu F., & Wang L. (2018). Advantageous inequity aversion does not always exist: The role of determining allocations modulates preferences for advantageous inequity. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 749-749.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00749 pmid: 29887817 |
[33] |
Liu Y., Li S., Lin W., Li W., Yan X., Wang X., … Ma Y. (2019). Oxytocin modulates social value representations in the amygdala. Nature Neuroscience, 22(4), 633-641.
doi: 10.1038/s41593-019-0351-1 pmid: 30911182 |
[34] |
Loewenstein G. F., Thompson L., & Bazerman M. H. (1989). Social utility and decision making in interpersonal contexts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57(3), 426-441.
doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.57.3.426 URL |
[35] |
McAuliffe K., Blake P. R., Kim G., Wrangham R. W., & Warneken F. (2013). Social influences on inequity aversion in children. PloS One, 8(12), e80966.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0080966 URL |
[36] |
McAuliffe K., Blake P. R., & Warneken F. (2020). Costly fairness in children is influenced by who is watching. Developmental Psychology, 56(4), 773-782.
doi: 10.1037/dev0000888 pmid: 31999186 |
[37] |
McAuliffe K., Jordan J. J., & Warneken F. (2015). Costly third-party punishment in young children. Cognition, 134, 1-10.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.08.013 pmid: 25460374 |
[38] |
McBride M., & Ridinger G. (2021). Beliefs also make social-norm preferences social. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 191(3), 765-784.
doi: 10.1016/j.jebo.2021.09.030 URL |
[39] |
Milinski M., Semmann D., & Krambeck H. (2002). Reputation helps solve the 'tragedy of the commons'. Nature, 415(6870), 424-426.
doi: 10.1038/415424a URL |
[40] |
Murphy R. O., Ackermann K. A., & Handgraaf M. J. J. (2011). Measuring social value orientation. Judgment and Decision Making, 6(8), 771-781.
doi: 10.1017/S1930297500004204 URL |
[41] |
Nettle D., Harper Z., Kidson A., Stone R., Penton-Voak I. S., & Bateson M. (2013). The watching eyes effect in the dictator game: It's not how much you give, it's being seen to give something. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34(1), 35-40.
doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2012.08.004 URL |
[42] | Oda R., & Ichihashi R. (2016). Effects of eye images and norm cues on charitable donation: A field experiment in an izakaya. Evolutionary Psychology, 14(4), 147470491666887. |
[43] |
Oda R., Niwa Y., Honma A., & Hiraishi K. (2011). An eye-like painting enhances the expectation of a good reputation. Evolution and Human Behavior, 32(3), 166-171.
doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.11.002 URL |
[44] | Palacios J., Fan Y., Yoeli E., Wang J., Chai Y., Sun W., … Zheng S. (2022). Encouraging the resumption of economic activity after COVID-19:Evidence from a large scale-field experiment in China. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 119(5), e2100719119. |
[45] |
Pereda M., Brañas-Garza P., Rodríguez-Lara I., & Sánchez A. (2017). The emergence of altruism as a social norm. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 9684.
doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-07712-9 pmid: 28851876 |
[46] |
Pfattheicher S., Nielsen Y. A., & Thielmann I. (2022). Prosocial behavior and altruism: A review of concepts and definitions. Current Opinion in Psychology, 44, 124-129.
doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2021.08.021 URL |
[47] |
Piazza J., & Bering J. M. (2008). Concerns about reputation via gossip promote generous allocations in an economic game. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29(3), 172-178.
doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2007.12.002 URL |
[48] | Raiffa H., & Schlaifer R. (1961). Applied statistical decision theory. University College London: Biometrika Office. |
[49] | Raihani N. J., & Bshary R. (2012). A positive effect of flowers rather than eye images in a large-scale, cross-cultural dictator game. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 279(1742), 3556-3564. |
[50] |
Raihani N. J., & McAuliffe K. (2014). Dictator game giving: The importance of descriptive versus injunctive norms. PloS One, 9(12), e113826.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0113826 URL |
[51] |
Roberts G. (1998). Competitive altruism: From reciprocity to the handicap principle. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 265(1394), 427-431.
doi: 10.1098/rspb.1998.0312 URL |
[52] |
Sáez I., Zhu L., Set E., Kayser A., & Hsu M. (2015). Dopamine modulates egalitarian behavior in humans. Current Biology, 25(7), 912-919.
doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.01.071 pmid: 25802148 |
[53] | Suzuki S., & Akiyama E. (2005). Reputation and the evolution of cooperation in sizable groups. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 272(1570), 1373-1377. |
[54] |
Sylwester K., & Roberts G. (2010). Cooperators benefit through reputation-based partner choice in economic games. Biology Letters, 6(5), 659-662.
doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2010.0209 pmid: 20410026 |
[55] | Tian Y. (2016). Research on game experiments of fairness preference: The influence of dominance status, punishment situation and fairness perception on the fairness preference (Unpublished master’s thesis). Harbin Engineering University, China. |
[ 田莹. (2016). 公平偏好的博弈实验研究——支配地位、惩罚情境和公平感知对公平偏好的影响 (硕士学位论文). 哈尔滨工程大学.] | |
[56] |
Tsoi L., & McAuliffe K. (2020). Individual differences in theory of mind predict inequity aversion in children. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 46(4), 559-571.
doi: 10.1177/0146167219867957 pmid: 31449439 |
[57] |
van Lange P. A. M. (1999). The pursuit of joint outcomes and equality in outcomes: An integrative model of social value orientation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(2), 337-349.
doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.77.2.337 URL |
[58] |
van Lange P. A. M., de Bruin E. M. N., Otten W., & Joireman J. A. (1997). The development of prosocial, individualistic, and competitive orientations: Theory and preliminary evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73(4), 733-746.
pmid: 9325591 |
[59] | van Vugt M., Roberts G., & Hardy C. (2012). ompetitive altruism:A theory of reputation-based cooperation in groups. In L. Barrett & R. Dunbar(Eds.), Oxford Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology (pp. 531-540). Oxford University Press. |
[60] |
Wedekind C., & Braithwaite V. A. (2002). The long-term benefits of human generosity in indirect reciprocity. Current Biology, 12(12), 1012-1015.
pmid: 12123575 |
[61] |
West S. A., Griffin A. S., & Gardner A. (2007). Social semantics: Altruism, cooperation, mutualism, strong reciprocity and group selection. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 20(2), 415-432.
pmid: 17305808 |
[62] |
Wu T., & Han S. (2021). Neural mechanisms of modulations of empathy and altruism by beliefs of others’ pain. eLife, 10, e66043.
doi: 10.7554/eLife.66043 URL |
[1] | 占友龙, 肖啸, 谭千保, 李琎, 钟毅平. 声誉关注与社会距离对伤害困境中道德决策的影响:来自行为与ERPs的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2022, 54(6): 613-627. |
[2] | 陈思静, 杨莎莎, 汪昊, 万丰华. 主观社会阶层正向预测利他性惩罚[J]. 心理学报, 2022, 54(12): 1548-1561. |
[3] | 杨焕, 卫旭华. 关系型人力资源管理实践对受益人利他行为的影响:基于道德补偿的视角[J]. 心理学报, 2022, 54(10): 1248-1261. |
[4] | 陈思静, 濮雪丽, 朱玥, 汪昊, 刘建伟. 规范错觉对外出就餐中食物浪费的影响:心理机制与应对策略[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(8): 904-918. |
[5] | 陈思静, 邢懿琳, 翁异静, 黎常. 第三方惩罚对合作的溢出效应:基于社会规范的解释[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(7): 758-772. |
[6] | 高娟, 王鹏, 王晓田, 孙倩, 刘永芳. 得失情境下他人参照点及心理距离对自我-他人利益权衡的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(5): 633-644. |
[7] | 陈思静, 徐烨超. “仁者”还是“智者”:第三方惩罚对惩罚者声誉的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(12): 1436-1451. |
[8] | 殷西乐, 李建标, 陈思宇, 刘晓丽, 郝洁. 第三方惩罚的神经机制:来自经颅直流电刺激的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2019, 51(5): 571-583. |
[9] | 何贵兵, 杨鑫蔚, 蒋多. 环境损益的社会折扣:利他人格的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(10): 1334-1343. |
[10] | 费定舟;钱东海;黄旭辰. 利他行为的自我控制过程模型:自我损耗下的道德情绪的正向作用[J]. 心理学报, 2016, 48(9): 1175-1183. |
[11] | 何宁;朱云莉. 自爱与他爱:自恋、共情与内隐利他的关系[J]. 心理学报, 2016, 48(2): 199-210. |
[12] | 肖二平;张积家;王娟. 摩梭走访制下的阿注关系:是亲属还是朋友?[J]. 心理学报, 2015, 47(12): 1486-1498. |
[13] | 任俊;李瑞雪;詹鋆;刘迪;林曼;彭年强. 好人可能做出坏行为的心理学解释 —— 基于自我控制资源损耗的研究证据[J]. 心理学报, 2014, 46(6): 841-851. |
[14] | 何贵兵;蒋多. 任务框架及利他人格对社会折扣的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2013, 45(10): 1131-1146. |
[15] | 吴燕,罗跃嘉. 利他惩罚中的结果评价—— ERP研究[J]. 心理学报, 2011, 43(06): 661-673. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||