心理学报 ›› 2018, Vol. 50 ›› Issue (12): 1356-1368.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2018.01356
高虹1, 李杨卓1, 胡蝶1, 朱敏2,3, 高湘萍1, 胡天翊1()
收稿日期:
2017-10-17
发布日期:
2018-10-30
出版日期:
2018-11-30
GAO Hong1, LI Yangzhuo1, HU Die1, ZHU Min2,3, GAO Xiangping1, HU Tianyi1()
Received:
2017-10-17
Online:
2018-10-30
Published:
2018-11-30
摘要:
研究采用注意网络测验任务(attention network test, ANT), 分别检验自我相关信息在注意的警觉、定向和执行控制网络上的加工效率差异, 以揭示自我信息识别优势的注意机制。实验1考察了面孔类型(自我面孔或他人面孔)在3种注意网络下的加工效率差异; 实验2采用自我联结学习范式对实验1进行验证; 实验3将颜色类型(红色或绿色)作为目标刺激, 面孔类型作为背景, 以考察任务无关自我信息是否对注意网络加工效率有影响。研究结果发现, 当自我信息为目标时, 个体在注意定向网络上存在加工效率优势, 而警觉和执行控制网络上没有加工效率差异。当自我相关信息与任务无关时, 在注意的警觉、定向和执行控制网络中均未表现出加工效率优势。说明了自我信息的注意优势发生在注意定向网络上, 且受任务优先性影响。
中图分类号:
高虹, 李杨卓, 胡蝶, 朱敏, 高湘萍, 胡天翊. (2018). 自我信息识别优势——来自注意定向网络的证据. 心理学报, 50(12), 1356-1368.
GAO Hong, LI Yangzhuo, HU Die, ZHU Min, GAO Xiangping, HU Tianyi. (2018). Cognitive advantage for self-information: Evidence from the orienting network of attention. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 50(12), 1356-1368.
侧翼类型 | 线索类型 | 自我面孔 | 朋友面孔 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
反应时 | 正确率 | 反应时 | 正确率 | ||
侧翼一致 | 无线索 | 684 ± 59 | 97.32 ± 3.96 | 714 ± 65 | 97.32 ± 6.02 |
双重线索 | 636 ± 60 | 97.62 ± 4.45 | 677 ± 63 | 98.21 ± 4.16 | |
中央线索 | 640 ± 56 | 96.13 ± 5.77 | 683 ± 59 | 98.51 ± 3.25 | |
空间线索 | 587 ± 62 | 97.92 ± 4.32 | 633 ± 61 | 97.62 ± 4.45 | |
平均值 | 636 ± 53 | 97.25 ± 2.60 | 676 ± 54 | 97.92 ± 2.78 | |
侧翼冲突 | 无线索 | 698 ± 58 | 91.13 ± 6.20 | 723 ± 45 | 97.02 ± 5.18 |
双重线索 | 654 ± 65 | 97.02 ± 4.66 | 686 ± 60 | 97.62 ± 4.45 | |
中央线索 | 670 ± 63 | 94.94 ± 7.64 | 698 ± 65 | 95.83 ± 6.99 | |
空间线索 | 596 ± 73 | 98.21 ± 4.12 | 653 ± 65 | 98.21 ± 4.16 | |
平均值 | 654 ± 56 | 96.80 ± 3.08 | 690 ± 51 | 97.17 ± 3.22 |
表1 实验1各条件下被试识别面孔的反应时(ms)和正确率(%) (M ± SD)
侧翼类型 | 线索类型 | 自我面孔 | 朋友面孔 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
反应时 | 正确率 | 反应时 | 正确率 | ||
侧翼一致 | 无线索 | 684 ± 59 | 97.32 ± 3.96 | 714 ± 65 | 97.32 ± 6.02 |
双重线索 | 636 ± 60 | 97.62 ± 4.45 | 677 ± 63 | 98.21 ± 4.16 | |
中央线索 | 640 ± 56 | 96.13 ± 5.77 | 683 ± 59 | 98.51 ± 3.25 | |
空间线索 | 587 ± 62 | 97.92 ± 4.32 | 633 ± 61 | 97.62 ± 4.45 | |
平均值 | 636 ± 53 | 97.25 ± 2.60 | 676 ± 54 | 97.92 ± 2.78 | |
侧翼冲突 | 无线索 | 698 ± 58 | 91.13 ± 6.20 | 723 ± 45 | 97.02 ± 5.18 |
双重线索 | 654 ± 65 | 97.02 ± 4.66 | 686 ± 60 | 97.62 ± 4.45 | |
中央线索 | 670 ± 63 | 94.94 ± 7.64 | 698 ± 65 | 95.83 ± 6.99 | |
空间线索 | 596 ± 73 | 98.21 ± 4.12 | 653 ± 65 | 98.21 ± 4.16 | |
平均值 | 654 ± 56 | 96.80 ± 3.08 | 690 ± 51 | 97.17 ± 3.22 |
侧翼类型 | 线索类型 | 自我图形 | 朋友图形 | 无社会意义图形 | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
反应时 | 正确率 | 反应时 | 正确率 | 反应时 | 正确率 | ||
侧翼一致 | 无线索 | 613 ± 64 | 98.33 ± 3.39 | 627 ± 58 | 97.50 ± 4.97 | 647 ± 80 | 97.22 ± 5.03 |
双重线索 | 553 ± 64 | 98.06 ± 3.58 | 562 ± 62 | 97.22 ± 4.55 | 581 ± 76 | 96.39 ± 5.66 | |
中央线索 | 563 ± 62 | 96.39 ± 4.20 | 566 ± 67 | 96.67 ± 4.69 | 570 ± 76 | 97.78 ± 6.16 | |
空间线索 | 526 ± 62 | 96.67 ± 6.03 | 549 ± 62 | 97.50 ± 4.46 | 572 ± 81 | 96.95 ± 4.63 | |
平均值 | 564 ± 58 | 97.36 ± 3.32 | 576 ± 59 | 97.22 ± 3.30 | 593 ± 71 | 97.08 ± 4.53 | |
侧翼冲突 | 无线索 | 635 ± 71 | 96.11 ± 6.09 | 634 ± 58 | 98.06 ± 4.20 | 649 ± 68 | 98.06 ± 3.58 |
双重线索 | 574 ± 62 | 98.33 ± 3.39 | 587 ± 68 | 96.94 ± 5.99 | 602 ± 68 | 98.61 ± 3.84 | |
中央线索 | 587 ± 54 | 98.06 ± 4.20 | 584 ± 56 | 97.22 ± 5.93 | 608 ± 72 | 97.78 ± 4.86 | |
空间线索 | 550 ± 59 | 97.78 ± 4.86 | 575 ± 61 | 98.61 ± 3.84 | 585 ± 68 | 97.22 ± 5.05 | |
平均值 | 586 ± 57 | 97.57 ± 3.33 | 595 ± 56 | 97.71 ± 3.60 | 611 ± 65 | 97.92 ± 3.37 |
表2 加工不同身份图形的反应时(ms)和正确率(%) (M ± SD)
侧翼类型 | 线索类型 | 自我图形 | 朋友图形 | 无社会意义图形 | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
反应时 | 正确率 | 反应时 | 正确率 | 反应时 | 正确率 | ||
侧翼一致 | 无线索 | 613 ± 64 | 98.33 ± 3.39 | 627 ± 58 | 97.50 ± 4.97 | 647 ± 80 | 97.22 ± 5.03 |
双重线索 | 553 ± 64 | 98.06 ± 3.58 | 562 ± 62 | 97.22 ± 4.55 | 581 ± 76 | 96.39 ± 5.66 | |
中央线索 | 563 ± 62 | 96.39 ± 4.20 | 566 ± 67 | 96.67 ± 4.69 | 570 ± 76 | 97.78 ± 6.16 | |
空间线索 | 526 ± 62 | 96.67 ± 6.03 | 549 ± 62 | 97.50 ± 4.46 | 572 ± 81 | 96.95 ± 4.63 | |
平均值 | 564 ± 58 | 97.36 ± 3.32 | 576 ± 59 | 97.22 ± 3.30 | 593 ± 71 | 97.08 ± 4.53 | |
侧翼冲突 | 无线索 | 635 ± 71 | 96.11 ± 6.09 | 634 ± 58 | 98.06 ± 4.20 | 649 ± 68 | 98.06 ± 3.58 |
双重线索 | 574 ± 62 | 98.33 ± 3.39 | 587 ± 68 | 96.94 ± 5.99 | 602 ± 68 | 98.61 ± 3.84 | |
中央线索 | 587 ± 54 | 98.06 ± 4.20 | 584 ± 56 | 97.22 ± 5.93 | 608 ± 72 | 97.78 ± 4.86 | |
空间线索 | 550 ± 59 | 97.78 ± 4.86 | 575 ± 61 | 98.61 ± 3.84 | 585 ± 68 | 97.22 ± 5.05 | |
平均值 | 586 ± 57 | 97.57 ± 3.33 | 595 ± 56 | 97.71 ± 3.60 | 611 ± 65 | 97.92 ± 3.37 |
侧翼类型 | 线索类型 | 红色 | 绿色 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
反应时 | 正确率 | 反应时 | 正确率 | ||
侧翼一致 | 无线索 | 664 ± 114 | 97.92 ± 3.68 | 674 ± 116 | 98.26 ± 5.48 |
双重线索 | 683 ± 104 | 98.61 ± 3.17 | 693 ± 116 | 98.61 ± 4.01 | |
中央线索 | 632 ± 117 | 96.88 ± 5.39 | 643 ± 123 | 98.96 ± 2.81 | |
空间线索 | 695 ± 114 | 98.96 ± 2.81 | 702 ± 110 | 98.61 ±3.17 | |
平均值 | 678 ± 111 | 97.66 ± 2.07 | 669 ± 105 | 98.35 ± 2.30 | |
侧翼冲突 | 无线索 | 656 ± 119 | 96.53 ± 6.46 | 678 ± 117 | 97.92 ± 4.43 |
双重线索 | 614 ± 123 | 97.57 ± 4.58 | 628 ± 142 | 98.61 ± 3.17 | |
中央线索 | 640 ± 122 | 96.18 ± 5.48 | 637 ± 125 | 97.22 ± 5.31 | |
空间线索 | 630 ± 116 | 98.96 ± 2.81 | 650 ± 133 | 98.96 ± 2.81 | |
平均值 | 648 ± 124 | 97.31 ± 2.34 | 635 ± 116 | 97.74 ± 2.67 |
表3 实验3各条件下被试识别颜色的反应时(ms)和正确率(%) (M ± SD)
侧翼类型 | 线索类型 | 红色 | 绿色 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
反应时 | 正确率 | 反应时 | 正确率 | ||
侧翼一致 | 无线索 | 664 ± 114 | 97.92 ± 3.68 | 674 ± 116 | 98.26 ± 5.48 |
双重线索 | 683 ± 104 | 98.61 ± 3.17 | 693 ± 116 | 98.61 ± 4.01 | |
中央线索 | 632 ± 117 | 96.88 ± 5.39 | 643 ± 123 | 98.96 ± 2.81 | |
空间线索 | 695 ± 114 | 98.96 ± 2.81 | 702 ± 110 | 98.61 ±3.17 | |
平均值 | 678 ± 111 | 97.66 ± 2.07 | 669 ± 105 | 98.35 ± 2.30 | |
侧翼冲突 | 无线索 | 656 ± 119 | 96.53 ± 6.46 | 678 ± 117 | 97.92 ± 4.43 |
双重线索 | 614 ± 123 | 97.57 ± 4.58 | 628 ± 142 | 98.61 ± 3.17 | |
中央线索 | 640 ± 122 | 96.18 ± 5.48 | 637 ± 125 | 97.22 ± 5.31 | |
空间线索 | 630 ± 116 | 98.96 ± 2.81 | 650 ± 133 | 98.96 ± 2.81 | |
平均值 | 648 ± 124 | 97.31 ± 2.34 | 635 ± 116 | 97.74 ± 2.67 |
1 |
Callejas A., Lupiàñez J., Funes M. J., & Tudela P . ( 2005). Modulations among the alerting, orienting and executive control networks. Experimental Brain Research, 167( 1), 27-37.
doi: 10.1007/s00221-005-2365-z URL pmid: 16021429 |
2 | Corbetta M., &Shulman , G. L . ( 2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3( 3), 201-215. |
3 |
Corbetta M., Patel G., & Shulman G. L . ( 2008). The reorienting system of the human brain: From environment to theory of mind. Neuron, 58( 3), 306-324.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.04.017 URL pmid: 18466742 |
4 |
Crottaz-Herbette S., &Menon V. , ( 2006). Where and when the anterior cingulate cortex modulates attentional response: Combined fMRI and ERP evidence. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18( 5), 766-780.
doi: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.5.766 URL pmid: 16768376 |
5 |
Devue C., &Brédart S. , ( 2008). Attention to self-referential stimuli: Can I ignore my own face? Acta Psychologica, 128( 2), 290-297.
doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2008.02.004 URL pmid: 18413272 |
6 | Devue C., Laloyaux C., Feyers D., Theeuwes J., & Brédart S . ( 2009). Do pictures of faces, and which ones, capture attention in the inattentional-blindness paradigm? Perception, 38( 4), 552-568. |
7 |
Devue C., Van der Stigchel S., Brédart S., & Theeuwes J . ( 2009). You do not find your own face faster; you just look at it longer. Cognition, 111( 1), 114-122.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.01.003 URL pmid: 19230872 |
8 | Engle R.W., &Kane , M. J . ( 2003). Executive attention, working memory capacity, and a two-factor theory of cognitive control. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 44, 145-199. |
9 |
Fan J., McCandliss B. D., Fossella J., Flombaum J. I., & Posner M. I . ( 2005). The activation of attentional networks. Neuroimage, 26( 2), 471-479.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.02.004 URL pmid: 15907304 |
10 |
Fan J., McCandliss B. D., Sommer T., Raz A., & Posner M. I . ( 2002). Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14( 3), 340-347.
doi: 10.1162/089892902317361886 URL pmid: 11970796 |
11 |
Farrant K., &Uddin , L. Q . ( 2015). Asymmetric development of dorsal and ventral attention networks in the human brain. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 165-174.
doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2015.02.001 URL pmid: 4396619 |
12 |
Federico F., Marotta A., Adriani T., Maccari L., & Casagrande M . ( 2013). Attention network test — The impact of social information on executive control, alerting and orienting. Acta Psychologica, 143( 1), 65-70.
doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.02.006 URL pmid: 23542806 |
13 |
Gao W., Gilmore J. H., Shen D., Smith J. K., Zhu H., & Lin W . ( 2013). The synchronization within and interaction between the default and dorsal attention networks in early infancy. Cerebral Cortex, 23( 3), 594-603.
doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs043 URL pmid: 3563337 |
14 |
Garza J. P., Strom M. J., Wright C. E., Roberts R. J., & Reed C. L . ( 2013). Top-down influences mediate hand bias in spatial attention. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 75( 5), 819-823.
doi: 10.3758/s13414-013-0480-7 URL pmid: 23722884 |
15 |
Hackley S.A., &Valle-Inclán F. , ( 1999). Accessory stimulus effects on response selection: Does arousal speed decision making? Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11( 3), 321-329.
doi: 10.1162/089892999563427 URL pmid: 10402259 |
16 |
Hackley S.A., &Valle-Inclán F. , ( 2003). Which stages of processing are speeded by a warning signal? Biological Psychology, 64( 1-2), 27-45.
doi: 10.1016/S0301-0511(03)00101-7 URL pmid: 14602354 |
17 | Haykin S., Fatemi M., Setoodeh P., & Xue Y . ( 2012). Cognitive control. Proceedings of the IEEE, 100( 12), 3156-3169. |
18 |
Indovina I. &Macaluso E. , ( 2007). Dissociation of stimulus relevance and saliency factors during shifts of visuospatial attention. Cerebral Cortex, 17( 7), 1701-1711.
doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl081 URL pmid: 17003078 |
19 |
Jannati A., Gaspar J. M., & McDonald J. J . ( 2013). Tracking target and distractor processing in fixed-feature visual search: Evidence from human electrophysiology. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 39( 6), 1713-1730.
doi: 10.1037/a0032251 URL pmid: 23527999 |
20 |
Keyes H. &Dlugokencka A. , ( 2014). Do I have my attention? Speed of processing advantages for the self-face are not driven by automatic attention capture. PLoS One, 9( 10), e110792.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0110792 URL pmid: 4206440 |
21 |
Kim J., Kang M. S., Cho Y. S., & Lee S. H . ( 2017). Prolonged interruption of cognitive control of conflict processing over human faces by task-irrelevant emotion expression. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1024.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01024 URL pmid: 5476788 |
22 |
Kratz O., Studer P., Malcherek S., Erbe K., Moll G. H., & Heinrich H . ( 2011). Attentional processes in children with ADHD: An event-related potential study using the attention network test. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 81( 2), 82-90.
doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2011.05.008 URL pmid: 21641942 |
23 |
Li H., &Cai H.D, . ( 2013). The modulation of emotion on the attentional function networks. Advances in Psychological Science, 21( 1), 59-67.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2013.00059 URL |
24 |
[ 李贺, 蔡厚德 . ( 2013). 情绪对注意功能网络的调制. 心理科学进展, 21( 1), 59-67.]
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2013.00059 URL |
25 | Liu M. H., Wang L. Y., Sui J., & Zhang M . ( 2012). Modulation of self-face for visual spatial attention: Evidence from a Posner’s cueing paradigm. Journal of Psychological Science, 35( 1), 24-29. |
26 |
Mackie M. A., Dam N. T. V., & Fan J . ( 2013). Cognitive control and attentional functions. Brain and Cognition, 82( 3), 301-312.
doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2013.05.004 URL pmid: 23792472 |
27 |
Moray N.., ( 1959). Attention in dichotic listening: Affective cues and the influence of instructions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 11( 1), 56-60.
doi: 10.1080/17470215908416289 URL |
28 |
Mueller. C.J., &Kuchinke , L. ( 2016). Processing of face identity in the affective flanker task: A diffusion model analysis. Psychological Research, 80( 6), 1-11.
doi: 10.1007/s00426-015-0696-3 URL pmid: 26253324 |
29 |
Neuhaus A. H., Urbanek C., Opgen-Rhein C., Hahn E., Ta T. M. T., Koehler S., .. Dettling M . ( 2010). Event-related potentials associated with attention network test. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 76( 2), 72-79.
doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2010.02.005 URL pmid: 20184924 |
30 |
Ninomiya H., Onitsuka T., Chen C. H., Sato E., & Tashiro N . ( 1998). P300 in response to the subject’s own face. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 52( 5), 519-522.
doi: 10.1046/j.1440-1819.1998.00445.x URL pmid: 10215014 |
31 |
Palermo R., & Rhodes G . ( 2007). Are you always on my mind? A review of how face perception and attention interact. Neuropsychologia, 45( 1), 75-92.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.04.025 URL pmid: 16797607 |
32 | Pannese A., &Hirsch J. , ( 2010). Self-specific priming effect. Consciousness and Cognition, 19( 4), 962-968. |
33 | Petersen, S. E., &Posner , M. I . ( 2012). The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 35( 5), 73-89. |
34 | Posner, M. I., &Petersen , S. E . ( 1990). The attention system of the human brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13, 25-42. |
35 |
Rothbart, M. K., &Posner , M. I . ( 2015). The developing brain in a multitasking world. Developmental Review, 35, 42-63.
doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2014.12.006 URL pmid: 25821335 |
36 |
Shulman G. L., D'Avossa G., Tansy A. P., & Corbetta M . ( 2002). Two attentional processes in the parietal lobe. Cerebral Cortex, 12( 11), 1124-1131.
doi: 10.1093/cercor/12.11.1124 URL pmid: 12379601 |
37 |
Spagna A., Martella D., Sebastiani M., Maccari L., Marotta A., & Casagrande M . ( 2014). Efficiency and interactions of alerting, orienting and executive networks: The impact of imperative stimulus type. Acta Psychologica, 148( 3), 209-215.
doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.02.007 URL pmid: 24607440 |
38 |
Sui J., Liu C. H., Wang L., & Han S . ( 2009). Attentional orientation induced by temporarily established self- referential cues. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62( 5), 844-849.
doi: 10.1080/17470210802559393 URL pmid: 19132633 |
39 |
Sui J., Rotshtein P., & Humphreys G. W . ( 2013). Coupling social attention to the self forms a network for personal significance. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 110( 19), 7607-7612.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1221862110 URL |
40 |
Sui J., Zhu Y., & Han S. H . ( 2006). Self-face recognition in attended and unattended conditions: An event-related brain potential study. Neuroreport, 17( 4), 423-427.
doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000203357.65190.61 URL pmid: 16514370 |
41 |
Tang X., Wu J., & Shen Y . ( 2006). The interactions of multisensory integration with endogenous and exogenous attention. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 61( 14), 208-224
doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.11.002 URL pmid: 26546734 |
42 |
Tong F.., &Nakayama , K. ( 1999). Robust representations for faces: Evidence from visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25( 4), 1016-1035.
doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.25.4.1016 URL pmid: 10464943 |
43 |
Turk D. J., Brady-van Den Bos M., Collard P., Gillespie- Smith K., Conway M. A., & Cunningham S. J . ( 2013). Divided attention selectively impairs memory for self- relevant information. Memory & Cognition, 41( 4), 503-510.
doi: 10.3758/s13421-012-0279-0 URL pmid: 23263878 |
44 |
Uddin L.Q . ( 2015). Salience processing and insular cortical function and dysfunction. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 16( 1), 55-61.
doi: 10.1038/nrn3857 URL pmid: 25406711 |
45 |
Visintin E., De Panfilis C., Antonucci C., Capecci C., Marchesi C., & Sambataro F . ( 2015). Parsing the intrinsic networks underlying attention: A resting state study. Behavioural Brain Research, 278, 315-322.
doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.10.002 URL pmid: 25311282 |
46 |
Wang X., Zhao X., Gui X., & Chen A . ( 2016). Alertness function of thalamus in conflict adaptation. NeuroImage, 132, 274-282.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.02.048 URL pmid: 26908318 |
47 |
Xuan B., Mackie M. A., Spagna A., Wu T., Tian Y., Hof P. R., & Fan J . ( 2016). The activation of interactive attentional networks. NeuroImage, 129, 308-319.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.01.017 URL pmid: 26794640 |
48 |
Yamada Y., Kawabe T., & Miura K . ( 2012). One's own name distorts visual space. Neuroscience Letters, 531( 2), 96-98.
doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2012.10.028 URL pmid: 23127857 |
49 |
Zhu M., Hu Y., Tang X., Luo J., & Gao X . ( 2015). Withholding response to self-face is faster than to other- face. Journal of Motor Behavior, 47( 2), 117-123.
doi: 10.1080/00222895.2014.959888 URL pmid: 25356599 |
[1] | 祖光耀, 李舒淇, 张天阳, 王爱君, 张明. 返回抑制对视听跨通道对应的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(8): 1220-1233. |
[2] | 李其容, 李春萱, 杨艳宇. 创业进展与创业努力的多层次关系:创业自我效能的中介与调节定向的调节作用[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(4): 642-657. |
[3] | 许销冰, 张忞硕, 曾帅帆, 范卓怡. 产品透明性对消费者品牌感知的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(10): 1696-1711. |
[4] | 张慧, 刘燕君, 史燕伟, 张南. 客户支持对一线服务员工服务绩效的影响——基于自我验证理论的视角[J]. 心理学报, 2022, 54(4): 398-410. |
[5] | 王慧媛, 陈艾睿, 张明. 意义关联的注意定向效应:基于空间位置的抑制和捕获[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(2): 113-127. |
[6] | 陈玉明, 李思瑾, 郭田友, 谢慧, 徐锋, 张丹丹. 背外侧前额叶对主动遗忘负性社会反馈的作用:针对抑郁症的TMS研究[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(10): 1094-1104. |
[7] | 罗禹, 念靖晴, 鲍未, 张静静, 赵守盈, 潘运, 许爽, 张禹. 急性应激损害对威胁刺激的注意解除[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(1): 26-37. |
[8] | 任小云, 李玉婷, 毛伟宾, 耿秋晨. 情绪对连续事件定向遗忘的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2019, 51(3): 269-279. |
[9] | 张燕, 曹慧敏, 郑元杰, 任衍具. 自上而下的目标调节奖赏联结干扰子 的注意定向和脱离[J]. 心理学报, 2018, 50(4): 377-389. |
[10] | 黄敏学;王艺婷; 廖俊云;刘茂红. 评论不一致性对消费者的双面影响:产品属性与调节定向的调节[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(3): 370-382. |
[11] | 杨文琪;李强;郭名扬;范谦;何伊丽. 权力感对个体的影响:调节定向的视角[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(3): 404-415. |
[12] | 耿晓伟, 姜宏艺. 调节定向和调节匹配对情感预测中 影响偏差的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(12): 1537-1547. |
[13] | 周希;宛小昂;杜頔康;熊异雷;黄蔚欣. 不连续虚拟现实空间中的再定向[J]. 心理学报, 2016, 48(8): 924-932. |
[14] | 王爱君;李毕琴;张明. 三维空间深度位置上基于空间的返回抑制[J]. 心理学报, 2015, 47(7): 859-868. |
[15] | 李晔;张文慧;龙立荣. 自我牺牲型领导对下属工作绩效的影响机制 ——战略定向与领导认同的中介作用[J]. 心理学报, 2015, 47(5): 653-662. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||