心理科学进展 ›› 2022, Vol. 30 ›› Issue (2): 436-448.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2022.00436
收稿日期:
2021-03-21
出版日期:
2022-02-15
发布日期:
2021-12-24
通讯作者:
陈璟,佘升翔
E-mail:cjbelinda@126.com;091019@cdnu.edu.cn;shengxiangs@glut.edu.cn
基金资助:
CHEN Jing1,2(), ZHANG Rong2, YUAN Jiaqi2, SHE Shengxiang3()
Received:
2021-03-21
Online:
2022-02-15
Published:
2021-12-24
Contact:
CHEN Jing,SHE Shengxiang
E-mail:cjbelinda@126.com;091019@cdnu.edu.cn;shengxiangs@glut.edu.cn
摘要:
博弈中的反社会惩罚是指博弈者对表现出亲社会行为的高贡献或高合作性他人实施有代价经济惩罚、消极评价或排斥打压的现象。已有研究用带惩罚的经典博弈范式证明了反社会惩罚受到多种个体与环境因素的影响, 并分别从侵犯、报复、社会比较、偏离群体规范、进化策略视角提出了解释其产生机制的5种假说。未来研究可在进一步厘清概念与测量指标、创新研究方法、拓展影响因素研究、明确产生机制、开展针对性干预研究方面做深入探索。
中图分类号:
陈璟, 张融, 袁佳琦, 佘升翔. (2022). 博弈中的反社会惩罚. 心理科学进展 , 30(2), 436-448.
CHEN Jing, ZHANG Rong, YUAN Jiaqi, SHE Shengxiang. (2022). Antisocial punishment in the game. Advances in Psychological Science, 30(2), 436-448.
合作 | 背叛 | 惩罚 | |
---|---|---|---|
合作 | (1, 1) | (-2, 3) | (-5, 1) |
背叛 | (3, -2) | (0, 0) | (-3, -2) |
惩罚 | (1, -5) | (-2, -3) | (-5, -5) |
表1 PD惩罚博弈的收益矩阵
合作 | 背叛 | 惩罚 | |
---|---|---|---|
合作 | (1, 1) | (-2, 3) | (-5, 1) |
背叛 | (3, -2) | (0, 0) | (-3, -2) |
惩罚 | (1, -5) | (-2, -3) | (-5, -5) |
[1] | 蔡华俭, 黄梓航, 林莉, 张明杨, 王潇欧, 朱慧珺, 谢怡萍, 杨盈, 杨紫嫣, 敬一鸣. (2020). 半个多世纪来中国人的心理与行为变化--心理学视野下的研究. 心理科学进展, 28(10), 1599-1618. |
[2] | 陈思静, 朱玥. (2020). 惩罚的另一张面孔: 惩罚的负面作用及破坏性惩罚. 心理科学, 43(4), 911-917. |
[3] | 陈欣, 赵国祥, 叶浩生. (2014). 公共物品困境中惩罚的形式与作用. 心理科学进展, 22(1), 160-170. |
[4] | 杜旌, 冉曼曼, 曹平. (2014). 中庸价值取向对员工变革行为的情景依存作用. 心理学报, 46(1), 113-124. |
[5] | 傅于玲, 邓富民, 杨帅, 徐玖平. (2018). 舌尖上的“自虐”--食辣中的心理学问题. 心理科学进展, 26(9), 1651-1660. |
[6] | 李晓博, 马剑虹. (2017). 公共物品两难中惩罚对合作的影响: 偏好异质的视角. 社会科学家, 6, 67-71. |
[7] | 童婷. (2017). 助人困境中反社会惩罚行为的影响因素及基于第三惩罚的干预研究 (硕士学位论文). 华中师范大学, 武汉. |
[8] | 汪崇金, 聂左玲. (2015). 破解社会合作难题: 强互惠真的够强吗?--基于公共品实验研究. 外国经济与管理, 37(5), 53-65. |
[9] | 汪崇金, 史丹, 聂左玲, 崔凤. (2018). 打开天窗说亮话:社会合作何以可能. 中国工业经济, 361(4), 163-180. |
[10] | 吴睿, 郭庆科, 李芳. (2018). 内隐和外显测量对利他行为的预测: 来自IAT和BIAT的证据. 心理学探新, 38(4), 356-362. |
[11] | 张耀. (2016). 公共品博弈中合作演化及异质性研究 (硕士学位论文). 杭州电子科技大学. |
[12] |
Abbink, K., & Herrmann, B. (2011). The moral costs of nastiness. Economic Inquiry, 49(2), 631-633.
doi: 10.1111/ecin.2011.49.issue-2 URL |
[13] |
Abrams, D., Marques, J. M., Bown, N., & Henson, M. (2000). Pro-norm and anti-norm deviance within and between groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(5), 906-912.
pmid: 10821197 |
[14] |
Anderson, C. M., & Putterman, L. (2006). Do non-strategic sanctions obey the law of demand? The demand for punishment in the voluntary contribution mechanism. Games and Economic Behavior, 54(1), 1-24.
doi: 10.1016/j.geb.2004.08.007 URL |
[15] |
Auerswald, H., Schmidt, C., Thum, M., & Torsvik, G. (2018). Teams in a public goods experiment with punishment. Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Economics, 72, 28-39.
doi: 10.1016/j.socec.2017.11.001 URL |
[16] |
Balliet, D., & van Lange, P. A. M. (2013). Trust, punishment, and cooperation across 18 societies: A meta-analysis. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(4), 363-379.
doi: 10.1177/1745691613488533 pmid: 26173117 |
[17] |
Barclay, P. (2016). Biological markets and the effects of partner choice on cooperation and friendship. Current Opinion in Psychology, 7, 33-38.
doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.07.012 URL |
[18] |
Bellezza, S., Gino, F., & Keinan, A. (2014). The red sneakers effect: Inferring status and competence from signals of nonconformity. Journal of Consumer Research, 41(1), 35-54.
doi: 10.1086/674870 URL |
[19] |
Bernhard, H., Fischbacher, U., & Fehr, E. (2006). Parochial altruism in humans. Nature, 442, 912-915.
doi: 10.1038/nature04981 URL |
[20] | Brañas-Garza, P., Espín, A. M., Exadaktylos, F., & Herrmann, B. (2014). Fair and unfair punishers coexist in the ultimatum game. Scientific Reports, 4(1), Article 6025. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/srep06025 |
[21] |
Bruhin, A., Janizzi, K., & Thöni, C. (2020). Uncovering the heterogeneity behind cross-cultural variation in antisocial punishment. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 180, 291-308.
doi: 10.1016/j.jebo.2020.10.005 URL |
[22] | Bryson, J. J., Mitchell, J., Powers, S. T., & Sylwester, K. (2014). Understanding and addressing cultural variation in costly antisocial punishment. In M. A.Gibson, & D. W.Lawson(Eds.), Advances in the evolutionary analysis of human behaviour: Vol. 1: Applied evolutionary anthropology: Darwinian approaches to contemporary world issues (pp.201-222). New York, America: Springer. |
[23] |
Carpenter, J. P. (2007). The demand for punishment. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 62(4), 522-542.
doi: 10.1016/j.jebo.2005.05.004 URL |
[24] |
Carré, J. M., Geniole, S. N., Ortiz, T. L., Bird, B. M., Videto, A., & Bonin, P. L. (2017). Exogenous testosterone rapidly increases aggressive behavior in dominant and impulsive men. Biological Psychiatry, 82(4), 249-256.
doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2016.06.009 URL |
[25] |
Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1994). A review and reformulation of social information-processing mechanisms in children’s social adjustment. Psychological Bulletin, 115(1), 74-101.
doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.115.1.74 URL |
[26] |
Denant-Boemont, L., Masclet, D., & Noussair, C. N. (2007). Punishment, counterpunishment and sanction enforcement in a social dilemma experiment. Economic Theory, 33, 145-167.
doi: 10.1007/s00199-007-0212-0 URL |
[27] | Deutchman, P., & Raihani, N. (2017). Dark Triad personality traits vary across countries and predict antisocial behavior. |
[28] | dos Santos, M., Braithwaite, V. A., & Wedekind, C. (2014). Exposure to superfluous information reduces cooperation and increases antisocial punishment in reputation-based interactions. Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution, 2(36), 224-230. |
[29] | Egas, M., & Riedl, A. (2008). The economics of altruistic punishment and the maintenance of cooperation. Proceedings of the Royal Society: Biological Sciences, 275, 871-878. |
[30] | Ellingsen, T., Herrmann, B., Nowak, M. A., Rand, D. G., & Tarnita, C. E. (2012). Civic capital in two cultures: The nature of cooperation in Romania and USA. SSRN Electronic Journal, 89(3), 575-581. |
[31] | Eriksson, K., Cownden, D., Ehn, M., & Strimling, P. (2014). ‘Altruistic’ and ‘antisocial’ punishers are one and the same. Social Science Electronic Publishing, 1, 209-221. |
[32] |
Ertan, A., Page, T., & Putterman, L. (2009). Who to punish? Individual decisions and majority rule in mitigating the free rider problem. European Economic Review, 53(5), 495-511.
doi: 10.1016/j.euroecorev.2008.09.007 URL |
[33] |
Evans, J. S. B. T. (2008). Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgment, and social cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59(1), 255-278.
doi: 10.1146/psych.2008.59.issue-1 URL |
[34] |
Falk, A., Fehr, E., & Fischbacher, U. (2005). Driving forces behind informal sanctions. Econometrica, 73(6), 2017-2030.
doi: 10.1111/ecta.2005.73.issue-6 URL |
[35] |
Fatas, E., Meléndez-Jiménez, M. A., & Solaz, H. (2020). Social hierarchies: A laboratory study on punishment patterns across networks. Economic Inquiry, 58(1), 104-119.
doi: 10.1111/ecin.v58.1 URL |
[36] |
Fehr, E., & Gächter, S. (2000). Cooperation and punishment in public goods experiments. American Economic Review, 90(4), 980-994.
doi: 10.1257/aer.90.4.980 URL |
[37] | Fehr, E., & Williams, T. (2018). Social norms, endogenous sorting and the culture of cooperation. SSRN Electronic Journal. |
[38] |
Fu, T., & Putterman, L. (2018). When is punishment harmful to cooperation? A note on antisocial and perverse punishment. Journal of the Economic Science Association, 4, 151-164.
doi: 10.1007/s40881-018-0053-6 URL |
[39] | Gächter, S., & Herrmann, B. (2009). Reciprocity, culture and human cooperation: Previous insights and a new cross-cultural experiment. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 364(1518), 791-806. |
[40] |
Gal, D., & Rucker, D. D. (2021). Act boldly: Important life decisions, courage, and the motivated pursuit of risk. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 120(6), 1607-1620.
doi: 10.1037/pspi0000329 URL |
[41] |
García, J., & Traulsen, A. (2012). Leaving the loners alone: Evolution of cooperation in the presence of antisocial punishment. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 307(2), 168-173.
doi: 10.1016/j.jtbi.2012.05.011 URL |
[42] |
Gelfand, M. J., Raver, J. L., Nishii, L., Leslie, L. M., Lun, J., Lim, B. C., ... Yamaguchi, S. (2011). Differences between tight and loose cultures: A 33-nation study. Science, 332(6033), 1100-1104.
doi: 10.1126/science.1197754 URL |
[43] |
Gerfo, E. L., Gallucci, A., Morese, R., Vergallito, A., Ottone, S., Ponzano, F., ... Lauro, L. J. R. (2019). The role of ventromedial prefrontal cortex and temporo-parietal junction in third-party punishment behavior. NeuroImage, 200, 501-510.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.06.047 URL |
[44] |
Goette, L., Huffman, D., Meier, S., & Sutter, M. (2012). Competition between organizational groups: Its impact on altruistic and antisocial motivations. Management Science, 58(5), 948-960.
doi: 10.1287/mnsc.1110.1466 URL |
[45] | Gordon, D. S., & Lea, S. E. G. (2016). Who punishes? The status of the punishers affects the perceived success of, and indirect benefits from, "moralistic" punishment. Evolutionary Psychology, 14(3), 1-14. |
[46] |
Gordon, D. S., & Puurtinen, M. (2021). High cooperation and welfare despite-and because of-the threat of antisocial punishments and feuds. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 150(7), 1373-1386.
doi: 10.1037/xge0001004 URL |
[47] |
Grechenig, K., Nicklisch, A., & Thöni, C. (2010). Punishment despite reasonable doubt-a public goods experiment with sanctions under uncertainty. Journal of Empirical Legal Studies, 7(4), 847-867.
doi: 10.1111/jels.2010.7.issue-4 URL |
[48] | Gross, J., Méder, Z. Z., Okamoto-Barth, S., & Riedl, A. (2016). Building the leviathan-voluntary centralisation of punishment power sustains cooperation in humans. Scientific Reports, 6(1), Article 20767. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/srep20767 |
[49] |
Han, D., Yan, S., & Li, D. (2019). The evolutionary public goods game model with punishment mechanism in an activity-driven network. Chaos Solitons & Fractals, 123, 254-259.
doi: 10.1016/j.chaos.2019.04.015 URL |
[50] |
Hauser, O. P., Nowak, M. A., & Rand, D. G. (2014). Punishment does not promote cooperation under exploration dynamics when anti-social punishment is possible. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 360(25), 163-171.
doi: 10.1016/j.jtbi.2014.06.041 URL |
[51] |
Henrich, J., McElreath, R., Barr, A., Ensminger, J., Barrett, C., Bolyanatz, A., ... Ziker, J. (2006). Costly punishment across human societies. Science, 312(5781), 1767-1770.
pmid: 16794075 |
[52] |
Herrmann, B., Thöni, C., & Gächter, S. (2008). Antisocial punishment across societies. Science, 319(5868), 1362-1367.
doi: 10.1126/science.1153808 URL |
[53] | Hilbe, C., & Traulsen, A. (2012). Emergence of responsible sanctions without second order free riders, antisocial punishment or spite. Scientific Reports, 2(6), Article 458. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/srep00458 |
[54] | Hofstede, G. (2001). Culture’s consequences: Comparing values, behaviors, institutions and organisations across nations. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. 27(1),89-94. |
[55] |
Horne, C., & Irwin, K. (2016). Metanorms and antisocial punishment. Social Influence, 11(1), 7-21.
doi: 10.1080/15534510.2015.1132255 URL |
[56] |
Irwin, K., & Horne, C. (2013). A normative explanation of antisocial punishment. Social Science Research, 42(2), 562-570.
doi: 10.1016/j.ssresearch.2012.10.004 pmid: 23347495 |
[57] |
Jordan, J. J., Hoffman, M., Bloom, P., & Rand, D. G. (2016). Third-party punishment as a costly signal of trustworthiness. Nature, 530(7591), 473-476.
doi: 10.1038/nature16981 URL |
[58] |
Kamei, K., & Putterman, L. (2015). In broad daylight: Fuller information and higher-order punishment opportunities can promote cooperation. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 120, 145-159.
doi: 10.1016/j.jebo.2015.09.020 URL |
[59] | Kawamura, Y., & Kusumi, T. (2020). Altruism does not always lead to a good reputation: A normative explanation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 90, Article 104021. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2020.104021 |
[60] |
Kirchkamp, O., & Mill, W. (2020). Conditional cooperation and the effect of punishment. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 174, 150-172.
doi: 10.1016/j.jebo.2020.03.035 URL |
[61] | Klein, N., Grossmann, I., Uskul, A. K., Kraus, A. A., & Epley, N. (2015). It pays to be nice, but not really nice: Asymmetric reputations from prosociality across 7 countries. Judgment & Decision Making, 10(4), 355-364. |
[62] |
Kosfeld, M., & Rustagi, D. (2015). Leader punishment and cooperation in groups: Experimental field evidence from commons management in Ethiopia. American Economic Review, 105(2), 747-783.
doi: 10.1257/aer.20120700 URL |
[63] | Kuběna, A. A., Houdek, P., Lindová, J., Příplatová, L., & Flegr, J. (2014). Justine effect: Punishment of the unduly self-sacrificing cooperative individuals. PLoS One, 9(3), Article e92336. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0092336 |
[64] | Lucas, P., & Malki, I. (2018). A note on the modelling and interpretation of a public goods game experiment. Journal of Applied Statistics, 46(4), 737-753. |
[65] |
Masui, K., Iriguchi, S., Terada, M., Nomura, M., & Ura, M. (2012). Lack of family support and psychopathy facilitates antisocial punishment behavior in college students. Psychology, 3(3), 284-288.
doi: 10.4236/psych.2012.33040 URL |
[66] | Minson, J. A., & Monin, B. (2012). Do-gooder derogation: Disparaging morally motivated minorities to defuse anticipated reproach. Social Psychological & Personality Science, 3(2), 200-207. |
[67] | Monin, B. (2007). Holier than me? Threatening social comparison in the moral domain. Revue Internationale De Psychologie Sociale, 20(1), 53-68. |
[68] | Morese, R., Rabellino, D., Sambataro, F., Perussia, F., Valentini, M. C., Bara, B. G., & Bosco, F. M. (2016). Group membership modulates the neural circuitry underlying third party punishment. PLoS One, 11(11), Article e0166357. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0166357 |
[69] |
Nikiforakis, N. (2008). Punishment and counter-punishment in public good games: Can we really govern ourselves? Journal of Public Economics, 92(1-2), 91-112.
doi: 10.1016/j.jpubeco.2007.04.006 URL |
[70] |
Parks, C. D., & Stone, A. B. (2010). The desire to expel unselfish members from the group. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99(2), 303-310.
doi: 10.1037/a0018403 URL |
[71] |
Pfattheicher, S., Böhm, R., & Kesberg, R. (2018). The advantage of democratic peer punishment in sustaining cooperation within groups. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 31(4), 562-571.
doi: 10.1002/bdm.v31.4 URL |
[72] | Pfattheicher, S., Keller, J., & Knezevic, G. (2017). Sadism, the intuitive system, and antisocial punishment in the public goods game. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 43(3), 337-346. |
[73] | Pfattheicher, S., Landhäeusßer, A., & Keller, J. (2014). Individual differences in antisocial punishment in public goods situations: The interplay of cortisol with testosterone and dominance. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 27(4), 340-348. |
[74] |
Pleasant, A., & Barclay, P. (2018). Why hate the good guy? Antisocial punishment of high cooperators is greater when people compete to be chosen. Psychological Science, 29(6), 868-876.
doi: 10.1177/0956797617752642 pmid: 29708860 |
[75] |
Powers, S. T., Taylor, D. J., & Bryson, J. J. (2012). Punishment can promote defection in group-structured populations. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 311(4), 107-116.
doi: 10.1016/j.jtbi.2012.07.010 URL |
[76] |
Rabellino, D., Morese, R., Ciaramidaro, A., Bara, B. G., & Bosco, F. M. (2016). Third-party punishment: Altruistic and anti-social behaviours in in-group and out-group settings. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 28(4), 486-495.
doi: 10.1080/20445911.2016.1138961 URL |
[77] |
Rand, D. G., Armao, I. V., Joseph, J., Nakamaru, M., & Ohtsuki, H. (2010). Anti-social punishment can prevent the co-evolution of punishment and cooperation. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 265(4), 624-632.
doi: 10.1016/j.jtbi.2010.06.010 URL |
[78] | Rand, D. G., & Nowak, M. A. (2011). The evolution of antisocial punishment in optional public goods games. Nature Communication, 2, Article 434. https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms1442 |
[79] | Stavrova, O., Schlosser, T., & Fetchenhauer, D. (2013). Are virtuous people happy all around the world? Civic virtue, antisocial punishment, and subjective well-being across cultures. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 39(7), 927-942. |
[80] | Sylwester, K., Herrmann, B., & Bryson, J. J. (2013). Homo homini lupus? Explaining antisocial punishment. Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology, and Economics, 6(3), 167-188. |
[81] | Szolnoki, A., & Perc, M. (2017). Second-order free-riding on antisocial punishment restores the effectiveness of prosocial punishment. Physical Review X, 7(4), Article 041027. https://doi.org/10.1103/PhysRevX.7.041027 |
[82] |
Talhelm, T., Zhang, X., Oishi, S., Shimin, C., Duan, D., Lan, X., & Kitayama, S. (2014). Large-scale psychological differences within China explained by rice versus wheat agriculture. Science, 344(6184), 603-608.
doi: 10.1126/science.1246850 pmid: 24812395 |
[83] | Tasimi, A., Dominguez, A., & Wynn, K. (2015). Do-gooder derogation in children: The social costs of generosity. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, Article 1036. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01036 |
[84] | Thöni, C. (2014). Inequality aversion and antisocial punishment. Theory & Decision, 76(4), 529-545. |
[85] |
Weiss, A., Burgmer, P., & Mussweiler, T. (2018). Two-faced morality: Distrust promotes divergent moral standards for the self versus others. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 44(12), 1712-1724.
doi: 10.1177/0146167218775693 URL |
[86] |
Weiss, A., Michels, C., Burgmer, P., Mussweiler, T., Ockenfels, A., & Hofmann, W. (2021). Trust in everyday life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 121(1), 95-114.
doi: 10.1037/pspi0000334 URL |
[87] |
Wu, J. J., Zhang, B. Y., Zhou, Z. X., He, Q. Q., Zheng, X. D., Cressman, R., & Tao, Y. (2009). Costly punishment does not always increase cooperation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(41), 17448-17451.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.0905918106 URL |
[88] |
Zhang, H., Qu, W., Ge, Y., Sun, X., & Zhang, K. (2017). Effect of personality traits, age and sex on aggressive driving: Psychometric adaptation of the Driver Aggression Indicators Scale in China. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 103, 29-36.
doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2017.03.016 URL |
[89] |
Zhou, Y., Jiao, P., & Zhang, Q. (2017). Second-party and third-party punishment in a public goods experiment. Applied Economics Letters, 24(1-3), 54-57.
doi: 10.1080/13504851.2016.1156226 URL |
[1] | 郝娜, 崔丽莹. 补偿他人还是保护自己?内疚与羞耻情绪对合作行为的影响差异[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(7): 1626-1636. |
[2] | 马昕玥, 崔丽莹. 人际同步对合作行为的促进机制及解释模型[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(6): 1317-1326. |
[3] | 唐辉, 李鑫宇, 魏一帆, 李晓彩, 陈柳燕, 张曜. 伙伴选择对合作行为的影响作用与机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(10): 2356-2371. |
[4] | 刘小浪, 刘善仕, 赵瑜, 秦传燕, 陆文珠. 临时知识型员工自主性动机激发:关系导向人力资源管理建构及其作用机理[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(9): 1534-1550. |
[5] | 尚俊辰, 刘智慧, 王笑雨, 迟智超, 李卫君. 两人和三人最后通牒博弈任务中嗓音吸引力对决策的影响[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(8): 1402-1409. |
[6] | 曲佳晨, 贡喆. 信任水平存在性别差异吗?[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(12): 2236-2245. |
[7] | 贡喆, 唐玉洁, 刘昌. 信任博弈范式真的能测量信任吗?[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(1): 19-30. |
[8] | 高青林, 周媛. 计算模型视角下信任形成的心理和神经机制——基于信任博弈中投资者的角度[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(1): 178-189. |
[9] | 施媛媛. 全球化语境下的多元文化能力模型及其在跨文化合作中的应用[J]. 心理科学进展, 2020, 28(7): 1108-1117. |
[10] | 吴琴, 崔丽莹. 合作行为中的“眼睛效应”:解释机制与限制因素[J]. 心理科学进展, 2020, 28(6): 994-1003. |
[11] | 陈思静, 杨莎莎. 利他性惩罚的动机[J]. 心理科学进展, 2020, 28(11): 1901-1910. |
[12] | 苏彦捷, 谢东杰, 王笑楠. 认知控制在第三方惩罚中的作用[J]. 心理科学进展, 2019, 27(8): 1331-1343. |
[13] | 卑力添, 蒋柯, 李先春, 熊哲宏. 博弈论视角下的超扫描多人互动任务新模型[J]. 心理科学进展, 2019, 27(7): 1284-1296. |
[14] | 张旭凯, 尹航, 李鹏, 李红. 催产素对社会决策行为的影响[J]. 心理科学进展, 2018, 26(8): 1438-1449. |
[15] | 张慧, 马红宇, 徐富明, 刘燕君, 史燕伟. 最后通牒博弈中的公平偏好:基于双系统理论的视角[J]. 心理科学进展, 2018, 26(2): 319-330. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||