心理学报 ›› 2024, Vol. 56 ›› Issue (2): 226-238.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2024.00226 cstr: 32110.14.2024.00226
收稿日期:
2022-07-05
发布日期:
2023-12-01
出版日期:
2024-02-25
基金资助:
ZHENG Xiaoying1,2, HAN Runlei3, LIU Ruhan3, XU Jing3()
Received:
2022-07-05
Online:
2023-12-01
Published:
2024-02-25
摘要:
在互联网公益情境下, 囿于人际接触与沟通的缺乏, 信息成为影响人们公益参与决策的最重要的因素。本研究探讨了信息加工流畅性与真实性对个体捐助行为的影响。通过4个实验, 作者发现:当求助信息真实性不明确时, 人们会将信息加工流畅性作为推断信息可信度的线索:信息加工越流畅, 感知信息可信度越高, 进而捐助意愿越高。当求助信息真实性得到核实之后, 人们会将信息加工流畅性作为推断求助者困难程度的线索:信息加工越不流畅, 感知求助者的处境越艰难, 进而捐助意愿越高。上述研究发现不仅丰富和拓展了信息加工流畅性对道德行为决策的影响, 同时也为互联网公益情境下个人求助及平台管理实践提供参考和启示。
中图分类号:
郑晓莹, 韩润蕾, 刘汝晗, 徐菁. (2024). 信息加工流畅性与真实性对互联网公益捐助的影响. 心理学报, 56(2), 226-238.
ZHENG Xiaoying, HAN Runlei, LIU Ruhan, XU Jing. (2024). The interactive effect of processing fluency and information credibility on donation in the digital philanthropy context. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 56(2), 226-238.
[1] |
Ackerman, J. M., Nocera, C. C., & Bargh, J. A. (2010). Incidental haptic sensations influence social judgments and decisions. Science, 328(5986), 1712-1715. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1189993
doi: 10.1126/science.1189993 URL pmid: 20576894 |
[2] |
Allen, A. M., Eilert, M., & Peloza, J. (2018). How deviations from performance norms impact charitable donations. Journal of Marketing Research, 55(2), 277-290. https://doi.org/10.1509/jmr.15.0509
doi: 10.1509/jmr.15.0509 URL |
[3] |
Alter, A. L. (2013). The benefits of cognitive disfluency. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22(6), 437-442. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721413498894
doi: 10.1177/0963721413498894 URL |
[4] |
Alter, A. L., & Oppenheimer, D. M. (2006). Predicting short- term stock fluctuations by using processing fluency. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 103(24), 9369-9372. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0601071103
doi: 10.1073/pnas.0601071103 URL |
[5] |
Alter, A. L., & Oppenheimer, D. M. (2009). Uniting the tribes of fluency to form a metacognitive nation. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 13(3), 219-235. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868309341564
doi: 10.1177/1088868309341564 URL pmid: 19638628 |
[6] |
Batson, C. D., Eklund, J. H., Chermok, V. L., Hoyt, J. L., & Ortiz, B. G. (2007). An additional antecedent of empathic concern: Valuing the welfare of the person in need. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93(1), 65-74. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.93.1.65
doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.93.1.65 URL pmid: 17605589 |
[7] |
Das, N., Guha, A., Biswas, A., & Krishnan, B. (2016). How product-cause fit and donation quantifier interact in cause-related marketing (CRM) settings: Evidence of the cue congruency effect. Marketing Letters, 27(2), 295-308. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11002-014-9338-6
doi: 10.1007/s11002-014-9338-6 URL |
[8] |
Fan, Y. F., Jiang, J., & Cui, W. Q. (2019). The backfire effect of default amounts on donation behavior in online donation platform. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 51(4), 415-427.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2019.00415 |
[樊亚凤, 蒋晶, 崔稳权. (2019). 网络公益平台默认选项设置对个人捐赠意愿的影响及作用机制. 心理学报, 51(4), 415-427. https://doi.org/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2019.00415 ]
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2019.00415 URL |
|
[9] |
Fisher, R. J., & Ma, Y. (2014). The price of being beautiful: Negative effects of attractiveness on empathy for children in need. Journal of Consumer Research, 41(2), 436-450. https://doi.org/10.1086/676967
doi: 10.1086/676967 URL |
[10] | Grau, S. L., & Folse, J. A. G. (2007). Cause-related marketing (CRM): The influence of donation proximity and message-framing cues on the less-involved consumer. Journal of Advertising, 36(4), 19-33. https://doi.org/10.2753/JOA0091-3367360402 |
[11] |
Hayes, A. F. (2015). An index and test of linear moderated mediation. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 50(1), 1-22.
doi: 10.1080/00273171.2014.962683 pmid: 26609740 |
[12] |
Jostmann, N. B., Lakens, D., & Schubert, T. W. (2009). Weight as an embodiment of importance. Psychological Science, 20(9), 1169-1174.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02426.x pmid: 19686292 |
[13] |
Jung, M. H., & Critcher, C. R. (2018). How encouraging niceness can incentivize nastiness: An unintended consequence of advertising reform. Journal of Marketing Research, 55(1), 147-161. https://doi.org/10.1509/jmr.14.0654
doi: 10.1509/jmr.14.0654 URL |
[14] |
Labroo, A. A., Dhar, R., & Schwarz, N. (2008). Of frog wines and frowning watches: Semantic priming, perceptual fluency, and brand evaluation. Journal of Consumer Research, 34(6), 819-831. https://doi.org/10.1086/523290
doi: 10.1086/523290 URL |
[15] |
Labroo, A. A., & Kim, S. (2009). The “instrumentality” heuristic: Why metacognitive difficulty is desirable during goal pursuit. Psychological Science, 20(1), 127-134. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02264.x
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02264.x URL |
[16] | Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. University of Chicago Press. |
[17] |
Landwehr, J. R., Labroo, A. A., & Herrmann, A. (2011). Gut liking for the ordinary: Incorporating design fluency improves automobile sales forecasts. Marketing Science, 30(3), 416-429. https://doi.org/10.1287/mksc.1110.0633
doi: 10.1287/mksc.1110.0633 URL |
[18] |
Le, N. Q., Supphellen, M., & Bagozzi, R. P. (2021). Effects of negative social information on the willingness to support charities: The moderating role of regulatory focus. Marketing Letters, 32(1), 111-122. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11002-020-09540-w
doi: 10.1007/s11002-020-09540-w URL |
[19] |
Lee, S., Winterich, K. P., & Ross, W. T. (2014). I’m moral, but I won’t help you: The distinct roles of empathy and justice in donations. Journal of Consumer Research, 41(3), 678-696. https://doi.org/10.1086/677226
doi: 10.1086/677226 URL |
[20] | Lee, S. W. S., & Schwarz, N. (2014). Metaphor in judgment and decision making. In M. Landau, M. D. Robinson, & B. P. Meier (Eds.), The power of metaphor: Examining its influence on social life (pp. 85-108). American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/14278-005 |
[21] |
Liang, J., Chen, Z., & Lei, J. (2016). Inspire me to donate: The use of strength emotion in donation appeals. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 26(2), 283-288. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcps.2015.09.001
doi: 10.1016/j.jcps.2015.09.001 URL |
[22] |
Liu, L., Suh, A., & Wagner, C. (2018). Empathy or perceived credibility? An empirical study on individual donation behavior in charitable crowdfunding. Internet Research, 28(3), 623-651. https://doi.org/10.1108/IntR-06-2017-0240
doi: 10.1108/IntR-06-2017-0240 URL |
[23] | Liu, W. M. (2019). Having power, donating more time? The effect of donors’power on their charitable donation intention of time and the underlying mechanisms. Nankai Business Review, 22(2), 23-32. |
[柳武妹. (2019). 权力越高越倾向于捐赠时间吗? 慈善营销中捐赠者的权力对其时间捐赠意愿的影响及机制研究. 南开管理评论, 22(2), 23-32.] | |
[24] |
Miele, D. B., & Molden, D. C. (2010). Naive theories of intelligence and the role of processing fluency in perceived comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139(3), 535-557. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019745
doi: 10.1037/a0019745 URL |
[25] |
Mittal, B. (1995). A comparative analysis of four scales of consumer involvement. Psychology & Marketing, 12(7), 663-682. https://doi.org/10.1002/mar.4220120708
doi: 10.1002/mar.v12:7 URL |
[26] |
Nielsen, J. H., & Escalas, J. E. (2010). Easier is not always better: The moderating role of processing type on preference fluency. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 20(3), 295-305. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcps.2010.06.016
doi: 10.1016/j.jcps.2010.06.016 URL |
[27] |
Oppenheimer, D. M., & Frank, M. C. (2008). A rose in any other font would not smell as sweet: Effects of perceptual fluency on categorization. Cognition, 106(3), 1178-1194. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2007.05.010
URL pmid: 17618616 |
[28] |
Pocheptsova, A., Labroo, A. A., & Dhar, R. (2010). Making products feel special: When metacognitive difficulty enhances evaluation. Journal of Marketing Research, 47(6), 1059-1069. https://doi.org/10.1509/jmkr.47.6.1059
doi: 10.1509/jmkr.47.6.1059 URL |
[29] |
Reber, R., & Schwarz, N. (1999). Effects of perceptual fluency on judgments of truth. Consciousness and Cognition, 8(3), 338-342. https://doi.org/10.1006/ccog.1999.0386
URL pmid: 10487787 |
[30] |
Reber, R., Schwarz, N., & Winkielman, P. (2004). Processing fluency and aesthetic pleasure: Is beauty in the perceiver’s processing experience? Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8(4), 364-382. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327957pspr0804_3
doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr0804_3 URL pmid: 15582859 |
[31] |
Rennekamp, K. (2012). Processing fluency and investors’ reactions to disclosure readability. Journal of Accounting Research, 50(5), 1319-1354. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-679X.2012.00460.x
doi: 10.1111/joar.2012.50.issue-5 URL |
[32] |
Rotliman, A. J., & Schwarz, N. (1998). Constructing perceptions of vulnerability: Personal relevance and the use of experiential information in health judgments. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24(10), 1053-1064. https://doi.org/10.1177/01461672982410003
doi: 10.1177/01461672982410003 URL |
[33] |
Schubert, T. W. (2005). Your highness: Vertical positions as perceptual symbols of power. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89(1), 1-21. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.89.1.1
URL pmid: 16060739 |
[34] |
Schwarz, N. (2004). Metacognitive experiences in consumer judgment and decision making. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 14(4), 332-334.
doi: 10.1207/s15327663jcp1404_2 URL |
[35] | Schwarz, N. (2012). Feelings-as-information theory. In P. A. M. van Lange, A. W. Kruglanski, & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of theories of social psychology (pp. 289-308). Sage Publications Ltd. https://doi.org/10.4135/9781446249215.n15 |
[36] |
Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (1983). Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well-being: Informative and directive functions of affective states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45(3), 513-523. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.45.3.513
doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.45.3.513 URL |
[37] | Schwarz, N., Sanna, L. J., Skurnik, I., & Yoon, C. (2007). Metacognitive experiences and the intricacies of setting people straight: Implications for debiasing and public information campaigns. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 127-161. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(06)39003-X |
[38] |
Shen, H., Jiang, Y., & Adaval, R. (2010). Contrast and assimilation effects of processing fluency. Journal of Consumer Research, 36(5), 876-889. https://doi.org/10.1086/612425
doi: 10.1086/612425 URL |
[39] |
Small, D. A., & Cryder, C. (2016). Prosocial consumer behavior. Current Opinion in Psychology, 10, 107-111. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2016.01.001
doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2016.01.001 URL |
[40] |
Small, D. A., & Loewenstein, G. (2003). Helping a victim or helping the victim: Altruism and identifiability. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 26(1), 5-16. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1022299422219
doi: 10.1023/A:1022299422219 URL |
[41] |
Song, H., & Schwarz, N. (2008). If it’s hard to read, it’s hard to do: Processing fluency affects effort prediction and motivation. Psychological Science, 19(10), 986-988. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02189.x
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02189.x URL |
[42] |
Song, W. J., Chen, Y. X., & Huang, Y. H. (2023). Emphasizing recovery or improvement in charitable fundraising should depend on event controllability. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 55(7), 1133-1147.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2023.01133 |
[宋文静, 陈怡煖, 黄韫慧. (2023). 募捐信息该强调恢复还是改善受事件可控性调节. 心理学报, 55(7), 1133-1147. https://doi.org/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2023.01133 ] | |
[43] |
Tsai, C. I., & Thomas, M. (2011). When does feeling of fluency matter? How abstract and concrete thinking influence fluency effects. Psychological Science, 22(3), 348-354. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611398494
doi: 10.1177/0956797611398494 URL |
[44] |
Tsiros, M., & Irmak, C. (2020). Lowering the minimum donation amount increases consumer purchase likelihood of products associated with cause-related marketing campaigns. Journal of Marketing Research, 57(4), 755-770. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022243720916463
doi: 10.1177/0022243720916463 URL |
[45] |
Wang, C., Zhu, R., & Handy, T. C. (2016). Experiencing haptic roughness promotes empathy. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 26(3), 350-362. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcps.2015.11.001
doi: 10.1016/j.jcps.2015.11.001 URL |
[46] |
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6), 1063-1070. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063
doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.54.6.1063 URL pmid: 3397865 |
[47] |
Williams, L. E., & Bargh, J. A. (2008). Experiencing physical warmth promotes interpersonal warmth. Science, 322(5901), 606-607. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1162548.
doi: 10.1126/science.1162548 URL pmid: 18948544 |
[48] | Winkielman, P., Schwarz, N., Fazendeiro, T. A., & Reber, R. (2003). The hedonic marking of processing fluency:Implications for evaluative judgment. In J. Musch & K. C. Klauer (Eds.), The psychology of evaluation: Affective processes in cognition and emotion (pp. 189-217). Erlbaum. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecoser.2018.05.004 |
[49] |
Yin, R., Su, D. Q., & Ye, H. S. (2013). Conceptual metaphor theory: Basing on theories of embodied cognition. Advances in Psychological Science, 21(2), 220-234.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2013.00220 |
[殷融, 苏得权, 叶浩生. (2013). 具身认知视角下的概念隐喻理论. 心理科学进展, 21(2), 220-234. https://doi.org/10.3724/SP.J.1042.2013.00220]
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2013.00220 URL |
|
[50] | Zhang, X. J. (2010). Effects of perceptual fluency on judgment and decision. Advances in Psychological Science, 18(4), 639-645. |
[张旭锦. (2010). 知觉流畅性对判断和决策的影响. 心理科学进展, 18(4), 639-645.] | |
[51] |
Zheng, X. Y., Peng, S. Q., & Peng, L. L. (2015). Feeling better and becoming more benevolent: Impact of social comparison on prosocial behavior. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 47(2), 243-250.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2015.00243 URL |
[郑晓莹, 彭泗清, 彭璐珞. (2015). “达”则兼济天下? 社会比较对亲社会行为的影响及心理机制. 心理学报, 47(2), 243-250. https://doi.org/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2015.00243 ] | |
[52] |
Zhong, C. B., & Leonardelli, G. J. (2008). Cold and lonely: Does social exclusion literally feel cold? Psychological Science, 19(9), 838-842. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02165.x
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02165.x URL |
[53] |
Zhong, C. B., & Liljenquist, K. (2006). Washing away your sins: Threatened morality and physical cleansing. Science, 313(5792), 1451-1452. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1130726
doi: 10.1126/science.1130726 URL |
[1] | 何宁, 王紫祎, 林嘉浩, 李梦, 游旭群. 叙述性信息对狭隘合作行为的影响机制[J]. 心理学报, 2025, 57(4): 513-525. |
[2] | 蒯玲, 卫海英, 姚琦, 肖婷文, 谢升成. 时间标志对炫耀性亲社会行为意愿的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2025, 57(4): 526-543. |
[3] | 韩宪国, 金国敏, 李丹, 刘世宏, 吴琴, 刘俊升, 陈欣银. 父母温暖与儿童晚期亲社会行为的关系: 集体取向的作用[J]. 心理学报, 2025, 57(4): 614-630. |
[4] | 林荣茂, 余巧华, 胡添祥, 张九妹, 叶玉珊, 连榕. 敬畏感与亲社会行为关系的三水平和结构方程模型元分析[J]. 心理学报, 2025, 57(4): 631-651. |
[5] | 许丽颖, 张语嫣, 喻丰. 感知机器人威胁降低亲社会倾向[J]. 心理学报, 2025, 57(4): 671-699. |
[6] | 冯文婷, 薛舒允, 汪涛. 拟人的品牌更环保?拟人化沟通对促进绿色消费倾向的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2025, 57(4): 720-738. |
[7] | 邓碧琳, 王婷, 张积家, 刘显翠, 刘莎. 裕固族中学生族群认同的容器隐喻和差序格局[J]. 心理学报, 2025, 57(3): 428-446. |
[8] | 任小云, 段锦云, 张茜. 社会阶层成长型心智对大学生学业和职业投入的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2025, 57(3): 447-462. |
[9] | 高学德, 马号云. 中国民众普遍信任的动态变迁——基于年龄-时期-世代效应的分析[J]. 心理学报, 2025, 57(3): 463-478. |
[10] | 冯萌萌, 都旭, 孙世南, 郭秀丽, 林爽, 白学军. 何为成功之母? 绝对和相对反馈情境下失败和成功次数对耐挫的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2025, 57(2): 260-274. |
[11] | 陆嘉琦, 李雨斯, 何贵兵. 双相障碍患者的风险决策偏好:来自三水平元分析的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2025, 57(1): 100-124. |
[12] | 姚雨佳, 颜之悦, 林慧慧, 陈静全, 宣雨阳. 人际距离和策略创造性对人际情绪调节的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2025, 57(1): 125-134. |
[13] | 刘新燕, 伍海兰, 涂菊, 王璐. 一心多用的双刃剑效应:多任务对亲社会行为的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(12): 1800-1820. |
[14] | 薛秋, 尹可丽. 同步动作对社会联结的影响: 集体欢腾的中介作用和团队规模的调节作用[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(12): 1821-1835. |
[15] | 王燕, 孙芯芸, 杨茵贝. 如何预测她会拥有“花心”男友?女性童年环境对其伴侣短期择偶策略的预测机制[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(11): 1512-1523. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||