心理学报 ›› 2019, Vol. 51 ›› Issue (8): 890-902.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2019.00890
收稿日期:
2018-06-04
发布日期:
2019-06-24
出版日期:
2019-08-25
通讯作者:
唐日新
E-mail:trxtrx518@nju.edu.cn
基金资助:
TANG Rixin(), LI Jiajia, WANG Zhipeng
Received:
2018-06-04
Online:
2019-06-24
Published:
2019-08-25
Contact:
TANG Rixin
E-mail:trxtrx518@nju.edu.cn
摘要:
研究探讨情绪正-负效价、唤醒度水平(高、中、低)以及有意识评价对推拉动作的影响。分3个实验进行, 收集反应时(RT)和手部动作指标等。实验1使用正-负效价下高、中、低唤醒水平的图片进行评价并执行推-拉动作任务; 实验2使用中性图片和灰色空白图片完成相同任务; 实验3在不进行效价评估情况下完成推-拉动作。结果表明(1)情绪效价对反应时, 运动时间和到达峰速时间等有显著影响:向上拖动图片时, 消极图片拖得更快; 向下拖动时, 则积极图片更快。(2)情绪唤醒度调节情绪效价对推-拉动作的作用:唤醒度越高, 情绪效价尤其是负性情绪对推-拉动作的作用越大; 反之, 则作用越小。(3)无评价条件下, 情绪效价对推-拉动作无显著影响; 即情绪对动作的影响需要有意识评价的参与。这些结果说明情绪对推拉动作影响可能发生在早期, 不仅影响动作计划, 而且影响到随后动作的执行, 而高唤醒度和有意识评价增强了这种影响。
中图分类号:
唐日新, 李佳佳, 王志鹏. (2019). 情绪效价、唤醒度和有无评价影响手部动作. 心理学报, 51(8), 890-902.
TANG Rixin, LI Jiajia, WANG Zhipeng. (2019). Valence, arousal and appraisal of emotion influence the hand movement. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 51(8), 890-902.
图1 实验流程图。图片在分辨率为1920×1080像素的21.5英寸电脑屏幕上呈现和拖动, 请被试按要求站在距电脑屏幕10~15 cm稍接近电脑左半侧的位置, 电脑屏幕中央矩形框会在程序打开后呈现不同效价图片, 图片内靠右侧有一个提示被试放食指(贴marker点)的圆形筐, 屏幕上下两个边缘处各有一个拖放图片的矩形框, 三个矩形框大小一样均为10cm×10cm, 矩形框背景颜色为中性的米黄色。
[1] | Balcetis E., . ( 2015). Approach and avoidance as organizing structures for motivated distance perception. Emotion Review, 8(2), 115-128. |
[2] | Bouman D., Stins J. F., & Beek P. J . ( 2015). Arousal and exposure duration affect forward step initiation. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1667, 1-10. |
[3] | Chapman C.S., & Goodale M.A . ( 2010). Seeing all the obstacles in your way: The effect of visual feedback and visual feedback schedule on obstacle avoidance while reaching. Experimental Brain Research, 202(2), 363-375. |
[4] | Chen M., & Bargh J.A . ( 1999). Consequences of automatic evaluation: Immediate behavioral predispositions to approach or avoid the stimulus. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 25(2), 215-224. |
[5] | Chester D.S., & DeWall C.N . ( 2017). Combating the sting of rejection with the pleasure of revenge: A new look at how emotion shapes aggression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 112(3), 413-430. |
[6] | Ciardo F., Campanini I., Merlo A., Rubichi S., & Iani C . ( 2018). The role of perspective in discriminating between social and non-social intentions from reach-to-grasp kinematics. Psychological Research, 82(5), 915-928. |
[7] | De Houwer J., Crombez G., Baeyens F., & Hermans D . ( 2001). On the generality of the affective Simon effect. Cognition & Emotion, 15(2), 189-206. |
[8] | Dixon M. L., Thiruchselvam R., Todd R., & Christoff K . ( 2017). Emotion and the prefrontal cortex: An integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 143(10), 1033-1081. |
[9] | Duckworth K. L., Bargh J. A., Garcia M., & Chaiken S . ( 2002). The automatic evaluation of novel stimuli. Psychological Science, 13(6), 513-519. |
[10] | Eder A.B., & Rothermund K. , ( 2008). When do motor behaviors (mis)match affective stimuli? An evaluative coding view of approach and avoidance reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137(2), 262-281. |
[11] | Fan N., Xu X., & Zhou F. J . ( 2017). The influence from arousal to affective priming effect of Chinese emotional words. Psychological Exploration. 37(1), 23-28. |
[ 范宁, 许骁, 周芳洁 . ( 2017). 唤醒度对汉语情绪词情绪启动效应的影响. 心理学探新, 37(1), 23-28.] | |
[12] | Fang P., Chen M. Q., & Jiang Y . ( 2006). Experimental paradigms of affective priming. Psychological Science, 29(6), 1396-1399. |
[ 方平, 陈满琪, 姜媛 . ( 2006). 情绪启动研究的实验范式. 心理科学, 29(6), 1396-1399.] | |
[13] | Frijda N. H., Kuipers P., & Ter Schure E . ( 1989). Relations among emotion, appraisal, and emotional action readiness. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 57(2), 212-228. |
[14] | Fu L. S., Yu J. J., Ni S. G., & Li H . ( 2018). Reduced framing effect: Experience adjusts affective forecasting with losses. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 76, 231-38. |
[15] | Goodale, M. A., & Humphrey, G. K . ( 1998). The objects of action and perception. Cognition, 67(1-2), 181-207. |
[16] | Grau J. W., Crown E. D., Ferguson A. R., Washburn S. N., Hook M. A., & Miranda R. C . ( 2006). Instrumental learning within the spinal cord: Underlying mechanisms and implications for recovery after injury. Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews, 5(4), 191-239. |
[17] | Jamieson J. P., Hangen E. J., Lee H. Y., & Yeager D. S . ( 2017). Capitalizing on appraisal processes to improve affective responses to social stress. Emotion Review, 10(1), 30-39. |
[18] | Kappes C., Streubel B., Droste K. L., & Folta-Schoofs K . ( 2017). Linking the positivity effect in attention with affective outcomes: Age group differences and the role of arousal . Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1877. 1-11. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01877. |
[19] | Kret M. E., & Bocanegra, B. R. ., (2016). Editorial: Adaptive hot cognition: How emotion drives information processing and cognition steers affective processing. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1920. 1-4. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01920. |
[20] | Krpan D., & Schnall S. , ( 2014). Too close for comfort: Stimulus valence moderates the influence of motivational orientation on distance perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 107(6), 978-993. |
[21] | Kuppens P., Tuerlinckx F., Russell J. A., & Barrett L. F . ( 2013). The relation between valence and arousal in subjective experience. Psychological Bulletin, 139(4), 917-940. |
[22] | Landau M. J., Meier B. P., & Keefer L. A . ( 2010). A metaphor-enriched social cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 136(6), 1045-1067. |
[23] | Lang P.J., & Bradley M.M . ( 2013). Appetitive and defensive motivation: Goal-directed or goal-determined? Emotion Review, 5(3), 230-234. |
[24] | Li X. D., Ye H. S., & Ding D. Q . ( 2018). Understanding by action: A perspect of embodied social cognition. Psychological Exploration, 38(1), 20-24. |
[ 黎晓丹, 叶浩生, 丁道群 . ( 2018). 通过身体动作理解人与环境:具身的社会认知. 心理学探新, 38(1), 20-24.] | |
[25] | Liu Y., Fu Q. F., Fu X. L . ( 2009). The interaction between cognition and emotion. Chinese Science Bulletin, 54(18), 2783-2796. |
[ 刘烨, 付秋芳, 傅小兰 . ( 2009). 认知与情绪的交互作用. 科学通报, 54(18), 2783-2796.] | |
[26] | Mather M., & Sutherland M.R . ( 2011). Arousal-biased competition in perception and memory. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6(2), 114-133. |
[27] | Moscarello J.M., & LeDoux J.E . ( 2013). The contribution of the amygdala to aversive and appetitive Pavlovian processes. Emotion Review, 5(3), 248-253. |
[28] | Panksepp J., . ( 2013). Cross-species neuroaffective parsing of primal emotional desires and aversions in mammals. Emotion Review, 5(3), 235-240. |
[29] | Pereira M. G., de Oliveira L., Erthal F. S., Joffily M., Mocaiber I. F., & Volchan E., & Pessoa L . ( 2010). Emotion affects action: Midcingulate cortex as a pivotal node of interaction between negative emotion and motor signals. Cognitive Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 10(1), 94-106. |
[30] | Pool E., Brosch T., Delplanque S., & Sander D . ( 2016). Attentional bias for positive emotional stimuli: A meta-analytic investigation. Psychological Bulletin, 142(1), 79-106. |
[31] | Putkinen V., Makkonen T., & Eerola T . ( 2017). Music-induced positive mood broadens the scope of auditory attention. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12(7), 1159-1168. |
[32] | Reimann M., & Bechara A. , ( 2010). The somatic marker framework as a neurological theory of decision-making: Review, conceptual comparisons, and future neuroeconomics research. Journal of Economic Psychology, 31(5), 767-776. |
[33] | Reisenzein R., , ( 2017). The legacy of cognition-arousal theory: Introduction to a special section of emotion review. Emotion Review, 9(1), 3-6. |
[34] | Rolls E.T . ( 2013). What are emotional states, and why do we have them? Emotion Review, 5(3), 241-247. |
[35] | Rotteveel M., & Phaf. RH . ( 2004). Automatic affective evaluation does not automatically predispose for arm flexion and extension. Emotion, 4(2), 156-172. |
[36] | Rougier M., Muller D., Ric F., Alexopoulos T., Batailler C., & Smeding A., & Aubéb B . ( 2018). A new look at sensorimotor aspects in approach/avoidance tendencies: The role of visual whole-body movement information. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 76, 42-53. |
[37] | Russell J.A . ( 1980). A circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39(6), 1161-1178. |
[38] | Saxton B. T., Myhre S. K., Siyaguna T., & Rokke P. D . ( 2018). Do arousal and valence have separable influences on attention across time? Psychological Research, 1-17. |
[39] | Siemer M., Mauss I., & Gross J. J . ( 2007). Same situation-different emotions: How appraisals shape our emotions. Emotion, 7(3), 592-600. |
[40] | Solarz A.K . ( 1960). Latency of instrumental responses as a function of compatibility with the meaning of eliciting verbal signs. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59(4), 239-245. |
[41] | Tang R. X., Whitwell R. L., & Goodale M. A . ( 2015). The influence of visual feedback from the recent past on the programming of grip aperture is grasp-specific, shared between hands, and mediated by sensorimotor memory not task set. Cognition, 138, 49-63. |
[42] | Tang R. X., Whitwell R. L., & Goodale M. A . ( 2016). Unusual hand postures but not familiar tools show motor equivalence with precision grasping. Cognition, 151, 28-36. |
[43] | Veling H., & Aarts H. , ( 2011). Unintentional preparation of motor impulses after incidental perception of need-rewarding objects. Cognition & Emotion, 25(6), 1131-1138. |
[44] | Wang H.L., Mo L. ., ( 2017). "Action-evaluation" effect: The interaction between body movement and cognitive activity. Studies of Psychology &Behavior . 15(5), 626-632. |
[ 王汉林, 莫雷 . ( 2017). “动作-评价” 效应: 动作与认知的交互作用. 心理与行为研究, 15(5), 626-632.] | |
[45] | Wang R. Y., Fan N., & University H . ( 2016). An explanation about reversal effect of affective priming in Chinese character: The interaction between frequency and task paradigm. Journal of Psychological Science. 39(3), 559-565. |
[ 王若茵, 范宁 . ( 2016). 任务与字频对汉字情绪启动反转效应的影响. 心理科学, 39(3), 559-565.] | |
[46] | Whitwell R. L., Lambert L. M., & Goodale M. A . ( 2008). Grasping future events: Explicit knowledge of the availability of visual feedback fails to reliably influence prehension. Experimental Brain Research, 188(4), 603-611. |
[47] | Xu H. F., Zhang Q., & Guo C. Y . ( 2015). Different effects of valence and arousal on familiarity and recollection. Journal of Psychological Science, 38(2), 263-269 |
[ 徐慧芳, 张钦, 郭春彦 . ( 2015). 情绪效价和唤醒对熟悉性和回想的不同影响. 心理科学, 38(2), 263-269] | |
[48] | Yan L., Zuo B., & Wu Y . ( 2016). Affective coherence and its influence on cognitive processing . Journal of Psychological Science. 39(6), 1379-1384. |
[ 严磊, 佐斌, 吴漾 ( 2016). 情感一致性及其对认知加工的影响. 心理科学, 39(6), 1379-1384.] | |
[49] | Ye H. S . ( 2010). Embodied cognition: A new approach in cognitive psychology. Advance in Psychological Science, 18(5), 705-710. |
[ 叶浩生 . ( 2010). 具身认知:认知心理学的新取向. 心理科学进展, 18(5), 705-710] | |
[50] | Zhang Y. M., Proctor R. W., & Wegener D. T . ( 2012). Approach-avoidance actions or categorization? A matching account of reference valence effects in affective S-R compatibility. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(3), 609-616. |
[51] | Zhao S.C., & Zheng X.F, . ( 2017). The behavior inhibition characteristics of different arousal negative stimulation. Psychological Research. 10(1), 17-23. |
[ 赵绍晨, 郑希付 . ( 2017). 不同唤醒度负性刺激抑制控制特点. 心理研究, 10(1), 17-23.] |
[1] | 陈沛琪, 张银玲, 胡馨木, 王静, 买晓琴. 10~12岁儿童社会价值取向对第三方利他行为的影响:情绪的作用[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(8): 1255-1269. |
[2] | 梅颖, 刘郡彤, 刘红红, 傅洋, 罗熙, 雷怡. 自悯写作对恐惧消退的促进作用[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(8): 1317-1329. |
[3] | 王妹, 程思, 李宜伟, 李红, 张丹丹. 背外侧前额叶在安慰剂效应中的作用:社会情绪调节研究[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(7): 1063-1073. |
[4] | 祝养浩, 龙立荣, 刘文兴. 领导感激表达能提高员工的追随行为吗?情绪表达真诚性的作用[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(7): 1160-1175. |
[5] | 郭晓栋, 郑泓, 阮盾, 胡丁鼎, 王毅, 王艳郁, 陈楚侨. 认知和情感共情与负性情绪:情绪调节的作用机制[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(6): 892-904. |
[6] | 谢慧, 林轩怡, 胡婉柔, 胡晓晴. 情绪调节促进负性社会反馈的遗忘:来自行为和脑电的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(6): 905-919. |
[7] | 张丹丹, 李宜伟, 于文汶, 莫李澄, 彭程, 刘黎黎. 0~1岁婴儿情绪偏向的发展:近红外成像研究[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(6): 920-929. |
[8] | 肖家乐, 申子姣, 李晓燕, 林丹华. 同伴侵害变化轨迹及其与抑郁情绪和外化问题的关系:共同增强还是风险易感[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(6): 978-993. |
[9] | 李丽源, 高祥宇, 郑晓明. 员工积极主动行为的组态效应:基于过程的视角[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(5): 792-811. |
[10] | 宋琪, 张璐, 高莉芳, 程豹, 陈扬. “行高人非”还是“见贤思齐”?职场上行比较对员工行为的双刃剑效应[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(4): 658-670. |
[11] | 郑茜, 张亭亭, 李量, 范宁, 杨志刚. 言语的情绪韵律和情绪语义对听觉去信息掩蔽的作用[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(2): 177-191. |
[12] | 王小琴, 谈雅菲, 蒙杰, 刘源, 位东涛, 杨文静, 邱江. 情绪调节灵活性对负性情绪的影响:来自经验取样的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(2): 192-209. |
[13] | 高可翔, 张岳瑶, 李思瑾, 袁加锦, 李红, 张丹丹. 腹内侧前额叶在内隐认知重评中的因果作用[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(2): 210-223. |
[14] | 黄成利, 胡超. 死亡意识的恐惧管理和悲伤管理[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(2): 318-335. |
[15] | 尹华站, 张丽, 刘鹏玉, 李丹. 负性情绪的动机维度对时距知觉的影响:注意控制和注意偏向的中介作用[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(12): 1917-1931. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||