心理学报 ›› 2019, Vol. 51 ›› Issue (8): 890-902.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2019.00890 cstr: 32110.14.2019.00890
收稿日期:
2018-06-04
发布日期:
2019-06-24
出版日期:
2019-08-25
基金资助:
TANG Rixin(), LI Jiajia, WANG Zhipeng
Received:
2018-06-04
Online:
2019-06-24
Published:
2019-08-25
摘要:
研究探讨情绪正-负效价、唤醒度水平(高、中、低)以及有意识评价对推拉动作的影响。分3个实验进行, 收集反应时(RT)和手部动作指标等。实验1使用正-负效价下高、中、低唤醒水平的图片进行评价并执行推-拉动作任务; 实验2使用中性图片和灰色空白图片完成相同任务; 实验3在不进行效价评估情况下完成推-拉动作。结果表明(1)情绪效价对反应时, 运动时间和到达峰速时间等有显著影响:向上拖动图片时, 消极图片拖得更快; 向下拖动时, 则积极图片更快。(2)情绪唤醒度调节情绪效价对推-拉动作的作用:唤醒度越高, 情绪效价尤其是负性情绪对推-拉动作的作用越大; 反之, 则作用越小。(3)无评价条件下, 情绪效价对推-拉动作无显著影响; 即情绪对动作的影响需要有意识评价的参与。这些结果说明情绪对推拉动作影响可能发生在早期, 不仅影响动作计划, 而且影响到随后动作的执行, 而高唤醒度和有意识评价增强了这种影响。
中图分类号:
唐日新, 李佳佳, 王志鹏. (2019). 情绪效价、唤醒度和有无评价影响手部动作. 心理学报, 51(8), 890-902.
TANG Rixin, LI Jiajia, WANG Zhipeng. (2019). Valence, arousal and appraisal of emotion influence the hand movement. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 51(8), 890-902.
图1 实验流程图。图片在分辨率为1920×1080像素的21.5英寸电脑屏幕上呈现和拖动, 请被试按要求站在距电脑屏幕10~15 cm稍接近电脑左半侧的位置, 电脑屏幕中央矩形框会在程序打开后呈现不同效价图片, 图片内靠右侧有一个提示被试放食指(贴marker点)的圆形筐, 屏幕上下两个边缘处各有一个拖放图片的矩形框, 三个矩形框大小一样均为10cm×10cm, 矩形框背景颜色为中性的米黄色。
[1] | Balcetis E., . ( 2015). Approach and avoidance as organizing structures for motivated distance perception. Emotion Review, 8(2), 115-128. |
[2] | Bouman D., Stins J. F., & Beek P. J . ( 2015). Arousal and exposure duration affect forward step initiation. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1667, 1-10. |
[3] | Chapman C.S., & Goodale M.A . ( 2010). Seeing all the obstacles in your way: The effect of visual feedback and visual feedback schedule on obstacle avoidance while reaching. Experimental Brain Research, 202(2), 363-375. |
[4] | Chen M., & Bargh J.A . ( 1999). Consequences of automatic evaluation: Immediate behavioral predispositions to approach or avoid the stimulus. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 25(2), 215-224. |
[5] | Chester D.S., & DeWall C.N . ( 2017). Combating the sting of rejection with the pleasure of revenge: A new look at how emotion shapes aggression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 112(3), 413-430. |
[6] | Ciardo F., Campanini I., Merlo A., Rubichi S., & Iani C . ( 2018). The role of perspective in discriminating between social and non-social intentions from reach-to-grasp kinematics. Psychological Research, 82(5), 915-928. |
[7] | De Houwer J., Crombez G., Baeyens F., & Hermans D . ( 2001). On the generality of the affective Simon effect. Cognition & Emotion, 15(2), 189-206. |
[8] | Dixon M. L., Thiruchselvam R., Todd R., & Christoff K . ( 2017). Emotion and the prefrontal cortex: An integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 143(10), 1033-1081. |
[9] | Duckworth K. L., Bargh J. A., Garcia M., & Chaiken S . ( 2002). The automatic evaluation of novel stimuli. Psychological Science, 13(6), 513-519. |
[10] | Eder A.B., & Rothermund K. , ( 2008). When do motor behaviors (mis)match affective stimuli? An evaluative coding view of approach and avoidance reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137(2), 262-281. |
[11] | Fan N., Xu X., & Zhou F. J . ( 2017). The influence from arousal to affective priming effect of Chinese emotional words. Psychological Exploration. 37(1), 23-28. |
[ 范宁, 许骁, 周芳洁 . ( 2017). 唤醒度对汉语情绪词情绪启动效应的影响. 心理学探新, 37(1), 23-28.] | |
[12] | Fang P., Chen M. Q., & Jiang Y . ( 2006). Experimental paradigms of affective priming. Psychological Science, 29(6), 1396-1399. |
[ 方平, 陈满琪, 姜媛 . ( 2006). 情绪启动研究的实验范式. 心理科学, 29(6), 1396-1399.] | |
[13] | Frijda N. H., Kuipers P., & Ter Schure E . ( 1989). Relations among emotion, appraisal, and emotional action readiness. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 57(2), 212-228. |
[14] | Fu L. S., Yu J. J., Ni S. G., & Li H . ( 2018). Reduced framing effect: Experience adjusts affective forecasting with losses. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 76, 231-38. |
[15] | Goodale, M. A., & Humphrey, G. K . ( 1998). The objects of action and perception. Cognition, 67(1-2), 181-207. |
[16] | Grau J. W., Crown E. D., Ferguson A. R., Washburn S. N., Hook M. A., & Miranda R. C . ( 2006). Instrumental learning within the spinal cord: Underlying mechanisms and implications for recovery after injury. Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews, 5(4), 191-239. |
[17] | Jamieson J. P., Hangen E. J., Lee H. Y., & Yeager D. S . ( 2017). Capitalizing on appraisal processes to improve affective responses to social stress. Emotion Review, 10(1), 30-39. |
[18] | Kappes C., Streubel B., Droste K. L., & Folta-Schoofs K . ( 2017). Linking the positivity effect in attention with affective outcomes: Age group differences and the role of arousal . Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1877. 1-11. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01877. |
[19] | Kret M. E., & Bocanegra, B. R. ., (2016). Editorial: Adaptive hot cognition: How emotion drives information processing and cognition steers affective processing. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1920. 1-4. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01920. |
[20] | Krpan D., & Schnall S. , ( 2014). Too close for comfort: Stimulus valence moderates the influence of motivational orientation on distance perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 107(6), 978-993. |
[21] | Kuppens P., Tuerlinckx F., Russell J. A., & Barrett L. F . ( 2013). The relation between valence and arousal in subjective experience. Psychological Bulletin, 139(4), 917-940. |
[22] | Landau M. J., Meier B. P., & Keefer L. A . ( 2010). A metaphor-enriched social cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 136(6), 1045-1067. |
[23] | Lang P.J., & Bradley M.M . ( 2013). Appetitive and defensive motivation: Goal-directed or goal-determined? Emotion Review, 5(3), 230-234. |
[24] | Li X. D., Ye H. S., & Ding D. Q . ( 2018). Understanding by action: A perspect of embodied social cognition. Psychological Exploration, 38(1), 20-24. |
[ 黎晓丹, 叶浩生, 丁道群 . ( 2018). 通过身体动作理解人与环境:具身的社会认知. 心理学探新, 38(1), 20-24.] | |
[25] | Liu Y., Fu Q. F., Fu X. L . ( 2009). The interaction between cognition and emotion. Chinese Science Bulletin, 54(18), 2783-2796. |
[ 刘烨, 付秋芳, 傅小兰 . ( 2009). 认知与情绪的交互作用. 科学通报, 54(18), 2783-2796.] | |
[26] | Mather M., & Sutherland M.R . ( 2011). Arousal-biased competition in perception and memory. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6(2), 114-133. |
[27] | Moscarello J.M., & LeDoux J.E . ( 2013). The contribution of the amygdala to aversive and appetitive Pavlovian processes. Emotion Review, 5(3), 248-253. |
[28] | Panksepp J., . ( 2013). Cross-species neuroaffective parsing of primal emotional desires and aversions in mammals. Emotion Review, 5(3), 235-240. |
[29] | Pereira M. G., de Oliveira L., Erthal F. S., Joffily M., Mocaiber I. F., & Volchan E., & Pessoa L . ( 2010). Emotion affects action: Midcingulate cortex as a pivotal node of interaction between negative emotion and motor signals. Cognitive Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 10(1), 94-106. |
[30] | Pool E., Brosch T., Delplanque S., & Sander D . ( 2016). Attentional bias for positive emotional stimuli: A meta-analytic investigation. Psychological Bulletin, 142(1), 79-106. |
[31] | Putkinen V., Makkonen T., & Eerola T . ( 2017). Music-induced positive mood broadens the scope of auditory attention. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12(7), 1159-1168. |
[32] | Reimann M., & Bechara A. , ( 2010). The somatic marker framework as a neurological theory of decision-making: Review, conceptual comparisons, and future neuroeconomics research. Journal of Economic Psychology, 31(5), 767-776. |
[33] | Reisenzein R., , ( 2017). The legacy of cognition-arousal theory: Introduction to a special section of emotion review. Emotion Review, 9(1), 3-6. |
[34] | Rolls E.T . ( 2013). What are emotional states, and why do we have them? Emotion Review, 5(3), 241-247. |
[35] | Rotteveel M., & Phaf. RH . ( 2004). Automatic affective evaluation does not automatically predispose for arm flexion and extension. Emotion, 4(2), 156-172. |
[36] | Rougier M., Muller D., Ric F., Alexopoulos T., Batailler C., & Smeding A., & Aubéb B . ( 2018). A new look at sensorimotor aspects in approach/avoidance tendencies: The role of visual whole-body movement information. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 76, 42-53. |
[37] | Russell J.A . ( 1980). A circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39(6), 1161-1178. |
[38] | Saxton B. T., Myhre S. K., Siyaguna T., & Rokke P. D . ( 2018). Do arousal and valence have separable influences on attention across time? Psychological Research, 1-17. |
[39] | Siemer M., Mauss I., & Gross J. J . ( 2007). Same situation-different emotions: How appraisals shape our emotions. Emotion, 7(3), 592-600. |
[40] | Solarz A.K . ( 1960). Latency of instrumental responses as a function of compatibility with the meaning of eliciting verbal signs. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59(4), 239-245. |
[41] | Tang R. X., Whitwell R. L., & Goodale M. A . ( 2015). The influence of visual feedback from the recent past on the programming of grip aperture is grasp-specific, shared between hands, and mediated by sensorimotor memory not task set. Cognition, 138, 49-63. |
[42] | Tang R. X., Whitwell R. L., & Goodale M. A . ( 2016). Unusual hand postures but not familiar tools show motor equivalence with precision grasping. Cognition, 151, 28-36. |
[43] | Veling H., & Aarts H. , ( 2011). Unintentional preparation of motor impulses after incidental perception of need-rewarding objects. Cognition & Emotion, 25(6), 1131-1138. |
[44] | Wang H.L., Mo L. ., ( 2017). "Action-evaluation" effect: The interaction between body movement and cognitive activity. Studies of Psychology &Behavior . 15(5), 626-632. |
[ 王汉林, 莫雷 . ( 2017). “动作-评价” 效应: 动作与认知的交互作用. 心理与行为研究, 15(5), 626-632.] | |
[45] | Wang R. Y., Fan N., & University H . ( 2016). An explanation about reversal effect of affective priming in Chinese character: The interaction between frequency and task paradigm. Journal of Psychological Science. 39(3), 559-565. |
[ 王若茵, 范宁 . ( 2016). 任务与字频对汉字情绪启动反转效应的影响. 心理科学, 39(3), 559-565.] | |
[46] | Whitwell R. L., Lambert L. M., & Goodale M. A . ( 2008). Grasping future events: Explicit knowledge of the availability of visual feedback fails to reliably influence prehension. Experimental Brain Research, 188(4), 603-611. |
[47] | Xu H. F., Zhang Q., & Guo C. Y . ( 2015). Different effects of valence and arousal on familiarity and recollection. Journal of Psychological Science, 38(2), 263-269 |
[ 徐慧芳, 张钦, 郭春彦 . ( 2015). 情绪效价和唤醒对熟悉性和回想的不同影响. 心理科学, 38(2), 263-269] | |
[48] | Yan L., Zuo B., & Wu Y . ( 2016). Affective coherence and its influence on cognitive processing . Journal of Psychological Science. 39(6), 1379-1384. |
[ 严磊, 佐斌, 吴漾 ( 2016). 情感一致性及其对认知加工的影响. 心理科学, 39(6), 1379-1384.] | |
[49] | Ye H. S . ( 2010). Embodied cognition: A new approach in cognitive psychology. Advance in Psychological Science, 18(5), 705-710. |
[ 叶浩生 . ( 2010). 具身认知:认知心理学的新取向. 心理科学进展, 18(5), 705-710] | |
[50] | Zhang Y. M., Proctor R. W., & Wegener D. T . ( 2012). Approach-avoidance actions or categorization? A matching account of reference valence effects in affective S-R compatibility. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(3), 609-616. |
[51] | Zhao S.C., & Zheng X.F, . ( 2017). The behavior inhibition characteristics of different arousal negative stimulation. Psychological Research. 10(1), 17-23. |
[ 赵绍晨, 郑希付 . ( 2017). 不同唤醒度负性刺激抑制控制特点. 心理研究, 10(1), 17-23.] |
[1] | 冯文婷, 薛舒允, 汪涛. 拟人的品牌更环保?拟人化沟通对促进绿色消费倾向的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2025, 57(4): 720-738. |
[2] | 张环, 秦锡权, 刘雨, 林琳, 吴捷. 不同强度趋近动机积极情绪对基于语义关联性错误记忆的影响及其神经机制[J]. 心理学报, 2025, 57(3): 349-362. |
[3] | 夏莲香, 刘凯歌, 李新宇, 叶群. 编码方式与叙事连贯性调节情绪对时间顺序记忆的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2025, 57(1): 1-17. |
[4] | 姚雨佳, 颜之悦, 林慧慧, 陈静全, 宣雨阳. 人际距离和策略创造性对人际情绪调节的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2025, 57(1): 125-134. |
[5] | 陈斯允, 熊继伟, 彭凯平. “羊毛薅尽口味乏”:节俭心态如何影响食物预期享受与体验评估[J]. 心理学报, 2025, 57(1): 152-172. |
[6] | 张环, 王晨, 李俊霞, 林琳, 吴捷. 情绪效价和动机强度对社会分享型提取诱发遗忘的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(8): 999-1014. |
[7] | 王丹, 付雨佳, 陈文锋. 社会情境对情绪感染的影响:一项基于EMG的超扫描研究[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(8): 1047-1060. |
[8] | 张明杨, 杨盈, 包寒吴霜, 蔡华俭. 中国人的积极理想情绪:近几十年来的变迁[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(7): 847-858. |
[9] | 黄顺森, 来枭雄, 张彩, 赵心媚, 代欣然, 祁梦迪, 王欢蕾, 王文荣, 王耘. 青少年手机压力与心理健康的关系:基于多元宇宙样分析和密集追踪方法[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(6): 745-758. |
[10] | 孟现鑫, 罗怡, 韩晨媛, 吴国伟, 常姣, 袁加锦, 千坤, 傅小兰. 社会接纳调节社会排斥对情绪冲突适应的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(5): 577-593. |
[11] | 童廷婷, 白幼玲, 冯廷勇. 情绪调节改善拖延行为的认知机制:任务厌恶中介作用[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(4): 458-468. |
[12] | 王伟晗, 曹斐臻, 余林伟, 曾珂, 杨鑫超, 徐强. 群体信息对面部表情识别的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(3): 268-280. |
[13] | 尹华站, 肖春花, 夏安妮, 袁中静, 崔晓冰, 李丹. 基本情绪对时距知觉的影响: 来自三水平元分析和网络元分析的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(12): 1676-1690. |
[14] | 张瑾, 许销冰, 庄晓涵. 产品包装的医药符号线索对产品偏好的影响: 感知有效性和规避情绪的中介作用[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(12): 1836-1850. |
[15] | 肖程元, 赵世瑞, 袁加锦. 积极认知重评对负性信息传播的调控及多维证据[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(11): 1471-1487. |
阅读次数 | ||||||||||||||||||||||
全文 3286
|
|
|||||||||||||||||||||
摘要 |
|
|||||||||||||||||||||