Advances in Psychological Science ›› 2024, Vol. 32 ›› Issue (9): 1379-1392.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2024.01379
• Conceptual Framework • Next Articles
LI Xingshan1,2(), ZHANG Qiwei1,2(), HUANG Linjieqiong1
Received:
2023-12-06
Online:
2024-09-15
Published:
2024-06-26
CLC Number:
LI Xingshan, ZHANG Qiwei, HUANG Linjieqiong. Computational modeling and experimental validation of Chinese lexical and semantic processing[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2024, 32(9): 1379-1392.
[1] |
曹海波, 兰泽波, 高峰, 于海涛, 李鹏, 王敬欣. (2023). 词素位置概率在中文阅读中的作用:词汇判断和眼动研究. 心理学报, 55(2), 159-176. https://doi.org/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2023.00159
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2023.00159 URL |
[2] |
刘志方, 仝文, 张智君, 赵亚军. (2020). 语境预测性对阅读中字词加工过程的影响:眼动证据. 心理学报, 52(9), 1031-1047. https://doi.org/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2020.01031
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2020.01031 URL |
[3] | 申薇, 李兴珊. (2012). 中文阅读中词优效应的特异性. 科学通报, 57(35), 3414-3420. https://doi.org/10.1360/972012-666 |
[4] | 王春茂, 彭聃龄. (1999). 合成词加工中的词频、词素频率及语义透明度. 心理学报, 31(3), 266-273. |
[5] | Bai, X., Yan, G., Liversedge, S., Zang, C., & Rayner, K. (2008). Reading spaced and unspaced Chinese text: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34(5), 1277-1287. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.34.5.1277 |
[6] | Bemis, D. K., & Pylkkänen, L. (2011). Simple composition: A magnetoencephalography investigation into the comprehension of minimal linguistic phrases. The Journal of Neuroscience, 31(8), 2801-2814. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5003-10.2011 |
[7] |
Cao, H., Gao, M., & Yan, H. (2016). Character decomposition and transposition processes in Chinese compound words modulates attentional blink. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 923. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00923
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00923 URL pmid: 27379003 |
[8] | Chang, Y.-N., Welbourne, S., & Lee, C.-Y. (2016). Exploring orthographic neighborhood size effects in a computational model of Chinese character naming. Cognitive Psychology, 91, 1-23. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2016.09.001 |
[9] | Chen, L., Perfetti, C., Leng, Y., & Li, Y. (2018). Word superiority effect for native Chinese readers and low-proficiency Chinese learners. Applied Psycholinguistics, 39(6), 1097-1115. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716418000255 |
[10] | Cui, L., Wang, J., Zhang, Y., Cong, F., Zhang, W., & Hyona, J. (2021). Compound word frequency modifies the effect of character frequency in reading Chinese. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 74(4), 610-633. https://doi.org/10.1177/1747021820973661 |
[11] | Cui, L., Zang, C., Xu, X., Zhang, W., Su, Y., & Liversedge, S. P. (2022). Predictability effects and parafoveal processing of compound words in natural Chinese reading. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 75(1), 18-29. https://doi.org/10.1177/17470218211048193 |
[12] | Dehaene, S. (2009). Reading in the brain: The science and evolution of a human invention. New York, NY: Penguin Press. https://api.semanticscholar.org/CorpusID:141548807 |
[13] | Engbert, R., & Kliegl, R. (2011). Parallel graded attention models of reading. In S. P. Liversedge, I. D. Gilchrist, & S. Everling (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of eye movements. (pp.787-800). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199539789.001.0001 |
[14] |
Flick, G., Oseki, Y., Kaczmarek, A. R., Al Kaabi, M., Marantz, A., & Pylkkänen, L. (2018). Building words and phrases in the left temporal lobe. Cortex, 106, 213-236. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2018.06.004
doi: S0010-9452(18)30190-4 URL pmid: 30007863 |
[15] |
Harm, M. W., & Seidenberg, M. S. (2004). Computing the meanings of words in reading: Cooperative division of labor between visual and phonological processes. Psychological Review, 111(3), 662-720. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.111.3.662
URL pmid: 15250780 |
[16] | Hsiao, J. H., & Shillcock, R. (2004). Connectionist modelling of Chinese character pronunciation based on foveal splitting. In K. Forbus, D. Gentner, & T. Regier (Eds.), Proceedings of the Twenty-Sixth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 601-606). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. |
[17] | Hsiao, J. H., & Shillcock, R. (2005). Foveal splitting causes differential processing of Chinese orthography in the male and female brain. Brain Research. Cognitive Brain Research, 25(2), 531-536. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.08.005 |
[18] | Hsu, C.-H., Pylkkänen, L., & Lee, C.-Y. (2019). Effects of morphological complexity in left temporal cortex: An MEG study of reading Chinese disyllabic words. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 49, 168-177. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2018.06.004 |
[19] | Huang, L., & Li, X. (2020). Early, but not overwhelming: The effect of prior context on segmenting overlapping ambiguous strings when reading Chinese. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 73(9), 1382-1395. https://doi.org/10.1177/1747021820926012 |
[20] | Huang, L., Staub, A., & Li, X. (2021). Prior context influences lexical competition when segmenting Chinese overlapping ambiguous strings. Journal of Memory and Language, 118, 104218. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2021.104218 |
[21] | Inhoff, A. W., & Wu, C. (2005). Eye movements and the identification of spatially ambiguous words during Chinese sentence reading. Memory & Cognition, 33(8), 1345-1356. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193367 |
[22] | Landauer, T. K., & Dumais, S. T. (1997). A solution to Plato’s problem: The latent semantic analysis theory of acquisition, induction, and representation of knowledge. Psychological Review, 104(2), 211-240. |
[23] | Leck, K. J., Weekes, B. S., & Chen, M. J. (1995). Visual and phonological pathways to the lexicon: Evidence from Chinese readers. Memory & Cognition, 23(4), 468-476. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03197248 |
[24] | Li, X., Bicknell, K., Liu, P., Wei, W., & Rayner, K. (2014). Reading is fundamentally similar across disparate writing systems: A systematic characterization of how words and characters influence eye movements in Chinese reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(2), 895-913. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0033580 |
[25] | Li, X., Gu, J., Liu, P., & Rayner, K. (2013). The advantage of word-based processing in Chinese reading: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 39(3), 879-889. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030337 |
[26] | Li, X., Huang, L., Yao, P., & Hyönä, J. (2022). Universal and specific reading mechanisms across different writing systems. Nature Reviews Psychology, 1(3), 133-144. https://doi.org/10.1038/s44159-022-00022-6 |
[27] | Li, X., & Pollatsek, A. (2020). An integrated model of word processing and eye-movement control during Chinese reading. Psychological Review, 127(6), 1139-1162. https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000248 |
[28] |
Li, X., Rayner, K., & Cave, K. R. (2009). On the segmentation of Chinese words during reading. Cognitive Psychology, 58(4), 525-552. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2009.02.003
doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2009.02.003 URL pmid: 19345938 |
[29] |
Ma, G., Li, X., & Rayner, K. (2014). Word segmentation of overlapping ambiguous strings during Chinese reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40(3), 1046-1059. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035389
doi: 10.1037/a0035389 URL pmid: 24417292 |
[30] |
Ma, G., Li, X., & Rayner, K. (2015). Readers extract character frequency information from nonfixated-target word at long pretarget fixations during Chinese reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 41(5), 1409-1419. https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000072
doi: 10.1037/xhp0000072 URL pmid: 26168144 |
[31] | McClelland, J. L., & Rumelhart, D. E. (1981). An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: I. An account of basic findings. Psychological Review, 88(5), 375-407. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.88.5.375 |
[32] | McCutchen, D., & Perfetti, C. A. (1982). The visual tongue- twister effect: Phonological activation in silent reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 21(6), 672-687. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(82)90870-2 |
[33] | Mikolov, T., Sutskever, I., Chen, K., Corrado, G., & Dean, J. (2013). Distributed representations of words and phrases and their compositionality. Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems, 26, 3111-3119. |
[34] | Peng, D. L., Liu, Y., & Wang, C. (1999). How is access representation organized? The relation of polymorphemic words and their morphemes in Chinese. In J. Wang, A. W. Inhoff, & H.-C. Chen (Eds.), Reading Chinese script: A cognitive analysis (pp. 65-89). Erlbaum. |
[35] |
Perfetti, C. A., Liu, Y., & Tan, L. H. (2005). The lexical constituency model: Some implications of research on Chinese for general theories of reading. Psychological Review, 112(1), 43-59. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.112.1.43
URL pmid: 15631587 |
[36] | Perfetti, C. A., & Zhang, S. (1995). Very early phonological activation in Chinese reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21(1), 24-33. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.21.1.24 |
[37] |
Plaut, D. C., McClelland, J. L., Seidenberg, M. S., & Patterson, K. (1996). Understanding normal and impaired word reading: Computational principles in quasi-regular domains. Psychological Review, 103(1), 56-115. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.103.1.56
doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.103.1.56 URL pmid: 8650300 |
[38] | Pylkkänen, L. (2020). Neural basis of basic composition: What we have learned from the red-boat studies and their extensions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 375(1791), 20190299. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2019.0299 |
[39] | Rayner, K., Li, X., Juhasz, B. J., & Yan, G. (2005). The effect of word predictability on the eye movements of Chinese readers. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12(6), 1089-1093. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03206448 |
[40] | Rayner, K., Pollatsek, A., Ashby, J., & Clifton, C., Jr. (2011). Psychology of reading (2nd ed.). London, UK: Psychology Press. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203155158 |
[41] |
Reicher, G. M. (1969). Perceptual recognition as a function of meaningfulness of stimulus material. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81(2), 275-280. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0027768
URL pmid: 5811803 |
[42] | Reichle, E. D. (2021). Computational models of reading: A handbook. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780195370669.001.0001 |
[43] |
Reichle, E. D., Pollatsek, A., Fisher, D. L., & Rayner, K. (1998). Toward a model of eye movement control in reading. Psychological Review, 105(1), 125-157. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.105.1.125
URL pmid: 9450374 |
[44] | Seidenberg, M. S. (2017). Language at the speed of sight: How we read, why so many can’t, and what can be done about it. New York: Basic Books. |
[45] |
Seidenberg, M. S., & McClelland, J. L. (1989). A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. Psychological Review, 96(4), 523-568. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.96.4.523
doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.96.4.523 URL pmid: 2798649 |
[46] | Share, D. L. (2008). On the Anglocentricities of current reading research and practice: The perils of overreliance on an “outlier” orthography. Psychological Bulletin, 134(4), 584-615. |
[47] | Shen, W., & Li, X. (2016). Processing and representation of ambiguous words in Chinese reading: Evidence from eye movements. Frontiers in Psychology, 7. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01713 |
[48] | Shen, W., Li, X., & Pollatsek, A. (2018). The processing of Chinese compound words with ambiguous morphemes in sentence context. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71(1), 131-139. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2016.1270975 |
[49] | Taft, M., Huang, J., & Zhu, X. (1994). The influence of character frequency on word recognition responses in Chinese. In H. W. Chang, J. T. Hung, C. W. Hue, & O. Tzeng (Eds.), Advances in the study of Chinese language processing (pp. 59-73). Taipei: National Taiwan University. |
[50] | Taft, M., & Nguyen-Hoan, M. (2010). A sticky stick? The locus of morphological representation in the lexicon. Language and Cognitive Processes, 25(2), 277-296. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960903043261 |
[51] | Taft, M., & Zhu, X. P. (1997). Submorphemic processing in reading Chinese. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 23(3), 761-775. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.23.3.761 |
[52] | Taft, M., Zhu, X., & Peng, D. (1999). Positional specificity of radicals in Chinese character recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 40(4), 498-519. https://doi.org/ 10.1006/jmla.1998.2625 |
[53] | Tan, L. H., & Perfetti, C. A. (1997). Visual Chinese character recognition: Does phonological information mediate access to meaning? Journal of Memory and Language, 37(1), 41-57. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1997.2508 |
[54] | Tan, L. H., & Perfetti, C. A. (1998). Phonological codes as early sources of constraint in Chinese word identification:A review of current discoveries and theoretical accounts. In C. K. Leong & K. Tamaoka (Eds.), Cognitive processing of the Chinese and the Japanese languages (Vol. 14, pp. 11-46). Springer Netherlands. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-015-9161-4_2 |
[55] | Tan, L. H., & Perfetti, C. A. (1999). Phonological activation in visual identification of Chinese two-character words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25(2), 382-393. |
[56] | Tsang, Y.-K., & Chen, H.-C. (2013). Early morphological processing is sensitive to morphemic meanings: Evidence from processing ambiguous morphemes. Journal of Memory and Language, 68(3), 223-239. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.003 |
[57] | Tsang, Y.-K., & Chen, H.-C. (2014). Activation of morphemic meanings in processing opaque words. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21(5), 1281-1286. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-014-0589-2 |
[58] | Tsang, Y.-K., Wong, A. W.-K., Huang, J., & Chen, H.-C. (2014). Morpho-orthographic and morpho-semantic processing in word recognition and production: Evidence from ambiguous morphemes. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 29(5), 543-560. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2013.790554 |
[59] | Tse, C.-S., & Yap, M. J. (2018). The role of lexical variables in the visual recognition of two-character Chinese compound words: A megastudy analysis. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71(9), 2022-2038. https://doi.org/10.1177/1747021817738965 |
[60] | Van Orden, G. C., & Kloos, H. (2005). The question of phonology and reading. In M. J. Snowling, & C. Hulme (Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook. (pp.61-78). Blackwell Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470757642.ch4 |
[61] |
Westerlund, M., & Pylkkänen, L. (2014). The role of the left anterior temporal lobe in semantic composition vs. Semantic memory. Neuropsychologia, 57, 59-70. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.03.001
doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.03.001 URL pmid: 24631260 |
[62] |
Wong, A. W.-K., Wu, Y., & Chen, H.-C. (2014). Limited role of phonology in reading Chinese two-character compounds: Evidence from an ERP study. Neuroscience, 256, 342-351. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.10.035
URL pmid: 24505608 |
[63] | Xing, H., Shu, H., & Li, P. (2002). A self-organizing connectionist model of character acquisition in Chinese. In W. D. Gray & C. D. Schunn (Eds.), Proceedings of the twenty-fourth annual conference of the cognitive science society (pp. 950-955). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315782379-198 |
[64] | Xing, H., Shu, H., & Li, P. (2004). The acquisition of Chinese characters: Corpus analyses and connectionist simulations. Journal of Cognitive Science, 5(1), 1-49. |
[65] | Xiong, J., Yu, L., Veldre, A., Reichle, E. D., & Andrews, S. (2023). A multitask comparison of word- and character- frequency effects in Chinese reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 49(5), 793-811. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001192 |
[66] | Yan, G., Tian, H., Bai, X., & Rayner, K. (2006). The effect of word and character frequency on the eye movements of Chinese readers. British Journal of Psychology, 97(2), 259-268. https://doi.org/10.1348/000712605X70066 |
[67] | Yan, M., Richter, E. M., Shu, H., & Kliegl, R. (2009). Readers of Chinese extract semantic information from parafoveal words. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16(3), 561-566. https://doi.org/10.3758/pbr.16.3.561 |
[68] | Yang, J. F., McCandliss, B. D., Shu, H., & Zevin, J. D. (2009). Simulating language-specific and language- general effects in a statistical learning model of Chinese reading. Journal of Memory and Language, 61(2), 238-257. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2009.05.001 |
[69] | Yang, J. F., Shu, H., McCandliss, B. D., & Zevin, J. D. (2013). Orthographic influences on division of labor in learning to read Chinese and English: Insights from computational modeling. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16(2), 354-366. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728912000296 |
[70] | Yang, J. F., Zevin, J. D., Shu, H., McCandliss, B. D., & Li, P. (2006). A “triangle model” of Chinese reading. In R. Sun (Ed.), Proceedings of the twenty eighth annual conference of the cognitive science society (pp. 912-917). Erlbaum. |
[71] | Yang, J. M., Staub, A., Li, N., Wang, S., & Rayner, K. (2012). Plausibility effects when reading one- and two-character words in Chinese: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38(6), 1801-1809. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028478 |
[72] | Yao, P., Alkhammash, R., & Li, X. (2022). Plausibility and syntactic reanalysis in processing novel noun-noun combinations during Chinese reading: Evidence from native and non-native speakers. Scientific Studies of Reading, 26(5), 390-408. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2021.2020796 |
[73] | Yao, P., Staub, A., & Li, X. (2022). Predictability eliminates neighborhood effects during Chinese sentence reading. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 29(1), 243-252. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-021-01966-1 |
[74] | Yu, L., Cutter, M. G., Yan, G., Bai, X., Fu, Y., Drieghe, D., & Liversedge, S. P. (2016). Word n + 2 preview effects in three-character Chinese idioms and phrases. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 31(9), 1130-1149. https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2016.1197954 |
[75] | Yu, L., Liu, Y., & Reichle, E. D. (2021). A corpus-based versus experimental examination of word- and character- frequency effects in Chinese reading: Theoretical implications for models of reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 150(8), 1612-1641. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001014 |
[76] | Zang, C. (2019). New perspectives on serialism and parallelism in oculomotor control during reading: The multi-constituent unit hypothesis. Vision, 3(4), 50. https://doi.org/10.3390/vision3040050 |
[77] | Zhang, H., Su, I.-F., Chen, F., Ng, M. L., Wang, L., & Yan, N. (2020). The time course of orthographic and semantic activation in Chinese character recognition: Evidence from an ERP study. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 35(3), 292-309. https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2019.1652762 |
[78] | Zhang, S., & Perfetti, C. A. (1993). The tongue-twister effect in reading Chinese. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19(5), 1082-1093. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.19.5.1082 |
[79] | Zhou, J., & Li, X. (2021). On the segmentation of Chinese incremental words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 47(8), 1353-1368. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000984 |
[80] | Zhou, W., Shu, H., Miller, K., & Yan, M. (2018). Reliance on orthography and phonology in reading of Chinese: A developmental study. Journal of Research in Reading, 41(2), 370-391. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12111 |
[81] | Zhou, X., & Marslen-Wilson, W. (2000). The relative time course of semantic and phonological activation in reading Chinese. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 26(5), 1245-1265. https://doi.org/10.1037//0278-7393.26.5.1245 |
[82] | Zhou, X., Marslen-Wilson, W., Taft, M., & Shu, H. (1999). Morphology, orthography, and phonology reading Chinese compound words. Language and Cognitive Processes, 14(5-6), 525-565. https://doi.org/10.1080/016909699386185 |
[83] | Zhou, X., Ye, Z., Cheung, H., & Chen, H.-C. (2009). Processing the Chinese language: An introduction. Language and Cognitive Processes, 24(7-8), 929-946. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960903201281 |
[1] | ZHANG Xiangyang, WANG Xiaojuan, YANG Jianfeng. The role of the left Angular Gyrus in lexical-semantic processing [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2024, 32(4): 616-626. |
[2] | LI Lin, ZHAO Sainan, ZHANG Lijuan, WANG Jingxin. Understanding mechanisms of prediction error cost in Chinese reading for older adults [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(1): 1-14. |
[3] | ZHANG Yinhua, LI Hong, WU Yin. The application of computational modelling in the studies of moral cognition [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2020, 28(7): 1042-1055. |
[4] | CHENG Shijing, HE Wenguang. The acquisition, development and aging of semantic cognition and related neural mechanism [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2020, 28(7): 1156-1163. |
[5] | ZHANG Manman, ZANG Chuanli, BAI Xuejun. The spatial extent and depth of parafoveal pre-processing during Chinese reading [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2020, 28(6): 871-882. |
[6] | ZANG Chuanli, LU Zijia, ZHANG Zhichao. The role of semantic and syntactic information in parafoveal prcoessing during reading [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2019, 27(1): 11-19. |
[7] | ZHAO Jing. Skills of visual attention span in developmental dyslexia [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2019, 27(1): 20-26. |
[8] | YANG Jianfeng, DANG Min, ZHANG Rui, WANG Xiaojuan. The neural circuit of semantic processing and its dynamic cooperation with the neural circuit of phonological processing in reading Chinese characters [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2018, 26(3): 381-390. |
[9] | LI Yugang; HUANG Ren; HUA Huimin; LI Xingshan. How do readers select the saccade targets? [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2017, 25(3): 404-412. |
[10] | LI Songqing; ZHAO Qingbai; ZHOU Zhijin; ZHANG Yi. The Cognitive and Neural Mechanism of Text and Picture Processing in Multimedia Learning [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2015, 23(8): 1361-1370. |
[11] | CAI Lin;ZHANG Yaxu. EEG Time-Frequency Analysis of Syntactic and Semantic Processing in Sentence Comprehension [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2014, 22(7): 1112-1121. |
[12] | ZANG Chuan-Li;ZHANG Man-Man;GUO Xiao-Feng;LIU Juan;YAN Guo-Li;BAI Xue-Jun. Several Effects on Chinese Lexical Processing: Evidence from Eye Movements [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2012, 20(9): 1382-1392. |
[13] | REN Gui-Qin;HAN Yu-Chang;LIU Ying. On-line Study on Chinese Word Recognition During Sentence Reading [J]. , 2012, 20(4): 493-503. |
[14] | LI Xing-Shan;LIU Ping-Ping;MA Guo-Jie. Advances in Cognitive Mechanisms of Word Segmentation During Chinese Reading [J]. , 2011, 19(4): 459-470. |
[15] | BAI Xue-Jun;LIU Juan;ZANG Chuan-Li;ZHANG Man-Man;GUO Xiao-Feng;YAN Guo-Li. The Advance of Parafoveal Preview Effects in Chinese Reading [J]. , 2011, 19(12): 1721-1729. |
Viewed | ||||||
Full text |
|
|||||
Abstract |
|
|||||