心理科学进展 ›› 2019, Vol. 27 ›› Issue (10): 1703-1712.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2019.01703
收稿日期:
2019-01-21
出版日期:
2019-10-15
发布日期:
2019-08-19
通讯作者:
何蔚祺
E-mail:weiqi79920686@sina.com
基金资助:
DU Bixuan, ZHANG Mingming, ZHANG Keye, REN Jie, HE Weiqi()
Received:
2019-01-21
Online:
2019-10-15
Published:
2019-08-19
Contact:
HE Weiqi
E-mail:weiqi79920686@sina.com
摘要:
肢体是社会性信息的重要载体。研究肢体表征的早期发展对于揭示其肢体表征加工的内在机制, 进一步理解婴儿的社会性发展具有重要意义。从肢体形状和肢体动作两个方面来看, 已有研究主要考察了婴儿在处理肢体结构信息和运动信息时的表征方式, 并提出了婴儿肢体表征发展机制的两种理论。未来应进一步明确婴儿肢体表征的具体发展过程及关键期, 并通过脑成像技术探究生物运动偏好对于肢体表征的作用机制。
中图分类号:
杜碧煊, 张明明, 张珂烨, 任杰, 何蔚祺. (2019). 肢体形状和肢体动作表征的早期发展. 心理科学进展 , 27(10), 1703-1712.
DU Bixuan, ZHANG Mingming, ZHANG Keye, REN Jie, HE Weiqi. (2019). Early development of the body shape and body movement in infancy. Advances in Psychological Science, 27(10), 1703-1712.
[1] | 蒋毅, 王莉 . ( 2011). 生物运动加工特异性:整体结构和局部运动的作用. 心理科学进展, 19( 3), 301-311. |
[2] | 张珂烨, 张明明, 刘田田, 罗文波, 何蔚祺 . ( 2019). 肢体识别的倒置效应. 心理科学进展, 27( 1), 27-36. |
[3] | Alexander G. M., Hawkins L. B., Wilcox T., & Hirshkowitz A . ( 2016). Infants prefer female body phenotypes; Infant girls prefer they have an hourglass shape. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 804. |
[4] | Aviezer H., Trope Y., & Todorov A . ( 2012). Body cues, not facial expressions, discriminate between intense positive and negative emotions. Science, 338( 6111), 1225. |
[5] | Axelsson E. L., Moore D. G., Murphy E. M., Goodwin J. E., & Clifford B. R . ( 2018). The role of bodies in infants' categorical representations of humans and non-human animals. Infant and Child Development, 27(6). |
[6] | Bachmann J., Munzert J., & Krüger B . ( 2018). Neural underpinnings of the perception of emotional states derived from biological human motion: A review of neuroimaging research. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1763. |
[7] | Bardi L., Regolin L., & Simion F . ( 2011). Biological motion preference in humans at birth: Role of dynamic and configural properties. Developmental Science, 14( 2), 353-359. |
[8] | Bertenthal B. I., Proffitt D. R., & Cutting J. E . ( 1984). Infant sensitivity to figural coherence in biomechanical motions. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 37( 2), 213-230. |
[9] | Bertenthal B. I., Proffitt D. R., & Kramer S. J . ( 1987). Perception of biomechanical motions by infants: implementation of various processing constraints. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13( 4), 577-585. |
[10] | Bhatt R. S., Hock A., White H., Jubran R., & Galati A . ( 2016). The development of body structure knowledge in infancy. Child Development Perspectives, 10( 1), 45-52. |
[11] | Bidet-Ildei C., Kitromilides E., Orliaguet J.-P., Pavlova M., & Gentaz E . ( 2014). Preference for point-light human biological motion in newborns: Contribution of translational displacement. Developmental Psychology, 50( 1), 113-120. |
[12] | Blake, R. ( 1993). Cats perceive biological motion. Psychological Science, 4( 1), 54-57 |
[13] | Booth A. E., Pinto J., & Bertenthal B. I . ( 2002). Perception of the symmetrical patterning of human gait by infants. Developmental Psychology, 38( 4), 554-563. |
[14] | Bottari D., Troje N. F., Ley P., Hense M., Kekunnaya R., & Röder B . ( 2015). The neural development of the biological motion processing system does not rely on early visual input. Cortex, 71, 359-367. |
[15] | Chang D. H. F., Ban H., Ikegaya Y., Fujita I., & Troje N. F . ( 2018). Cortical and subcortical responses to biological motion. Neuroimage, 174, 87-96. |
[16] | Chang D. H. F., & Troje, N. F. ( 2009). Characterizing global and local mechanisms in biological motion perception. Journal of Vision, 9( 5), 8 1-10. |
[17] | Christie T., & Slaughter V. ( 2010). Movement contributes to infants' recognition of the human form. Cognition, 114( 3), 329-337. |
[18] | de Gelder B., de Borst A. W., & Watson R . ( 2015). The perception of emotion in body expressions. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 6( 2), 149-158. |
[19] | Deen B., Richardson H., Dilks D. D., Takahashi A., Keil B., Wald L. L., .. Saxe R . ( 2017). Organization of high-level visual cortex in human infants. Nature Communications, 8, 13995. |
[20] | di Giorgio E., Loveland J. L., Mayer U., Rosa-Salva O., Versace E., & Vallortigara G . ( 2017). Filial responses as predisposed and learned preferences: Early attachment in chicks and babies. Behavioural Brain Research, 325, 90-104. |
[21] | Fox R., & McDaniel C. ( 1982). The perception of biological motion by human infants. Science, 218( 4571), 486-487. |
[22] | Gliga T., & Dehaene-Lambertz G. ( 2005). Structural encoding of body and face in human infants and adults. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17( 8), 1328-1340. |
[23] | Greenough W. T., Black J. E., & Wallace C. S . ( 1987). Experience and brain development. Child Development, 58( 3), 539-559. |
[24] | Harris A., Vyas D. B., & Reed C. L . ( 2016). Holistic processing for bodies and body parts: New evidence from stereoscopic depth manipulations. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23( 5), 1513-1519. |
[25] | Heron-Delaney M., Wirth S., & Pascalis O . ( 2011). Infants' knowledge of their own species. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences, 366( 1571), 1753-1763. |
[26] | Heron M., & Slaughter V. ( 2010). Infants' responses to real humans and representations of humans. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 34( 1), 34-45. |
[27] | Hock A., Kangas A., Zieber N., & Bhatt R. S . ( 2015). The development of sex category representation in infancy: Matching of faces and bodies. Developmental Psychology, 51( 3), 346-352. |
[28] | Hock A., White H., Jubran R., & Bhatt R. S . ( 2016). The whole picture: Holistic body posture recognition in infancy. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23( 2), 426-431. |
[29] | Israelashvili J., Hassin R. R., & Aviezer H . ( 2018). When emotions run high: A critical role for context in the unfolding of dynamic, real-life facial affect. Emotion, in press. |
[30] | Johansson, G. ( 1973). Visual-perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis. Perception & Psychophysics, 14( 2), 201-211. |
[31] | Kirby L. A., Moraczewski D., Warnell K., Velnoskey K., & Redcay E . ( 2018). Social network size relates to developmental neural sensitivity to biological motion. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 30, 169-177. |
[32] | Longhi E., Senna I., Bolognini N., Bulf H., Tagliabue P., Cassia V. M., & Turati C . ( 2015). Discrimination of biomechanically possible and impossible hand movements at birth. Child Development, 86( 2), 632-641. |
[33] | Lorenzi E., Mayer U., Rosa-Salva O., & Vallortigara G . ( 2017). Dynamic features of animate motion activate septal and preoptic areas in visually naïve chicks (gallus gallus). Neuroscience, 354, 54-68. |
[34] | Marinović V., Hoehl S., & Pauen S . ( 2014). Neural correlates of human-animal distinction: An ERP-study on early categorical differentiation with 4- and 7-month-old infants and adults. Neuropsychologia, 60, 60-76. |
[35] | Marshall P. J., & Meltzoff, A. N. ( 2015). Body maps in the infant brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19( 9), 499-505. |
[36] | Marshall P. J., & Shipley, T. F. ( 2009). Event-related potentials to point-light displays of human actions in five-month-old infants. Developmental Neuropsychology, 34( 3), 368-377. |
[37] | Maurer D., Le Grand R., & Mondloch C. J . ( 2002). The many faces of configural processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6( 6), 255-260. |
[38] | Moore D. G., Goodwin J. E., George R., Axelsson E. L., & Braddick, F. M. B . ( 2007). Infants perceive human point-light displays as solid forms. Cognition, 104( 2), 377-396. |
[39] | Morita T., Slaughter V., Katayama N., Kitazaki M., Kakigi R., & Itakura S . ( 2012). Infant and adult perceptions of possible and impossible body movements: An eye-tracking study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 113( 3), 401-414. |
[40] | Morton J., & Johnson, M. H. ( 1991). Conspec and conlern: A 2-process theory of infant face recognition. Psychological Review, 98( 2), 164-181. |
[41] | Peelen M. V., & Downing, P. E. ( 2007). The neural basis of visual body perception. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8( 8), 636-648. |
[42] | Peelen M. V., Glaser B., Vuilleumier P., & Eliez S . ( 2009). Differential development of selectivity for faces and bodies in the fusiform gyrus. Developmental Science, 12( 6), F16-F25. |
[43] | Peykarjou S., Wissner J., & Pauen S . ( 2015). Categorical ERP repetition effects for human and furniture items in 7-month-old infants. Infant and Child Development, 26( 5), 1-12. |
[44] | Reed C. L., Stone V. E., Bozova S., & Tanaka J . ( 2003). The body-inversion effect. Psychological Science, 14( 4), 302-308. |
[45] | Reed C. L., Stone V. E., Grubb J. D., & McGoldrick J. E . ( 2006). Turning configural processing upside down: Part and whole body postures. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32( 1), 73-87. |
[46] | Reid V. M., Hoehl S., Landt J., & Striano T . ( 2008). Human infants dissociate structural and dynamic information in biological motion: Evidence from neural systems. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 3( 2), 161-167. |
[47] | Reid V. M., Hoehl S., & Striano T . ( 2006). The perception of biological motion by infants: An event-related potential study. Neuroscience Letters, 395( 3), 211-214 |
[48] | Reid V. M., Kaduk K., & Lunn J . ( 2019). Links between action perception and ac.tion production in 10-week-old infants. Neuropsychologia, 126, 69-74. |
[49] | Seitz, K. ( 2002). Parts and wholes in person recognition: Developmental trends. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 82( 4), 367-381. |
[50] | Shi Y., Ma X., Ma Z., Wang J., Yao N., Gu Q., .. Gao Z . ( 2018). Using a kinect sensor to acquire biological motion: Toolbox and evaluation. Behavior Research Methods, 50( 2), 518-529 |
[51] | Sifre R., Olson L., Gillespie S., Klin A., Jones W., & Shultz S . ( 2018). A longitudinal investigation of preferential attention to biological motion in 2- to 24-month-old infants. Scientific Reports, 8( 1), 2527. |
[52] | Simion F., Regolin L., & Bulf H . ( 2008). A predisposition for biological motion in the newborn baby. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105( 2), 809-813. |
[53] | Slaughter V., & Heron M. ( 2004). Origins and early development of human body knowledge. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 69( 2), 1-102. |
[54] | Slaughter V., Heron M., & Sim S . ( 2002). Development of preferences for the human body shape in infancy. Cognition, 85( 3), B71-B81. |
[55] | Stekelenburg J. J., & de Gelder , B. ( 2004). The neural correlates of perceiving human bodies: An ERP study on the body-inversion effect. Neuroreport, 15( 5), 777-780. |
[56] | Taylor J. C., Wiggett A. J., & Downing P. E . ( 2007). Functional MRI analysis of body and body part representations in the extrastriate and fusiform body areas. Journal of Neurophysiology, 98( 3), 1626-1633. |
[57] | Wenzler S., Levine S., van Dick R., Oertel-Knöchel V., & Aviezer H . ( 2016). Beyond pleasure and pain: Facial expression ambiguity in adults and children during intense situations. Emotion, 16( 6), 807-814. |
[58] | White H., Hock A., Jubran R., Heck A., & Bhatt R. S . ( 2018). Visual scanning of male and female bodies in infancy. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 166, 79-95. |
[59] | White H., Jubran R., Heck A., Chroust A., & Bhatt R. S . ( 2019). Sex-specific scanning in infancy: Developmental changes in the use of face/head and body information. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 182, 126-143. |
[60] | Zieber N., Bhatt R. S., Hayden A., Kangas A., Collins R., & Bada H . ( 2010). Body representation in the first year of life. Infancy, 15( 5), 534-544. |
[61] | Zieber N., Kangas A., Hock A., & Bhatt R. S . ( 2015). Body structure perception in infancy. Infancy, 20( 1), 1-17. |
[1] | 程羽慧, 袁祥勇, 蒋毅. 社会互动加工的认知特性及脑机制——第三人称的视角[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(3): 472-480. |
[2] | 魏华, 汪涛, 毛磊, 冯文婷, 熊莎莎. 叠音品牌名称对消费者知觉和态度的影响[J]. 心理科学进展, 2020, 28(7): 1071-1082. |
[3] | 毕小彬, 范晓壮, 米文丽, 贺荟中. 高风险婴儿前瞻性纵向研究与孤独症谱系障碍早期识别[J]. 心理科学进展, 2020, 28(3): 443-455. |
[4] | 刘慧晨, 陈坚. 婴儿的痛觉[J]. 心理科学进展, 2020, 28(10): 1723-1732. |
[5] | 程刚, 贾云丞, 丁芳媛, 张大均, 陈加, 龙女. 表情对婴儿面孔图式效应的调节及其机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2019, 27(5): 761-772. |
[6] | 张珂烨, 张明明, 刘田田, 罗文波, 何蔚祺. 肢体识别的倒置效应[J]. 心理科学进展, 2019, 27(1): 27-36. |
[7] | 杨伟星;张明亮;李红霞;杨雅琳;司继伟. 人类基本数学能力的进化证据[J]. 心理科学进展, 2017, 25(5): 810-824. |
[8] | 黄琪钰;刘秀丽. 婴儿是否拥有错误信念理解能力?[J]. 心理科学进展, 2017, 25(3): 431-442. |
[9] | 钱浩悦;朱敏;高湘萍. 老年人面孔构形加工特点[J]. 心理科学进展, 2017, 25(2): 230-236. |
[10] | 许少月, 王争艳. 婴儿期外显问题行为的发生与表现及成因分析[J]. 心理科学进展, 2017, 25(11): 1910-1921. |
[11] | 伍珍, 郭睿. 婴儿指示性手势与其语言学习的关系[J]. 心理科学进展, 2017, 25(10): 1705-1712. |
[12] | 周玉;张丹丹. 婴儿情绪与社会认知相关的听觉加工[J]. 心理科学进展, 2017, 25(1): 67-75. |
[13] | 李琎; 范伟; 张文洁; 钟毅平. 旋转自我面孔识别及其认知机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2016, 24(Suppl.): 50-. |
[14] | 窦东徽;刘肖岑;张玉洁. 娃娃脸效应:对婴儿面孔的偏好及过度泛化[J]. 心理科学进展, 2014, 22(5): 760-771. |
[15] | 皮忠玲;莫书亮. 婴儿心理理论的发展:表现和机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2013, 21(8): 1408-1421. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||