心理科学进展 ›› 2022, Vol. 30 ›› Issue (3): 556-579.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2022.00556
收稿日期:
2020-11-16
出版日期:
2022-03-15
发布日期:
2022-01-25
通讯作者:
李敏谊
E-mail:minyili@bnu.edu.cn
Received:
2020-11-16
Online:
2022-03-15
Published:
2022-01-25
Contact:
LI Minyi
E-mail:minyili@bnu.edu.cn
摘要:
家庭读写环境(home literacy environment, HLE)与儿童接受性词汇(receptive vocabulary)发展的关系一直备受关注, 但HLE内涵不清、各指标效应值强度不明, 以及近年来两者关系差别较大等问题极大地限制了人们对该领域的认识。本文运用元分析技术对近30年国内外84篇相关实证研究进行分析。结果显示:HLE与儿童接受性词汇发展为中等程度正相关(r = 0.31)。针对年代、文化背景、儿童年龄以及测量方法的调节效应检验表明:HLE效应值随年代发展显著降低, 但其核心指标亲子阅读频率的效应值基本稳定; 评估HLE的问卷法和现场观察法效应值无差异, 但评估亲子阅读频率的书目清单法效应值显著高于问卷法。未见文化背景和儿童年龄的显著调节作用, 原因值得进一步探究。后续研究应完善HLE的概念框架, 更关注社会经济及文化视角下的概念建构以及测量改进。
中图分类号:
刘海丹, 李敏谊. (2022). 家庭读写环境与儿童接受性词汇发展关系的元分析. 心理科学进展 , 30(3), 556-579.
LIU Haidan, LI Minyi. (2022). Associations between home literacy environment and children’s receptive vocabulary: A meta-analysis. Advances in Psychological Science, 30(3), 556-579.
时期 | 作者/年份 | 国家 | 问卷/量表 | 测量方法 | 理论 视角 | 指标 | 说明 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
20世纪60年代到80/90年代 | Taylor ( | 美国 | 《家庭读写实践清单》(Familia Inventory) | 问卷 | 互动理论 | 亲子阅读频率、亲子阅读时长、儿童开始阅读的年龄、儿童阅读兴趣、自主阅读频率等 | 主要围绕亲子阅读。 |
Bradley和 Caldwell ( | 美国 | 《家庭》 (the Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment) | 现场 观察 | 人类发展生态学理论 | 学习资料、物质环境、示范、语言刺激、学业刺激、回应性、多样性、接纳性 | ||
时期 | 作者/年份 | 国家 | 问卷/量表 | 测量方法 | 理论 视角 | 指标 | 说明 |
20世纪90年代初期到21世纪初期 | Whitehurst ( | 美国 | 《石溪家庭阅读调查表》(Stony Brook Family Reading Survey) | 问卷 | 人类发展生态学理论 | 藏书量、亲子阅读频率、亲子阅读时长、儿童开始阅读的年龄 | 基本形成读写资源、动机氛围、读写活动的主体框架。 |
Griffin和 Morrison ( | 美国 | 《家庭读写环境量表》(Family Literacy Environment Scale) | 问卷 | 人类发展生态学理论 | 藏书量、杂志和报纸数量、父母读写习惯、儿童看电视时长、去图书馆频率、亲子阅读频率 | ||
Heath等 ( | 美国 | 《家庭学习环境概况核查》(Home Learning Environment Profile) | 问卷 | 人类发展生态学理论 | 父母期望、父母参与、父母职责、儿童自主活动、儿童参与10项活动的时长、儿童参与其他活动的时长 | ||
Stipek等 ( | 美国 | 《斯帝佩克家庭学习活动调查表》(Stipek Home Learning Activities) | 问卷 | 互动理论 | 听讲故事、玩玩具、讨论幼儿园的事情等; 教字母和单词, 教数数或运算等 | ||
Sénéchal等 ( | 加拿大 | “家庭读写模型”(Home Literacy Model) | 问卷 | 互动理论 | 非正式读写活动(亲子阅读)、正式读写活动(教读写) | ||
21世纪 初期至今 | Teepe等 ( | 美国 | 改编《家庭读写环境量表》 | 问卷 | 人类发展生态学理论 | 丰富读写活动指标, 如教育类游戏(拼图等)、在电脑或平板上玩教育类游戏、看教育类电视 | 在上述框架基础上丰富相关指标, 主要是非正式读写活动。 |
Buhs等 ( | 美国 | 改编《家庭读写实践清单》 | 问卷 | 人类发展生态学理论 | 从亲子阅读延伸到三大方面, 分别是父母参与家庭学习活动、父母参与学校活动、儿童接触读写材料总量 | ||
Deckner等 ( | 美国 | 改编《石溪家庭阅读调查表》 | 问卷 | 人类发展生态学理论 | 增加“过去一周亲子阅读总时长” | ||
Burgess等 ( | 美国 | 改编《石溪家庭阅读调查表》 | 问卷 | 人类发展生态学理论 | 划分为主动HLE和被动HLE。主动HLE包括:亲子阅读频率、亲子阅读时长、藏书量、去图书馆频率、儿童开始阅读的年龄; 被动HLE包括:父母阅读频率、父母阅读时长、父母藏书量、父母阅读兴趣、儿童看见父母阅读的频率 | ||
Kirby和 Hogan ( | 美国 | 自编问卷 | 问卷 | 混合取向 | 总藏书量、儿童藏书量、成人阅读频率、亲子阅读频率、积木等游戏活动频率、教字母/单词/发音等频率 | ||
Frijters等 ( | 美国 | 自编问卷 | 问卷 | 人类发展生态学理论 | 儿童藏书量、儿童开始阅读的年龄、亲子阅读频率、其他人和儿童的阅读频率、去图书馆频率 | ||
Niklas和 Schneider ( | 德国 | 自编问卷 | 问卷 | 人类发展生态学理论 | 订阅报纸、总藏书量、儿童藏书量、有图书馆卡、父母阅读频率、儿童开始阅读的年龄、儿童自主阅读频率、亲子阅读频率、去图书馆频率、父母看电视时长、儿童看电视时长 | ||
Krijnen等 ( | 荷兰 | 修正“家庭读写模型” | 问卷 | 互动理论 | 口头语言接触活动(如亲子阅读、请儿童讲故事)、口头语言教学活动(如教单词、请儿童重复新单词)、代码技能接触活动(如字母游戏、韵律诗)、代码技能教学活动(如认读字母表、写名字) |
表1 HLE评估问卷及量表
时期 | 作者/年份 | 国家 | 问卷/量表 | 测量方法 | 理论 视角 | 指标 | 说明 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
20世纪60年代到80/90年代 | Taylor ( | 美国 | 《家庭读写实践清单》(Familia Inventory) | 问卷 | 互动理论 | 亲子阅读频率、亲子阅读时长、儿童开始阅读的年龄、儿童阅读兴趣、自主阅读频率等 | 主要围绕亲子阅读。 |
Bradley和 Caldwell ( | 美国 | 《家庭》 (the Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment) | 现场 观察 | 人类发展生态学理论 | 学习资料、物质环境、示范、语言刺激、学业刺激、回应性、多样性、接纳性 | ||
时期 | 作者/年份 | 国家 | 问卷/量表 | 测量方法 | 理论 视角 | 指标 | 说明 |
20世纪90年代初期到21世纪初期 | Whitehurst ( | 美国 | 《石溪家庭阅读调查表》(Stony Brook Family Reading Survey) | 问卷 | 人类发展生态学理论 | 藏书量、亲子阅读频率、亲子阅读时长、儿童开始阅读的年龄 | 基本形成读写资源、动机氛围、读写活动的主体框架。 |
Griffin和 Morrison ( | 美国 | 《家庭读写环境量表》(Family Literacy Environment Scale) | 问卷 | 人类发展生态学理论 | 藏书量、杂志和报纸数量、父母读写习惯、儿童看电视时长、去图书馆频率、亲子阅读频率 | ||
Heath等 ( | 美国 | 《家庭学习环境概况核查》(Home Learning Environment Profile) | 问卷 | 人类发展生态学理论 | 父母期望、父母参与、父母职责、儿童自主活动、儿童参与10项活动的时长、儿童参与其他活动的时长 | ||
Stipek等 ( | 美国 | 《斯帝佩克家庭学习活动调查表》(Stipek Home Learning Activities) | 问卷 | 互动理论 | 听讲故事、玩玩具、讨论幼儿园的事情等; 教字母和单词, 教数数或运算等 | ||
Sénéchal等 ( | 加拿大 | “家庭读写模型”(Home Literacy Model) | 问卷 | 互动理论 | 非正式读写活动(亲子阅读)、正式读写活动(教读写) | ||
21世纪 初期至今 | Teepe等 ( | 美国 | 改编《家庭读写环境量表》 | 问卷 | 人类发展生态学理论 | 丰富读写活动指标, 如教育类游戏(拼图等)、在电脑或平板上玩教育类游戏、看教育类电视 | 在上述框架基础上丰富相关指标, 主要是非正式读写活动。 |
Buhs等 ( | 美国 | 改编《家庭读写实践清单》 | 问卷 | 人类发展生态学理论 | 从亲子阅读延伸到三大方面, 分别是父母参与家庭学习活动、父母参与学校活动、儿童接触读写材料总量 | ||
Deckner等 ( | 美国 | 改编《石溪家庭阅读调查表》 | 问卷 | 人类发展生态学理论 | 增加“过去一周亲子阅读总时长” | ||
Burgess等 ( | 美国 | 改编《石溪家庭阅读调查表》 | 问卷 | 人类发展生态学理论 | 划分为主动HLE和被动HLE。主动HLE包括:亲子阅读频率、亲子阅读时长、藏书量、去图书馆频率、儿童开始阅读的年龄; 被动HLE包括:父母阅读频率、父母阅读时长、父母藏书量、父母阅读兴趣、儿童看见父母阅读的频率 | ||
Kirby和 Hogan ( | 美国 | 自编问卷 | 问卷 | 混合取向 | 总藏书量、儿童藏书量、成人阅读频率、亲子阅读频率、积木等游戏活动频率、教字母/单词/发音等频率 | ||
Frijters等 ( | 美国 | 自编问卷 | 问卷 | 人类发展生态学理论 | 儿童藏书量、儿童开始阅读的年龄、亲子阅读频率、其他人和儿童的阅读频率、去图书馆频率 | ||
Niklas和 Schneider ( | 德国 | 自编问卷 | 问卷 | 人类发展生态学理论 | 订阅报纸、总藏书量、儿童藏书量、有图书馆卡、父母阅读频率、儿童开始阅读的年龄、儿童自主阅读频率、亲子阅读频率、去图书馆频率、父母看电视时长、儿童看电视时长 | ||
Krijnen等 ( | 荷兰 | 修正“家庭读写模型” | 问卷 | 互动理论 | 口头语言接触活动(如亲子阅读、请儿童讲故事)、口头语言教学活动(如教单词、请儿童重复新单词)、代码技能接触活动(如字母游戏、韵律诗)、代码技能教学活动(如认读字母表、写名字) |
关键方面 | 具体指标 | |
---|---|---|
读写资源 | 藏书量、学习材料(书籍、游戏材料、磁带)、图书馆卡 | |
动机氛围 | 父母读写频率、父母读写时长、父母读写兴趣 | |
父母读写信念 | ||
读写活动 | 与阅读相关的其他指标 | 儿童开始阅读的年龄 |
儿童阅读兴趣 | ||
自主阅读频率 | ||
儿童请求阅读的频率 | ||
亲子阅读时长 | ||
非正式读写活动 | 亲子阅读频率 | |
其他学习活动(如唱歌、绘画、游戏) | ||
看电视频次(教育类或非教育类节目) | ||
外出活动频次(以去图书馆为典型代表) | ||
正式读写活动 | 教字母、拼读、单词、认字、写字的频率 |
表2 HLE效应值提取框架
关键方面 | 具体指标 | |
---|---|---|
读写资源 | 藏书量、学习材料(书籍、游戏材料、磁带)、图书馆卡 | |
动机氛围 | 父母读写频率、父母读写时长、父母读写兴趣 | |
父母读写信念 | ||
读写活动 | 与阅读相关的其他指标 | 儿童开始阅读的年龄 |
儿童阅读兴趣 | ||
自主阅读频率 | ||
儿童请求阅读的频率 | ||
亲子阅读时长 | ||
非正式读写活动 | 亲子阅读频率 | |
其他学习活动(如唱歌、绘画、游戏) | ||
看电视频次(教育类或非教育类节目) | ||
外出活动频次(以去图书馆为典型代表) | ||
正式读写活动 | 教字母、拼读、单词、认字、写字的频率 |
论文信息a | 发表年份/数据收集 年份 | 样本量 | 年龄 | 国家 | 文化b | 相关系数 | 文献质量 | HLE测量方法c | 亲子阅读频率测量方法d | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Payne | 236 | 4.4 | 美国 | 1 | HLE (0.42)、父母阅读时长(0.09)*、父母读写兴趣(0.00)*、藏书量(0.30)、儿童请求阅读频率(0.21)、儿童自主阅读频率(0.04)*、儿童开始阅读的年龄(-0.23)、亲子阅读频率(0.23)、亲子阅读时长(0.08)*、去图书馆频率(0.25) | 5 | 1 | 1 | ||
Whitehurst | 73 | 3.4 | 美国 | 1 | 藏书量(0.11)、儿童阅读兴趣(0.28) | 5 | ||||
Sénéchal | 32 | 4 | 加拿大 | 1 | 亲子阅读频率(0.39) | 6 | 1 | |||
48 | 4.8 | 亲子阅读频率(0.40) | 1 | |||||||
Sénéchal | 117 | 4.3 | 加拿大 | 1 | 藏书量(0.34)、父母读写频率(0.41)、儿童阅读兴趣(0.28)、儿童请求阅读频率(0.34)、儿童开始阅读的年龄(-0.07)、亲子阅读频率(0.40、0.24)、去图书馆频率(0.37) | 5 | 1/2 | |||
47 | 4 | 藏书量(0.37)、父母读写频率(0.43)、儿童请求阅读频率(0.19)、儿童开始阅读的年龄(-0.39)、亲子阅读频率(0.41、0.10)、去图书馆频率(0.37) | 1/2 | |||||||
Griffin | 295 | 5.4 | 美国 | 1 | HLE (0.63) | 7 | 1 | |||
Lee | 119 | 2.5 | 韩国 | 2 | 藏书量(0.38)、父母读写频率(0.23)、儿童开始阅读的年龄(-0.37)、亲子阅读时长(-0.08)*、正式读写活动(0.40)、 | 5 | ||||
Sénéchal | 110 | 5 | 加拿大 | 1 | 亲子阅读频率(0.48)、正式读写活动(0.34) | 7 | 2 | |||
58 | 6.4 | 亲子阅读频率(0.41)、正式读写活动(0.17) | 2 | |||||||
Dickinson | 47 | 4.4 | 美国 | 1 | HLE (0.38) | 6 | 1 | |||
Christian | 317 | 5.4 | 美国 | 1 | HLE (0.62) | 6 | 1 | |||
Frijters | 95 | 5.6 | 加拿大 | 1 | HLE (0.46)、藏书量(0.43)、儿童开始阅读的年龄(0.37)、亲子阅读频率(0.39、0.24)、去图书馆频率(0.19) | 8 | 1 | 1/2 | ||
Evans | 67 | 6 | 加拿大 | 1 | 亲子阅读频率(0.21)、正式读写(0.04) | 7 | 2 | |||
Roth | 66 | 5.4 | 美国 | 1 | HLE (0.33) | 3 | 1 | |||
Sénéchal | 93 | 4.4 | 加拿大 | 1 | 父母读写频率(0.20)、亲子阅读频率(0.38)、正式读写活动(0.15) | 8 | 2 | |||
Chamberlai-n, 硕 | 316 | 3.9 | 美国 | 1 | 藏书量(0.13)、父母读写频率(0.15)、儿童开始阅读的年龄(-0.05) | 3 | ||||
McTaggart, 博 | 65 | 5.3 | 加拿大 | 1 | 儿童阅读兴趣(-0.03)、亲子阅读频率(0.53)、正式读写活动(0.13) | 4 | 2 | |||
Daniels, 博 | 51 | 4.8 | 美国 | 1 | HLE (0.26)、儿童阅读兴趣(0.06) | 4 | 2 | |||
Roberts | 59 | 3 | 美国 | 1 | HLE (0.50)、儿童阅读兴趣(0.00)、亲子阅读频率(0.1) | 3 | 2 | 1 | ||
61 | 5 | HLE (0.46)、儿童阅读兴趣(0.24)、亲子阅读频率(0.21) | 2 | 1 | ||||||
Foster | 325 | 4.8 | 美国 | 1 | 亲子阅读频率(0.12)、其他学习活动(唱歌、游戏、手工) (-0.01)*、外出活动(0.02) | 8 | 1 | |||
论文信息a | 发表年份/数据收集 年份 | 样本量 | 年龄 | 国家 | 文化b | 相关系数 | 文献质量 | HLE测量方法c | 亲子阅读频率测量方法d | |
Farver | 122 | 3.6 | 美国 | 1 | 父母读写频率(0.26)、读写活动(0.25)、儿童阅读兴趣(0.38)、儿童开始阅读的年龄(0.03)、亲子阅读频率(0.24) | 9 | 2 | |||
Sénéchal | 90 | 6 | 加拿大 | 1 | 亲子阅读频率(0.20)、正式读写频率(0.13) | 5 | 1 | |||
Kelman, 博 | 91 | 4.4 | 美国 | 1 | 去图书馆频率(0.23) | 5 | ||||
Deckner | 53 | 2.5 | 美国 | 1 | 儿童阅读兴趣(0.09) | 3 | 1 | |||
Bonnett, 博 | 245 | 4 | 美国 | 1 | 藏书量(0.33)、儿童请求阅读频率(0.16)、正式读写活动(0.06) | 3 | ||||
Wu, 博 | 82 | 3.4 | 中国 | 2 | HLE (0.28) | 5 | 2 | |||
赵瑾东 | 113 | 4.3 | 中国 | 2 | 亲子阅读频率(0.37、0.20) | 4 | 1/2 | |||
Rodriguez, 博 | 1851 | 5.3 | 美国 | 1 | HLE (0.35) | 6 | 2 | |||
Stephenson | 61 | 5.4 | 加拿大 | 1 | 藏书量(-0.01)、父母读写信念(0.34、0.38、0.31)、亲子阅读频率(0.18)、正式读写活动(0.19) | 7 | 1 | |||
Bracken | 233 | 4.3 | 美国 | 1 | 父母读写兴趣(0.19)*、儿童阅读兴趣(0.23) | 7 | ||||
Hindman, 博 | 816 | 4.4 | 美国 | 1 | 亲子阅读频率(0.58) | 6 | 1 | |||
See, 博 | 2513 | 4.1 | 美国 | 1 | 亲子阅读频率(0.19)、去图书馆频率(0.12)、正式读写活动(0.07) | 6 | 1 | |||
Rodriguez | 1046 | 3 | 美国 | 1 | 藏书量(0.26)、读写活动(0.14) | 8 | ||||
Manolitsis | 77 | 5.5 | 加拿大 | 1 | 藏书量(0.00)、父母读写信念(0.32)、亲子阅读频率(0.17)、正式读写活动(0.17) | 9 | 1 | |||
95 | 5.5 | 希腊 | 1 | 藏书量(0.35)、父母读写信念(0.22)、亲子阅读频率(0.35)、正式读写活动(0.00) | 1 | |||||
Hubbs-Tait | 122 | 4.1 | 美国 | 1 | 藏书量(0.32)、去图书馆频率(0.27) | 6 | ||||
Chazan- Cohen | 1273 | 5 | 美国 | 1 | HLE (0.25) | 6 | 2 | |||
陈晓 | 96 | 6.4 | 中国 | 2 | 亲子阅读频率(0.39)、正式读写活动(0.15) | 5 | 2 | |||
63 | 7.4 | 亲子阅读频率(0.10)、正式读写活动(0.17) | 2 | |||||||
Buhs | 219 | 3.5 | 美国 | 1 | 藏书量(0.37)、读写活动(0.19) | 9 | ||||
Manolitsis | 70 | 5.5 | 希腊 | 1 | 正式读写活动(-0.05) | 6 | ||||
Davidse | 228 | 4.4 | 荷兰 | 1 | 父母读写频率(0.16)、亲子阅读频率(0.06) | 4 | 1 | |||
Suggate | 82 | 7.2 | 德国 | 1 | HLE (0.23) | 3 | 1 | |||
Ladd | 70 | 5.8 | 加拿大 | 1 | 父母读写频率(0.50)、亲子阅读频率(0.46) | 5 | 2 | |||
Baroody | 81 | 4.6 | 美国 | 1 | 父母读写频率(0.01) | 8 | ||||
Fielding- barnsley | 141 | 7 | 澳大 利亚 | 1 | 藏书量(0.47)、儿童请求阅读频率(0.40)、儿童自主阅读频率(0.17)*、儿童开始阅读的年龄(0.33)、亲子阅读频率(0.43) | 6 | 1 | |||
Aram | 78 | 4.4 | 以色列 | 1 | 亲子阅读频率(0.18)、读写活动(0.19) | 2 | 1 | |||
Bulotsky- Shearer | 1770 | 4.1 | 美国 | 1 | 外出活动(0.04) | 8 | ||||
林珮伃 | 309 | 6.7 | 中国 台湾 | 2 | HLE (0.10) | 9 | 1 | |||
论文信息a | 发表年份/数据收集 年份 | 样本量 | 年龄 | 国家 | 文化b | 相关系数 | 文献质量 | HLE测量方法c | 亲子阅读频率测量方法d | |
Sparks | 60 | 4.8 | 美国 | 1 | 亲子阅读频率(-0.04)、儿童请求阅读频率(0.15)、正式读写活动(-0.07) | 5 | 1 | |||
Korat | 109 | 5.4 | 以色列 | 1 | HLE (0.18) | 7 | 1 | 1 | ||
Crystal, 博 | 146 | 4 | 美国 | 1 | 儿童阅读兴趣(0.00)、去图书馆频率(0.13)、正式读写(0.02) | 4 | ||||
Aram | 88 | 5.6 | 以色列 | 1 | 亲子阅读频率(0.03) | 3 | 1 | |||
Froiland | 551 | 4 | 美国 | 1 | 藏书量(0.24)、亲子阅读频率(0.14) | 5 | 1 | |||
Lehrl, 书 | 343 | 8.2 | 德国 | 1 | 正式读写活动(-0.04) | 4 | ||||
Torppa | 1006 | 6.2 | 芬兰 | 1 | 亲子阅读频率(0.15)、正式读写(-0.05) | 5 | 1 | |||
Sénéchal | 110 | 5 | 加拿大 | 1 | 父母读写频率(0.20)、亲子阅读频率(0.31)、正式读写(0.08) | 9 | 2 | |||
96 | 6 | 父母读写频率(0.13)、亲子阅读频率(0.40)、正式读写(0.11) | 2 | |||||||
84 | 7 | 父母读写频率(0.13)、亲子阅读频率(0.09)、正式读写(-0.13) | 2 | |||||||
Skwarchuk | 121 | 6 | 加拿大 | 1 | 亲子阅读频率(0.33)、正式读写(0.12) | 10 | 2 | |||
Mol | 60 | 5.5 | 美国 | 1 | 藏书量(4.5) | 8 | ||||
Iruka | 4450 | 4.4 | 美国 | 1 | 藏书量(0.05)、外出活动(0.14) | 7 | ||||
1750 | 藏书量(0.13)、外出活动(-0.01) | |||||||||
2200 | 藏书量(0.20)、外出活动(0.09) | |||||||||
1250 | 藏书量(0.24)、外出活动(0.09) | |||||||||
Meng | 2611 | 4 | 美国 | 1 | HLE (0.10) | 7 | 1 | |||
Wang, 博 | 103 | 4.4 | 中国 | 2 | HLE (0.26) | 8 | 2 | |||
Willard | 119 | 6 | 德国 | 1 | HLE (0.19) | 8 | 2 | |||
121 | 10 | HLE (0.09) | 2 | |||||||
晏盈盈, 硕 | 131 | 3.4 | 中国 | 2 | 藏书量(0.23)、亲子阅读频率(0.28)、亲子阅读时长(0.16)* | 3 | 1 | |||
Williams | 3031 | 4.5 | 澳大 利亚 | 1 | 亲子阅读频率(0.22) | 6 | 1 | |||
Bingham | 181 | 4.2 | 美国 | 1 | 正式读写活动(-0.22) | 7 | 1 | |||
王娟 | 154 | 5.3 | 中国 | 2 | HLE (0.36) | 6 | 1 | |||
Gonzalez | 252 | 4.6 | 美国 | 1 | HLE (0.11)、藏书量(0.23)、父母读写信念(0.13)、亲子阅读频率(0.19) | 9 | 1 | 1 | ||
Chansa- Kabali | 72 | 7 | 赞比亚 | 3 | 藏书量(0.23)、正式读写活动(0.28) | 9 | 1 | |||
Iruka | 800 | 3 | 美国 | 1 | HLE (0.22)、亲子阅读频率(0.08) | 4 | 2 | 1 | ||
Zhang | 147 | 6 | 中国 | 2 | 藏书量(0.24)、亲子阅读频率(0.24、0.19) | 10 | 1/2 | |||
Bojczyk | 112 | 4.6 | 美国 | 1 | HLE (0.27)、读写活动(0.38) | 9 | 1 | |||
Inoue | 2018 | 214 | 5.5 | 美国 | 1 | 藏书量(0.19)、亲子阅读频率(0.31)、正式读写(0.15) | 7 | 1 | ||
论文信息a | 发表年份/数据收集 年份 | 样本量 | 年龄 | 国家 | 文化b | 相关系数 | 文献质量 | HLE测量方法c | 亲子阅读频率测量方法d | |
Shahaeian | 4768 | 4.4 | 澳大 利亚 | 1 | 藏书量(0.32)、亲子阅读频率(0.32) | 5 | 1 | |||
Lohndorf | 77 | 3.4 | 智利 | 1 | HLE (0.28) | 5 | 1 | |||
Hayes | 3836 | 6.8 | 澳大 利亚 | 1 | 亲子阅读频率(0.10) | 6 | 1 | |||
Schmerse | 1331 | 2.5 | 德国 | 1 | HLE (0.35) | 5 | 2 | |||
Kluczniok | 2406 | 4 | 德国 | 1 | 外出活动(0.21) | 7 | ||||
Ren | 656 | 5 | 中国 | 2 | 正式读写(0.05) | 8 | ||||
Schlesinger | 68 | 3.3 | 美国 | 1 | 亲子阅读频率(0.23) | 5 | 1 | |||
Burris | 256 | 4 | 美国 | 1 | 藏书量(0.22)、父母阅读频率(-0.09)、儿童开始阅读的年龄(0.00)、亲子阅读频率(0.07)、去图书馆频率(0.03) | 5 | 2 | |||
Lambrecht | 2996 | 5 | 德国 | 1 | 藏书量(0.23) | 4 | 1 | |||
Bojczyk | 98 | 3 | 美国 | 1 | HLE (0.33)、读写活动(0.01) | 7 | 1 | |||
100 | 6 | HLE (0.12)、读写活动(-0.04) | 1 | |||||||
Korucu | 3273 | 3 | 美国 | 1 | HLE (0.32) | 5 | 2 | |||
2020/2004 | 2021 | 5 | HLE (0.34) | 2 | ||||||
Xiao | 105 | 5 | 中国 | 2 | 正式读写活动(0.17) | 7 | 1 | |||
Silinskas | 1839 | 7 | 芬兰 | 1 | 亲子阅读频率(0.19)、正式读写活动(0.04) | 5 | 1 | |||
Krijnen | 214 | 4.6 | 荷兰 | 1 | 亲子阅读频率(0.25) | 7 | 1 | |||
Ren | 588 | 5.1 | 中国 | 2 | 正式读写活动(0.06) | 10 |
表3 HLE与儿童接受性词汇关系的元分析编码表
论文信息a | 发表年份/数据收集 年份 | 样本量 | 年龄 | 国家 | 文化b | 相关系数 | 文献质量 | HLE测量方法c | 亲子阅读频率测量方法d | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Payne | 236 | 4.4 | 美国 | 1 | HLE (0.42)、父母阅读时长(0.09)*、父母读写兴趣(0.00)*、藏书量(0.30)、儿童请求阅读频率(0.21)、儿童自主阅读频率(0.04)*、儿童开始阅读的年龄(-0.23)、亲子阅读频率(0.23)、亲子阅读时长(0.08)*、去图书馆频率(0.25) | 5 | 1 | 1 | ||
Whitehurst | 73 | 3.4 | 美国 | 1 | 藏书量(0.11)、儿童阅读兴趣(0.28) | 5 | ||||
Sénéchal | 32 | 4 | 加拿大 | 1 | 亲子阅读频率(0.39) | 6 | 1 | |||
48 | 4.8 | 亲子阅读频率(0.40) | 1 | |||||||
Sénéchal | 117 | 4.3 | 加拿大 | 1 | 藏书量(0.34)、父母读写频率(0.41)、儿童阅读兴趣(0.28)、儿童请求阅读频率(0.34)、儿童开始阅读的年龄(-0.07)、亲子阅读频率(0.40、0.24)、去图书馆频率(0.37) | 5 | 1/2 | |||
47 | 4 | 藏书量(0.37)、父母读写频率(0.43)、儿童请求阅读频率(0.19)、儿童开始阅读的年龄(-0.39)、亲子阅读频率(0.41、0.10)、去图书馆频率(0.37) | 1/2 | |||||||
Griffin | 295 | 5.4 | 美国 | 1 | HLE (0.63) | 7 | 1 | |||
Lee | 119 | 2.5 | 韩国 | 2 | 藏书量(0.38)、父母读写频率(0.23)、儿童开始阅读的年龄(-0.37)、亲子阅读时长(-0.08)*、正式读写活动(0.40)、 | 5 | ||||
Sénéchal | 110 | 5 | 加拿大 | 1 | 亲子阅读频率(0.48)、正式读写活动(0.34) | 7 | 2 | |||
58 | 6.4 | 亲子阅读频率(0.41)、正式读写活动(0.17) | 2 | |||||||
Dickinson | 47 | 4.4 | 美国 | 1 | HLE (0.38) | 6 | 1 | |||
Christian | 317 | 5.4 | 美国 | 1 | HLE (0.62) | 6 | 1 | |||
Frijters | 95 | 5.6 | 加拿大 | 1 | HLE (0.46)、藏书量(0.43)、儿童开始阅读的年龄(0.37)、亲子阅读频率(0.39、0.24)、去图书馆频率(0.19) | 8 | 1 | 1/2 | ||
Evans | 67 | 6 | 加拿大 | 1 | 亲子阅读频率(0.21)、正式读写(0.04) | 7 | 2 | |||
Roth | 66 | 5.4 | 美国 | 1 | HLE (0.33) | 3 | 1 | |||
Sénéchal | 93 | 4.4 | 加拿大 | 1 | 父母读写频率(0.20)、亲子阅读频率(0.38)、正式读写活动(0.15) | 8 | 2 | |||
Chamberlai-n, 硕 | 316 | 3.9 | 美国 | 1 | 藏书量(0.13)、父母读写频率(0.15)、儿童开始阅读的年龄(-0.05) | 3 | ||||
McTaggart, 博 | 65 | 5.3 | 加拿大 | 1 | 儿童阅读兴趣(-0.03)、亲子阅读频率(0.53)、正式读写活动(0.13) | 4 | 2 | |||
Daniels, 博 | 51 | 4.8 | 美国 | 1 | HLE (0.26)、儿童阅读兴趣(0.06) | 4 | 2 | |||
Roberts | 59 | 3 | 美国 | 1 | HLE (0.50)、儿童阅读兴趣(0.00)、亲子阅读频率(0.1) | 3 | 2 | 1 | ||
61 | 5 | HLE (0.46)、儿童阅读兴趣(0.24)、亲子阅读频率(0.21) | 2 | 1 | ||||||
Foster | 325 | 4.8 | 美国 | 1 | 亲子阅读频率(0.12)、其他学习活动(唱歌、游戏、手工) (-0.01)*、外出活动(0.02) | 8 | 1 | |||
论文信息a | 发表年份/数据收集 年份 | 样本量 | 年龄 | 国家 | 文化b | 相关系数 | 文献质量 | HLE测量方法c | 亲子阅读频率测量方法d | |
Farver | 122 | 3.6 | 美国 | 1 | 父母读写频率(0.26)、读写活动(0.25)、儿童阅读兴趣(0.38)、儿童开始阅读的年龄(0.03)、亲子阅读频率(0.24) | 9 | 2 | |||
Sénéchal | 90 | 6 | 加拿大 | 1 | 亲子阅读频率(0.20)、正式读写频率(0.13) | 5 | 1 | |||
Kelman, 博 | 91 | 4.4 | 美国 | 1 | 去图书馆频率(0.23) | 5 | ||||
Deckner | 53 | 2.5 | 美国 | 1 | 儿童阅读兴趣(0.09) | 3 | 1 | |||
Bonnett, 博 | 245 | 4 | 美国 | 1 | 藏书量(0.33)、儿童请求阅读频率(0.16)、正式读写活动(0.06) | 3 | ||||
Wu, 博 | 82 | 3.4 | 中国 | 2 | HLE (0.28) | 5 | 2 | |||
赵瑾东 | 113 | 4.3 | 中国 | 2 | 亲子阅读频率(0.37、0.20) | 4 | 1/2 | |||
Rodriguez, 博 | 1851 | 5.3 | 美国 | 1 | HLE (0.35) | 6 | 2 | |||
Stephenson | 61 | 5.4 | 加拿大 | 1 | 藏书量(-0.01)、父母读写信念(0.34、0.38、0.31)、亲子阅读频率(0.18)、正式读写活动(0.19) | 7 | 1 | |||
Bracken | 233 | 4.3 | 美国 | 1 | 父母读写兴趣(0.19)*、儿童阅读兴趣(0.23) | 7 | ||||
Hindman, 博 | 816 | 4.4 | 美国 | 1 | 亲子阅读频率(0.58) | 6 | 1 | |||
See, 博 | 2513 | 4.1 | 美国 | 1 | 亲子阅读频率(0.19)、去图书馆频率(0.12)、正式读写活动(0.07) | 6 | 1 | |||
Rodriguez | 1046 | 3 | 美国 | 1 | 藏书量(0.26)、读写活动(0.14) | 8 | ||||
Manolitsis | 77 | 5.5 | 加拿大 | 1 | 藏书量(0.00)、父母读写信念(0.32)、亲子阅读频率(0.17)、正式读写活动(0.17) | 9 | 1 | |||
95 | 5.5 | 希腊 | 1 | 藏书量(0.35)、父母读写信念(0.22)、亲子阅读频率(0.35)、正式读写活动(0.00) | 1 | |||||
Hubbs-Tait | 122 | 4.1 | 美国 | 1 | 藏书量(0.32)、去图书馆频率(0.27) | 6 | ||||
Chazan- Cohen | 1273 | 5 | 美国 | 1 | HLE (0.25) | 6 | 2 | |||
陈晓 | 96 | 6.4 | 中国 | 2 | 亲子阅读频率(0.39)、正式读写活动(0.15) | 5 | 2 | |||
63 | 7.4 | 亲子阅读频率(0.10)、正式读写活动(0.17) | 2 | |||||||
Buhs | 219 | 3.5 | 美国 | 1 | 藏书量(0.37)、读写活动(0.19) | 9 | ||||
Manolitsis | 70 | 5.5 | 希腊 | 1 | 正式读写活动(-0.05) | 6 | ||||
Davidse | 228 | 4.4 | 荷兰 | 1 | 父母读写频率(0.16)、亲子阅读频率(0.06) | 4 | 1 | |||
Suggate | 82 | 7.2 | 德国 | 1 | HLE (0.23) | 3 | 1 | |||
Ladd | 70 | 5.8 | 加拿大 | 1 | 父母读写频率(0.50)、亲子阅读频率(0.46) | 5 | 2 | |||
Baroody | 81 | 4.6 | 美国 | 1 | 父母读写频率(0.01) | 8 | ||||
Fielding- barnsley | 141 | 7 | 澳大 利亚 | 1 | 藏书量(0.47)、儿童请求阅读频率(0.40)、儿童自主阅读频率(0.17)*、儿童开始阅读的年龄(0.33)、亲子阅读频率(0.43) | 6 | 1 | |||
Aram | 78 | 4.4 | 以色列 | 1 | 亲子阅读频率(0.18)、读写活动(0.19) | 2 | 1 | |||
Bulotsky- Shearer | 1770 | 4.1 | 美国 | 1 | 外出活动(0.04) | 8 | ||||
林珮伃 | 309 | 6.7 | 中国 台湾 | 2 | HLE (0.10) | 9 | 1 | |||
论文信息a | 发表年份/数据收集 年份 | 样本量 | 年龄 | 国家 | 文化b | 相关系数 | 文献质量 | HLE测量方法c | 亲子阅读频率测量方法d | |
Sparks | 60 | 4.8 | 美国 | 1 | 亲子阅读频率(-0.04)、儿童请求阅读频率(0.15)、正式读写活动(-0.07) | 5 | 1 | |||
Korat | 109 | 5.4 | 以色列 | 1 | HLE (0.18) | 7 | 1 | 1 | ||
Crystal, 博 | 146 | 4 | 美国 | 1 | 儿童阅读兴趣(0.00)、去图书馆频率(0.13)、正式读写(0.02) | 4 | ||||
Aram | 88 | 5.6 | 以色列 | 1 | 亲子阅读频率(0.03) | 3 | 1 | |||
Froiland | 551 | 4 | 美国 | 1 | 藏书量(0.24)、亲子阅读频率(0.14) | 5 | 1 | |||
Lehrl, 书 | 343 | 8.2 | 德国 | 1 | 正式读写活动(-0.04) | 4 | ||||
Torppa | 1006 | 6.2 | 芬兰 | 1 | 亲子阅读频率(0.15)、正式读写(-0.05) | 5 | 1 | |||
Sénéchal | 110 | 5 | 加拿大 | 1 | 父母读写频率(0.20)、亲子阅读频率(0.31)、正式读写(0.08) | 9 | 2 | |||
96 | 6 | 父母读写频率(0.13)、亲子阅读频率(0.40)、正式读写(0.11) | 2 | |||||||
84 | 7 | 父母读写频率(0.13)、亲子阅读频率(0.09)、正式读写(-0.13) | 2 | |||||||
Skwarchuk | 121 | 6 | 加拿大 | 1 | 亲子阅读频率(0.33)、正式读写(0.12) | 10 | 2 | |||
Mol | 60 | 5.5 | 美国 | 1 | 藏书量(4.5) | 8 | ||||
Iruka | 4450 | 4.4 | 美国 | 1 | 藏书量(0.05)、外出活动(0.14) | 7 | ||||
1750 | 藏书量(0.13)、外出活动(-0.01) | |||||||||
2200 | 藏书量(0.20)、外出活动(0.09) | |||||||||
1250 | 藏书量(0.24)、外出活动(0.09) | |||||||||
Meng | 2611 | 4 | 美国 | 1 | HLE (0.10) | 7 | 1 | |||
Wang, 博 | 103 | 4.4 | 中国 | 2 | HLE (0.26) | 8 | 2 | |||
Willard | 119 | 6 | 德国 | 1 | HLE (0.19) | 8 | 2 | |||
121 | 10 | HLE (0.09) | 2 | |||||||
晏盈盈, 硕 | 131 | 3.4 | 中国 | 2 | 藏书量(0.23)、亲子阅读频率(0.28)、亲子阅读时长(0.16)* | 3 | 1 | |||
Williams | 3031 | 4.5 | 澳大 利亚 | 1 | 亲子阅读频率(0.22) | 6 | 1 | |||
Bingham | 181 | 4.2 | 美国 | 1 | 正式读写活动(-0.22) | 7 | 1 | |||
王娟 | 154 | 5.3 | 中国 | 2 | HLE (0.36) | 6 | 1 | |||
Gonzalez | 252 | 4.6 | 美国 | 1 | HLE (0.11)、藏书量(0.23)、父母读写信念(0.13)、亲子阅读频率(0.19) | 9 | 1 | 1 | ||
Chansa- Kabali | 72 | 7 | 赞比亚 | 3 | 藏书量(0.23)、正式读写活动(0.28) | 9 | 1 | |||
Iruka | 800 | 3 | 美国 | 1 | HLE (0.22)、亲子阅读频率(0.08) | 4 | 2 | 1 | ||
Zhang | 147 | 6 | 中国 | 2 | 藏书量(0.24)、亲子阅读频率(0.24、0.19) | 10 | 1/2 | |||
Bojczyk | 112 | 4.6 | 美国 | 1 | HLE (0.27)、读写活动(0.38) | 9 | 1 | |||
Inoue | 2018 | 214 | 5.5 | 美国 | 1 | 藏书量(0.19)、亲子阅读频率(0.31)、正式读写(0.15) | 7 | 1 | ||
论文信息a | 发表年份/数据收集 年份 | 样本量 | 年龄 | 国家 | 文化b | 相关系数 | 文献质量 | HLE测量方法c | 亲子阅读频率测量方法d | |
Shahaeian | 4768 | 4.4 | 澳大 利亚 | 1 | 藏书量(0.32)、亲子阅读频率(0.32) | 5 | 1 | |||
Lohndorf | 77 | 3.4 | 智利 | 1 | HLE (0.28) | 5 | 1 | |||
Hayes | 3836 | 6.8 | 澳大 利亚 | 1 | 亲子阅读频率(0.10) | 6 | 1 | |||
Schmerse | 1331 | 2.5 | 德国 | 1 | HLE (0.35) | 5 | 2 | |||
Kluczniok | 2406 | 4 | 德国 | 1 | 外出活动(0.21) | 7 | ||||
Ren | 656 | 5 | 中国 | 2 | 正式读写(0.05) | 8 | ||||
Schlesinger | 68 | 3.3 | 美国 | 1 | 亲子阅读频率(0.23) | 5 | 1 | |||
Burris | 256 | 4 | 美国 | 1 | 藏书量(0.22)、父母阅读频率(-0.09)、儿童开始阅读的年龄(0.00)、亲子阅读频率(0.07)、去图书馆频率(0.03) | 5 | 2 | |||
Lambrecht | 2996 | 5 | 德国 | 1 | 藏书量(0.23) | 4 | 1 | |||
Bojczyk | 98 | 3 | 美国 | 1 | HLE (0.33)、读写活动(0.01) | 7 | 1 | |||
100 | 6 | HLE (0.12)、读写活动(-0.04) | 1 | |||||||
Korucu | 3273 | 3 | 美国 | 1 | HLE (0.32) | 5 | 2 | |||
2020/2004 | 2021 | 5 | HLE (0.34) | 2 | ||||||
Xiao | 105 | 5 | 中国 | 2 | 正式读写活动(0.17) | 7 | 1 | |||
Silinskas | 1839 | 7 | 芬兰 | 1 | 亲子阅读频率(0.19)、正式读写活动(0.04) | 5 | 1 | |||
Krijnen | 214 | 4.6 | 荷兰 | 1 | 亲子阅读频率(0.25) | 7 | 1 | |||
Ren | 588 | 5.1 | 中国 | 2 | 正式读写活动(0.06) | 10 |
HLE指标 | k | Nfs | Egger’s regression | Trim and fill | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
截距 | p | ||||
HLE | 29 | 7222 | 0.67 | 0.50 | [0.32, 0.45] |
读写资源(藏书量) | 30 | 5784 | 1.18 | 0.17 | [0.21, 0.31] |
动机氛围 | |||||
父母读写频率 | 13 | 185 | 3.79 | 0.06 | [0.11, 0.30] |
父母读写信念 | 6 | 55 | 3.36 | 0.12 | [0.15, 0.29] |
与阅读相关的其它指标 | |||||
儿童阅读兴趣 | 10 | 58 | -1.68 | 0.37 | [0.07, 0.27] |
儿童请求阅读频率 | 6 | 69 | 0.56 | 0.80 | [0.16, 0.35 ] |
儿童开始阅读的年龄 | 9 | 0 | -1.75 | 0.02 | [-0.10, 0.22 ] |
读写活动 | 7 | 59 | 0.36 | 0.80 | [0.08, 0.26] |
非正式读写活动 | |||||
亲子阅读频率 | 55 | 1915 | 0.59 | 0.29 | [0.22, 0.30 ] |
去图书馆频率 | 9 | 170 | 1.60 | 0.06 | [0.07, 0.22 ] |
外出活动频率 | 7 | 211 | -4.85 | 0.28 | [0.03, 0.15] |
正式读写活动 | 31 | 219 | 0.73 | 0.13 | [0.01, 0.08] |
表4 出版偏误检验结果
HLE指标 | k | Nfs | Egger’s regression | Trim and fill | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
截距 | p | ||||
HLE | 29 | 7222 | 0.67 | 0.50 | [0.32, 0.45] |
读写资源(藏书量) | 30 | 5784 | 1.18 | 0.17 | [0.21, 0.31] |
动机氛围 | |||||
父母读写频率 | 13 | 185 | 3.79 | 0.06 | [0.11, 0.30] |
父母读写信念 | 6 | 55 | 3.36 | 0.12 | [0.15, 0.29] |
与阅读相关的其它指标 | |||||
儿童阅读兴趣 | 10 | 58 | -1.68 | 0.37 | [0.07, 0.27] |
儿童请求阅读频率 | 6 | 69 | 0.56 | 0.80 | [0.16, 0.35 ] |
儿童开始阅读的年龄 | 9 | 0 | -1.75 | 0.02 | [-0.10, 0.22 ] |
读写活动 | 7 | 59 | 0.36 | 0.80 | [0.08, 0.26] |
非正式读写活动 | |||||
亲子阅读频率 | 55 | 1915 | 0.59 | 0.29 | [0.22, 0.30 ] |
去图书馆频率 | 9 | 170 | 1.60 | 0.06 | [0.07, 0.22 ] |
外出活动频率 | 7 | 211 | -4.85 | 0.28 | [0.03, 0.15] |
正式读写活动 | 31 | 219 | 0.73 | 0.13 | [0.01, 0.08] |
HLE指标 | k | Heterogeneity | Tau-squared | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Q组内 | df | p | I2 | Tau-squared | SE | Variance | Tau | ||
HLE | 29 | 296.26 | 28 | 0.00 | 90.55 | 0.02 | 0.01 | 0.00 | 0.14 |
读写资源(藏书量) | 30 | 261.84 | 29 | 0.00 | 68.70 | 0.01 | 0.01 | 0.00 | 0.11 |
动机氛围 | |||||||||
父母读写频率 | 13 | 42.64 | 12 | 0.00 | 71.86 | 0.02 | 0.01 | 0.00 | 0.14 |
父母读写信念 | 6 | 5.28 | 5 | 0.28 | 19.78 | 0.00 | 0.01 | 0.00 | 0.05 |
与阅读相关的其它指标 | |||||||||
儿童阅读兴趣 | 10 | 18.96 | 9 | 0.03 | 52.53 | 0.01 | 0.01 | 0.00 | 0.11 |
儿童请求阅读频率 | 6 | 8.40 | 5 | 0.14 | 40.44 | 0.01 | 0.01 | 0.00 | 0.07 |
儿童开始阅读的年龄 | 9 | 67.23 | 8 | 0.00 | 88.10 | 0.05 | 0.03 | 0.00 | 0.22 |
读写活动 | 7 | 14.63 | 6 | 0.02 | 58.93 | 0.01 | 0.01 | 0.00 | 0.09 |
非正式读写活动 | |||||||||
亲子阅读频率 | 55 | 393.21 | 54 | 0.00 | 86.27 | 0.02 | 0.01 | 0.00 | 0.12 |
去图书馆频率 | 9 | 20.00 | 8 | 0.01 | 60.00 | 0.01 | 0.01 | 0.00 | 0.08 |
外出活动频率 | 7 | 67.45 | 6 | 0.00 | 71.11 | 0.01 | 0.00 | 0.00 | 0.07 |
正式读写活动 | 31 | 73.36 | 30 | 0.00 | 59.11 | 0.01 | 0.00 | 0.00 | 0.07 |
表5 异质性检验结果
HLE指标 | k | Heterogeneity | Tau-squared | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Q组内 | df | p | I2 | Tau-squared | SE | Variance | Tau | ||
HLE | 29 | 296.26 | 28 | 0.00 | 90.55 | 0.02 | 0.01 | 0.00 | 0.14 |
读写资源(藏书量) | 30 | 261.84 | 29 | 0.00 | 68.70 | 0.01 | 0.01 | 0.00 | 0.11 |
动机氛围 | |||||||||
父母读写频率 | 13 | 42.64 | 12 | 0.00 | 71.86 | 0.02 | 0.01 | 0.00 | 0.14 |
父母读写信念 | 6 | 5.28 | 5 | 0.28 | 19.78 | 0.00 | 0.01 | 0.00 | 0.05 |
与阅读相关的其它指标 | |||||||||
儿童阅读兴趣 | 10 | 18.96 | 9 | 0.03 | 52.53 | 0.01 | 0.01 | 0.00 | 0.11 |
儿童请求阅读频率 | 6 | 8.40 | 5 | 0.14 | 40.44 | 0.01 | 0.01 | 0.00 | 0.07 |
儿童开始阅读的年龄 | 9 | 67.23 | 8 | 0.00 | 88.10 | 0.05 | 0.03 | 0.00 | 0.22 |
读写活动 | 7 | 14.63 | 6 | 0.02 | 58.93 | 0.01 | 0.01 | 0.00 | 0.09 |
非正式读写活动 | |||||||||
亲子阅读频率 | 55 | 393.21 | 54 | 0.00 | 86.27 | 0.02 | 0.01 | 0.00 | 0.12 |
去图书馆频率 | 9 | 20.00 | 8 | 0.01 | 60.00 | 0.01 | 0.01 | 0.00 | 0.08 |
外出活动频率 | 7 | 67.45 | 6 | 0.00 | 71.11 | 0.01 | 0.00 | 0.00 | 0.07 |
正式读写活动 | 31 | 73.36 | 30 | 0.00 | 59.11 | 0.01 | 0.00 | 0.00 | 0.07 |
HLE指标 | k | N | r | 95% CI | Z | p | 敏感性分析 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
HLE | 29 | 16105 | 0.31 | [0.26, 0.36] | 10.72 | 0.00 | 0.29~0.32 |
读写资源(藏书量) | 30 | 22238 | 0.26 | [0.21, 0.31] | 10.73 | 0.00 | 0.25~0.27 |
动机氛围 | |||||||
父母读写频率 | 13 | 1692 | 0.20 | [0.11, 0.29] | 4.25 | 0.00 | 0.18~0.23 |
父母读写信念 | 6 | 607 | 0.25 | [0.16, 0.35] | 5.28 | 0.00 | 0.23~0.32 |
与阅读相关的其它指标 | |||||||
儿童阅读兴趣 | 10 | 980 | 0.17 | [0.07, 0.26] | 3.46 | 0.00 | 0.14~0.20 |
儿童请求阅读频率 | 6 | 846 | 0.25 | [0.16, 0.33] | 5.33 | 0.00 | 0.21~0.27 |
儿童开始阅读的年龄 | 9 | 1449 | 0.06 | [-0.10, 0.22 ] | - | - | - |
读写活动 | 7 | 1785 | 0.17 | [0.08, 0.26] | 3.63 | 0.00 | 0.14~0.19 |
非正式读写活动 | |||||||
亲子阅读频率 | 55 | 24299 | 0.26 | [0.22, 0.30] | 12.55 | 0.00 | 0.25~0.27 |
去图书馆频率 | 9 | 10897 | 0.19 | [0.11, 0.26] | 5.23 | 0.00 | 0.17~0.22 |
外出活动频率 | 7 | 14151 | 0.09 | [0.03, 0.14] | 2.95 | 0.00 | 0.07~0.11 |
正式读写活动 | 31 | 9575 | 0.07 | [0.03, 0.11] | 3.68 | 0.00 | 0.06~0.08 |
表6 主效应检验结果
HLE指标 | k | N | r | 95% CI | Z | p | 敏感性分析 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
HLE | 29 | 16105 | 0.31 | [0.26, 0.36] | 10.72 | 0.00 | 0.29~0.32 |
读写资源(藏书量) | 30 | 22238 | 0.26 | [0.21, 0.31] | 10.73 | 0.00 | 0.25~0.27 |
动机氛围 | |||||||
父母读写频率 | 13 | 1692 | 0.20 | [0.11, 0.29] | 4.25 | 0.00 | 0.18~0.23 |
父母读写信念 | 6 | 607 | 0.25 | [0.16, 0.35] | 5.28 | 0.00 | 0.23~0.32 |
与阅读相关的其它指标 | |||||||
儿童阅读兴趣 | 10 | 980 | 0.17 | [0.07, 0.26] | 3.46 | 0.00 | 0.14~0.20 |
儿童请求阅读频率 | 6 | 846 | 0.25 | [0.16, 0.33] | 5.33 | 0.00 | 0.21~0.27 |
儿童开始阅读的年龄 | 9 | 1449 | 0.06 | [-0.10, 0.22 ] | - | - | - |
读写活动 | 7 | 1785 | 0.17 | [0.08, 0.26] | 3.63 | 0.00 | 0.14~0.19 |
非正式读写活动 | |||||||
亲子阅读频率 | 55 | 24299 | 0.26 | [0.22, 0.30] | 12.55 | 0.00 | 0.25~0.27 |
去图书馆频率 | 9 | 10897 | 0.19 | [0.11, 0.26] | 5.23 | 0.00 | 0.17~0.22 |
外出活动频率 | 7 | 14151 | 0.09 | [0.03, 0.14] | 2.95 | 0.00 | 0.07~0.11 |
正式读写活动 | 31 | 9575 | 0.07 | [0.03, 0.11] | 3.68 | 0.00 | 0.06~0.08 |
调节变量 | HLE | 亲子阅读频率 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
b (95% CI) | SE | b (95% CI) | SE | |
截距 | 0.55**(0.42, 0.67) | 0.06 | 0.26**(0.16, 0.36) | 0.05 |
年代 | ||||
1990~1999(参照组) | ||||
2000~2009 | -0.24**(-0.40, -0.09) | 0.08 | -0.04(-0.13, 0.11) | 0.06 |
2010~2021 | -0.28**(-0.43, -0.12) | 0.08 | -0.08(-0.19, 0.04) | 0.06 |
文化背景 | ||||
西方(参照组) | ||||
东方 | 0.02(-0.15, 0.19) | 0.09 | —— | — |
年龄 | ||||
6岁以下(2.5~6) (参照组) | ||||
6岁以上 | -0.14(-0.31, 0.04) | 0.09 | -0.03(-0.13, 0.07) | 0.05 |
测量方法 | ||||
问卷(参照组) | ||||
HOME/CTC | -0.03(-0.15, 0.10) | 0.06 | 0.13** (0.04, 0.22) | 0.05 |
R² | 18.00% | 0.00% | ||
Q (df) | 23.00**(5) | 10.83 (4) |
表7 调节变量的元回归分析
调节变量 | HLE | 亲子阅读频率 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
b (95% CI) | SE | b (95% CI) | SE | |
截距 | 0.55**(0.42, 0.67) | 0.06 | 0.26**(0.16, 0.36) | 0.05 |
年代 | ||||
1990~1999(参照组) | ||||
2000~2009 | -0.24**(-0.40, -0.09) | 0.08 | -0.04(-0.13, 0.11) | 0.06 |
2010~2021 | -0.28**(-0.43, -0.12) | 0.08 | -0.08(-0.19, 0.04) | 0.06 |
文化背景 | ||||
西方(参照组) | ||||
东方 | 0.02(-0.15, 0.19) | 0.09 | —— | — |
年龄 | ||||
6岁以下(2.5~6) (参照组) | ||||
6岁以上 | -0.14(-0.31, 0.04) | 0.09 | -0.03(-0.13, 0.07) | 0.05 |
测量方法 | ||||
问卷(参照组) | ||||
HOME/CTC | -0.03(-0.15, 0.10) | 0.06 | 0.13** (0.04, 0.22) | 0.05 |
R² | 18.00% | 0.00% | ||
Q (df) | 23.00**(5) | 10.83 (4) |
亚组两两比较 | HLE | 亲子阅读频率 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
p值 | Bonferroni法校正p值 | p值 | Bonferroni 法校正p值 | |
1990~1999—2000~2009 | 0.0020 | 0.0060 | 0.3323 | 0.9969 |
1990~1999—2010~2021 | 0.0004 | 0.0012 | 0.2090 | 0.6270 |
2000~2009—2010~2021 | 0.2681 | 0.8043 | 0.1345 | 0.4035 |
表8 年代的三个亚组效应量两两比较结果
亚组两两比较 | HLE | 亲子阅读频率 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
p值 | Bonferroni法校正p值 | p值 | Bonferroni 法校正p值 | |
1990~1999—2000~2009 | 0.0020 | 0.0060 | 0.3323 | 0.9969 |
1990~1999—2010~2021 | 0.0004 | 0.0012 | 0.2090 | 0.6270 |
2000~2009—2010~2021 | 0.2681 | 0.8043 | 0.1345 | 0.4035 |
(带*为元分析纳入文献) | |
[1] | 陈晓, 周晖, 赵瑾东. (2010). 儿童家庭读写活动、早期读写水平与小学一年级语文课成绩的关系. 心理发展与教育, 26(3), 258-266. |
[2] | 李敏谊, 张祎, 王诗棋, 秦思语. (2021). 幼小衔接阶段儿童的屏幕时间、纸读时间与早期读写能力的关系研究. 教育生物学杂志, 9(2), 102-108. |
[3] | 李燕芳, 董奇. (2004). 儿童早期读写能力发展的环境影响因素研究. 心理科学, (3), 531-535. |
[4] | * 林珮伃. (2012). 家庭语文环境与儿童接受性词汇的表现. 幼儿教育年刊, 23, 23-44. |
[5] | 刘杰, 孟会敏. (2009). 关于布郎芬布伦纳发展心理学生态系统理论. 中国健康心理学杂志, 17(2), 250-252. |
[6] | 刘丽, 石岩. (2012). 心理科学进展, 20(9), 1495-1506. |
[7] | 吕雪, 郭力平, 蒋路易. (2019). 电子阅读对年幼儿童故事理解的影响--基于24篇论文的元分析. 电化教育研究, 40(4), 76-84. |
[8] | 蒙台梭利, M. (2005). 童年的秘密(马荣根,单中惠译). 北京: 人民教育出版社. |
[9] | 桑标, 缪小春. (1990). 皮博迪图片词汇测验修订版(PPVT-R)上海市区试用常模的修订. 心理科学通讯, (5), 22-27. |
[10] | * 王娟, 沈秋苹. (2017). 家庭读写环境对儿童词汇发展的影响--母亲语言支架的中介效应. 中国临床心理学杂志, 25(4), 750-753. |
[11] | 吴瑶. (2016). 儿童数字阅读变革与反思. 中国出版, (2), 40-44. |
[12] | * 晏盈盈. (2015). 3-4岁幼儿家庭亲子阅读与言语能力发展水平的相关研究(硕士学位论文). 陕西师范大学, 西安. |
[13] | 杨书, 李婷婷, 刘新. (2007). 应用漏斗图识别发表性偏倚的效率研究. 成都医学院学报, 2(1), 33-34. |
[14] | 杨晓辉, 王振宏, 朱莉琪. (2016). 促进低龄儿童发展的电子媒体使用. 学前教育研究, (11), 24-37. |
[15] | 张天嵩, 张苏贤. (2017). 如何实现Meta分析中不同亚组合并效应量的比较. 中国循证医学杂志, 17(12), 1465- 1470. |
[16] | 张亚利, 李森, 俞国良. (2019). 自尊与社交焦虑的关系: 基于中国学生群体的元分析. 心理科学进展, 27(6), 1005-1018. |
[17] | 张义宾, 周兢. (2016). 纸媒还是屏媒? --数字时代儿童阅读的选择. 现代教育技术, 26(12), 24-30. |
[18] | * 赵瑾东, 周晖, 陈晓. (2008). 幼儿书目清单的编制及其与口头词汇的关系. 心理发展与教育, 24(1), 14-18. |
[19] |
Aminipour, S., Asgari, A., Hejazi, E., & Roßbach, H. G. (2020). Home learning environments: A cross-cultural study between Germany and Iran. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 38(4), 411-425.
doi: 10.1177/0734282918778465 URL |
[20] |
Anderson, D. R., & Hanson, K. G. (2010). From blooming, buzzing confusion to media literacy: The early development of television viewing. Developmental Review, 30(2), 239-255.
doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2010.03.004 URL |
[21] |
* Aram, D., Korat, O., & Hassunah-Arafat, S. (2013). The contribution of early home literacy activities to first grade reading and writing achievements in Arabic. Reading and Writing, 26(9), 1517-1536.
doi: 10.1007/s11145-013-9430-y URL |
[22] |
Aram, D., & Levin, I. (2004). The role of maternal mediation of writing to kindergartners in promoting literacy in school: A longitudinal perspective. Reading and Writing, 17(4), 387-409.
doi: 10.1023/B:READ.0000032665.14437.e0 URL |
[23] | * Aram, D., & Shapira, R. (2012). Parent-child shared book reading and children’s language, literacy, and empathy development. Rivista Italiana Di Educazione Familiare, 2(2), 55-65. |
[24] |
* Baroody, A. E., & Diamond, K. E. (2012). Links among home literacy environment, literacy interest, and emergent literacy skills in preschoolers at risk for reading difficulties. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 32(2), 78-87.
doi: 10.1177/0271121410392803 URL |
[25] | Bassok, D., Finch, J. E., Lee, R., Reardon, S. F., & Waldfogel, J. (2016). Socioeconomic gaps in early childhood experiences. AERA Open, 2(3), 233285841665392. |
[26] | * Bingham, G. E., Jeon, H., Kwon, K., & Lim, C. (2017). Parenting styles and home literacy opportunities: Associations with children’s oral language skills. Infant and Child Development, 26(5), 1-18. |
[27] |
Bojczyk, K. E., Davis, A. E., & Rana, V. (2016). Mother-child interaction quality in shared book reading: Relation to child vocabulary and readiness to read. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 36, 404-414.
doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2016.01.006 URL |
[28] |
* Bojczyk, K. E., Haverback, H. R., & Pae, H. K. (2018). Investigating maternal self-efficacy and home learning environment of families enrolled in Head Start. Early Childhood Education Journal, 46(2), 169-178.
doi: 10.1007/s10643-017-0853-y URL |
[29] |
* Bojczyk, K. E., Haverback, H. R., Pae, H. K., Hairston, M., & Haring, C. D. (2019). Parenting practices focusing on literacy: A study of cultural capital of kindergarten and first-grade students from low-income families. Early Child Development and Care, 189(3), 500-512.
doi: 10.1080/03004430.2017.1328416 URL |
[30] |
* Bonnett, R. (2007). Relation of home literacy, parental support, and child initiation of reading to emergent literacy in Head Start preschool children. Journal of Chemical Information and Modeling, 53(9), 1689-1699.
doi: 10.1021/ci400128m URL |
[31] | Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P. T., & Rothstein, H. R. (Eds). (2009). Introduction to meta-analysis. Chichester, West Sussex: John Wiley & Sons. |
[32] |
* Bracken, S. S., & Fischel, J. E. (2008). Family reading behavior and early literacy skills in preschool children from low-income backgrounds. Early Education and Development, 19(1), 45-67.
doi: 10.1080/10409280701838835 URL |
[33] |
Bradley, R. H., & Caldwell, B. M. (1988). Using the HOME inventory to assess the family environment. Pediatric Nursing, 14, 97-102.
pmid: 3353146 |
[34] | Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. |
[35] | Bronfenbrenner, U., & Morris, P. A. (1998). The ecology of developmental processes. In W. Damon, & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: The theorectical models of human development (pp.993-1023). New York, NY: Wiley. |
[36] |
* Buhs, E. S., Welch, G., Burt, J., & Knoche, L. (2011). Family engagement in literacy activities: Revised factor structure for The Familia-an instrument examining family support for early literacy development. Early Child Development and Care, 181(7), 989-1006.
doi: 10.1080/03004430.2011.564758 URL |
[37] |
* Bulotsky-Shearer, R. J., Wen, X., Faria, A.-M., Hahs-Vaughn, D. L., & Korfmacher, J. (2012). National profiles of classroom quality and family involvement: A multilevel examination of proximal influences on Head Start children’s school readiness. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 27(4), 627-639.
doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2012.02.001 URL |
[38] |
Burgess, S. R., Hecht, S. A., & Lonigan, C. J. (2002). Relations of the home literacy environment (HLE) to the development of reading-related abilities: A one-year longitudinal study. Reading Research Quarterly, 37(4), 408-426.
doi: 10.1598/RRQ.37.4.4 URL |
[39] |
* Burris, P. W., Phillips, B. M., & Lonigan, C. J. (2019). Examining the relations of the home literacy environments of families of low SES with children’s early literacy skills. Journal of Education for Students Placed at Risk, 24(2), 154-173.
doi: 10.1080/10824669.2019.1602473 URL |
[40] | Bus, A. G., Neuman, S. B., & Roskos, K. (2020). Screens, apps, and digital books for young children: The promise of multimedia. AERA Open, 6(1), 233285842090149. |
[41] |
Bus, A. G., van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Pellegrini, A. D. (1995). Joint book reading makes for success in learning to read: A meta-analysis on intergenerational transmission of literacy. Review of Educational Research, 65(1), 1-21.
doi: 10.3102/00346543065001001 URL |
[42] |
Camisón-Zornoza, C., Lapiedra-Alcamí, R., Segarra-Ciprés, M., & Boronat-Navarro, M. (2004). A meta-analysis of innovation and organizational size. Organization Studies, 25(3), 331-361.
doi: 10.1177/0170840604040039 URL |
[43] | Card, N. A. (2012). Applied meta-analysis for social science research. New York, NY: Guilford Press. |
[44] | * Chamberlain, S. E. (2003). Emergent literacy differences in Latino and African American children: Culture or poverty? (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of South California. |
[45] | * Chansa-Kabali, T. (2017). Home literacy activities: Accounting for differences in early grade literacy outcomes in low-income families in Zambia. South African Journal of Childhood Education, 7, 1-9. |
[46] | * Chazan-Cohen, R., Raikes, H., Brooks-Gunn, J., Ayoub, C., Pan, B. A., Kisker, E. E., ... Fuligni, A. S. (2009). Low-income children's school readiness: Parent contributions over the first five years. Early Education & Development, 20(6), 958-977. |
[47] |
* Christian, K., Morrison, F. J., & Bryant, F. B. (1998). Predicting kindergarten academic skills: Interactions among child care, maternal education, and family literacy environments. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 13(3), 501-521.
doi: 10.1016/S0885-2006(99)80054-4 URL |
[48] | Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. |
[49] | * Crystal, C. (2013). The effects of parental literacy involvement and child reading interest on the development of emergent literacy skills (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee. |
[50] |
Cunningham, A. E., & Stanovich, K. E. (1991). Tracking the unique effects of print exposure in children: Associations with vocabulary, general knowledge, and spelling. Journal of Educational Psychology, 83(2), 264-274.
doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.83.2.264 URL |
[51] |
Cunningham, A. E., & Stanovich, K. E. (1993). Children’s literacy environments and early word recognition subskills. Reading and Writing, 5(2), 193-204.
doi: 10.1007/BF01027484 URL |
[52] | * Daniels, J. K. (2004). Literate behaviors in African American Head Start families: A multiple literacies perspective (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Maryland. |
[53] |
Danielson, K., Wong, K. M., & Neuman, S. B. (2019). Vocabulary in educational media for preschoolers: A content analysis of word selection and screen-based pedagogical supports. Journal of Children and Media, 13(3), 345-362.
doi: 10.1080/17482798.2019.1585892 |
[54] |
* Davidse, N. J., de Jong, M. T., Bus, A. G., Huijbregts, S. C. J., & Swaab, H. (2011). Cognitive and environmental predictors of early literacy skills. Reading and Writing, 24(4), 395-412.
doi: 10.1007/s11145-010-9233-3 URL |
[55] |
de Bondt, M., Willenberg, I. A., & Bus, A. G. (2020). Do book giveaway programs promote the home literacy environment and children’s literacy-related behavior and skills? Review of Educational Research, 90(3), 349-375.
doi: 10.3102/0034654320922140 URL |
[56] |
DeBaryshe, B. D., & Binder, J. C. (1994). Development of an instrument for measuring parental beliefs about reading aloud to young children. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 78(3_suppl), 1303-1311.
doi: 10.2466/pms.1994.78.3c.1303 URL |
[57] |
* Deckner, D. F., Adamson, L. B., & Bakeman, R. (2006). Child and maternal contributions to shared reading: Effects on language and literacy development. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 27(1), 31-41.
doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2005.12.001 URL |
[58] |
* Dickinson, D. K., & de Temple, J. (1998). Putting parents in the picture: Maternal reports of preschoolers’ literacy as a predictor of early reading. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 13(2), 241-261.
doi: 10.1016/S0885-2006(99)80037-4 URL |
[59] |
Dong, Y., Dong, W. Y., Wu, S. X. Y., & Tang, Y. (2020). The effects of home literacy environment on children's reading comprehension development: A meta-analysis. Educational Sciences: Theory and Practice, 20(2), 63-82.
doi: 10.12738/jestp.2020.2.005 URL |
[60] | Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, D. M. (2007). The peabody picture vocabulary test (4th ed.). Bloomington, MN: NCS Pearson, Inc. |
[61] | Durkin, D. (1966). Children who read early. New York, NY: Teachers College Press. |
[62] |
* Evans, M. A., Shaw, D., & Bell, M. (2000). Home literacy activities and their influence on early literacy skills. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54(2), 65-75.
doi: 10.1037/h0087330 URL |
[63] |
Evans, M., Kelley, J., Sikora, J., & Treiman, D. (2010). Family scholarly culture and educational success: Books and schooling in 27 nations. Research in Social Stratification and Mobility, 28, 171-197.
doi: 10.1016/j.rssm.2010.01.002 URL |
[64] |
Farkas, G., & Beron, K. (2004). The detailed age trajectory of oral vocabulary knowledge: Differences by class and race. Social Science Research, 33(3), 464-497.
doi: 10.1016/j.ssresearch.2003.08.001 URL |
[65] |
* Farver, J. A. M., Xu, Y., Eppe, S., & Lonigan, C. J. (2006). Home environments and young Latino children’s school readiness. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 21(2), 196-212.
doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2006.04.008 URL |
[66] |
Fernald, A., Marchman, V. A., & Weisleder, A. (2013). SES differences in language processing skill and vocabulary are evident at 18 months. Developmental Science, 16(2), 234-248.
doi: 10.1111/desc.12019 pmid: 23432833 |
[67] | * Fielding-barnsley, R., & Hay, I. (2012). Comparative effectiveness of phonological awareness and oral language intervention for children with low emergent literacy skills. Australian Journal of Language and Literacy, 35(3), 271- 286. |
[68] |
Finneran, D. A., Heilmann, J. J., Moyle, M. J., & Chen, S. (2020). An examination of cultural-linguistic influences on PPVT-4 performance in African American and Hispanic preschoolers from low-income communities. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 34(3), 242-255.
doi: 10.1080/02699206.2019.1628811 pmid: 31238750 |
[69] |
* Foster, M. A., Lambert, R., Abbott-Shim, M., McCarty, F., & Franze, S. (2005). A model of home learning environment and social risk factors in relation to children’s emergent literacy and social outcomes. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 20(1), 13-36.
doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2005.01.006 URL |
[70] |
* Frijters, J. C., Barron, R. W., & Brunello, M. (2000). Direct and mediated influences of home literacy and literacy interest on prereaders’ oral vocabulary and early written language skill. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92(3), 466-477.
doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.92.3.466 URL |
[71] |
* Froiland, J. M., Powell, D. R., Diamond, K. E., & Son, H. C. S. (2013). Neighborhood socioeconomic well-being, home literacy, and early literacy skills of at-risk preschoolers. Psychology in the Schools, 50(8), 755-769.
doi: 10.1002/pits.2013.50.issue-8 URL |
[72] |
Gettinger, M., & Stoiber, K. C. (2014). Increasing opportunities to respond to print during storybook reading: Effects of evocative print-referencing techniques. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 29(3), 283-297.
doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2014.03.001 URL |
[73] |
Gilkerson, J., Richards, J. A., & Topping, K. J. (2017). The impact of book reading in the early years on parent-child language interaction. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, 17(1), 92-110.
doi: 10.1177/1468798415608907 URL |
[74] |
* Gonzalez, J. E., Acosta, S., Davis, H., Pollard-Durodola, S., Saenz, L., Soares, D., ... Zhu, L. (2017). Latino maternal literacy beliefs and practices mediating socioeconomic status and maternal education effects in predicting child receptive vocabulary. Early Education and Development, 28(1), 78-95.
doi: 10.1080/10409289.2016.1185885 URL |
[75] |
Gonzalez, J. E., Taylor, A. B., McCormick, A. S., Villareal, V., Kim, M., Perez, E., ... Haynes, R. (2011). Exploring the underlying factor structure of the home literacy environment (HLE) in the English and Spanish versions of the Familia Inventory: A cautionary tale. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 26(4), 475-483.
doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2010.12.001 URL |
[76] |
* Griffin, E. A., & Morrison, F. J. (1997). The unique contribution of home literacy environment to differences in early literacy skills. Early Child Development and Care, 127(1), 233-243.
doi: 10.1080/0300443971270119 URL |
[77] |
Hamilton, L. G., Hayiou-Thomas, M. E., Hulme, C., & Snowling, M. J. (2016). The home literacy environment as a predictor of the early literacy development of children at family-risk of dyslexia. Scientific Studies of Reading, 20(5), 401-419.
doi: 10.1080/10888438.2016.1213266 pmid: 28250707 |
[78] | Hart, B., & Risley, T. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday lives of American children. Baltimore, MD: Brookes Publishing. |
[79] |
* Hayes, N., Berthelsen, D. C., Nicholson, J. M., & Walker, S. (2018). Trajectories of parental involvement in home learning activities across the early years: Associations with socio-demographic characteristics and children’s learning outcomes. Early Child Development and Care, 188(10), 1405-1418.
doi: 10.1080/03004430.2016.1262362 URL |
[80] | Heath, R. W., Levin, P. F., & Tibbetts, K. A. (1993). Development of the home learning environment profile. In R. N. Roberts (Ed.), Coming home to preschool: The sociocultural context of early education (pp.91-132). Norwood, NJ: Ablex. |
[81] | Hiebert, E. H. (2015). Changing readers, changing texts: Beginning reading texts from 1960 to 2010. Journal of Education, 195(3), 1-13. |
[82] |
Higgins, J. P. T., Thompson, S. G., Deeks, J. J., & Altman, D. G. (2003). Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. British Medical Journal, 327(7414), 557-560.
doi: 10.1136/bmj.327.7414.557 URL |
[83] | * Hindman, A. H. (2008). School-family partnership and early learning: The role of school outreach and family involvement in preschoolers' literacy and learning-related social skills (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Michigan. |
[84] |
Hindman, A. H., Connor, C. M., Jewkes, A. M., & Morrison, F. J. (2008). Untangling the effects of shared book reading: Multiple factors and their associations with preschool literacy outcomes. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 23(3), 330-350.
doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2008.01.005 URL |
[85] | Hofstede, G. (2011). Dimensionalizing cultures: The Hofstede model in context. Online Readings in Psychology and Culture, Unit 2. Retrieved from http://scholarworks.gvsu.edu/orpc/vol2/iss1/8 |
[86] | Hofstede, G., Hofstede, G. J., & Minkov, M. (2010). Cultures and organizations: Software of the mind (Rev. 3rd ed.). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill. |
[87] |
Hu, B. Y., Johnson, G. K., Teo, T., & Wu, Z. (2020). Relationship between screen time and Chinese children’s cognitive and social development. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 34(2), 183-207.
doi: 10.1080/02568543.2019.1702600 URL |
[88] |
* Hubbs-Tait, L., Mulugeta, A., Bogale, A., Kennedy, T. S., Baker, E. R., & Stoecker, B. J. (2009). Main and interaction effects of iron, zinc, lead, and parenting on children’s cognitive outcomes. Developmental Neuropsychology, 34(2), 175-195.
doi: 10.1080/87565640802646759 pmid: 19267294 |
[89] |
Huntsinger, C. S., Jose, P. E., Liaw, F.-R., & Ching, W. D. (1997). Cultural differences in early mathematics learning: A comparison of euro-american, chinese-american, and taiwan-chinese families. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 21(2), 371-388.
doi: 10.1080/016502597384929 URL |
[90] | Hussar, B., Zhang, J., Hein, S., Wang, K., Roberts, A., Cui, J., ... Dilig, R. (2020). The condition of education 2020. https://nces.ed.gov/pubsearch/pubsinfo.asp?pubid=2020144 |
[91] |
Inkeles, A., & Sirowy, L. (1983). Convergent and divergent trends in national educational systems. Social Forces, 62 (2), 303-333.
doi: 10.2307/2578310 URL |
[92] |
* Inoue, T., Georgiou, G. K., Parrila, R., & Kirby, J. R. (2018). Examining an extended home literacy model: The mediating roles of emergent literacy skills and reading fluency. Scientific Studies of Reading, 22(4), 273-288.
doi: 10.1080/10888438.2018.1435663 URL |
[93] |
* Iruka, I. U., Brown, D., Jerald, J., & Blitch, K. (2018). Early steps to school success (ESSS): Examining pathways linking home visiting and language outcomes. Child and Youth Care Forum, 47(2), 283-301.
doi: 10.1007/s10566-017-9430-1 URL |
[94] |
* Iruka, I. U., Dotterer, A. M., & Pungello, E. P. (2014). Ethnic variations of pathways linking socioeconomic status, parenting, and preacademic skills in a nationally representative sample. Early Education and Development, 25(7), 973-994.
doi: 10.1080/10409289.2014.892806 URL |
[95] |
Keller, H., Lamm, B., Abels, M., Yovsi, R., Borke, J., Jensen, H., ... Chaudhary, N. (2006). Cultural models, socialization goals, and parenting ethnotheories: A multicultural analysis. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 37, 155-172.
doi: 10.1177/0022022105284494 URL |
[96] | * Kelman, M. E. (2006). An investigation of preschool children’s primary literacy skills (Unpublished doctorial dissertation). Wichita State University. |
[97] | Kirby, J. R., & Hogan, B. (2008). Family literacy environment and early literacy development. Exceptionality Education International, 18(3), 112-130. |
[98] |
Klein, O., & Becker, B. (2017). Preschools as language learning environments for children of immigrants: Differential effects by familial language use across different preschool contexts. Research in Social Stratification and Mobility, 48, 20-31.
doi: 10.1016/j.rssm.2017.01.001 URL |
[99] |
* Kluczniok, K., & Mudiappa, M. (2019). Relations between socio-economic risk factors, home learning environment and children’s language competencies: Findings from a German study. European Educational Research Journal, 18(1), 85-104.
doi: 10.1177/1474904118790854 URL |
[100] |
* Korat, O., Arafat, S. H., Aram, D., & Klein, P. (2013). Book reading mediation, SES, home literacy environment, and children’s literacy: Evidence from Arabic-speaking families. First Language, 33(2), 132-154.
doi: 10.1177/0142723712455283 URL |
[101] |
* Korucu, I., & Schmitt, S. A. (2020). Continuity and change in the home environment: Associations with school readiness. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 53, 97- 107.
doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2020.03.002 URL |
[102] |
* Krijnen, E., van Steensel, R., Meeuwisse, M., Jongerling, J., & Severiens, S. (2020). Exploring a refined model of home literacy activities and associations with children’s emergent literacy skills. Reading and Writing, 33(1), 207- 238.
doi: 10.1007/s11145-019-09957-4 URL |
[103] |
* Ladd, M., Martin-Chang, S., & Levesque, K. (2011). Parents’ reading-related knowledge and children’s reading acquisition. Annals of Dyslexia, 61(2), 201-222.
doi: 10.1007/s11881-011-0053-1 URL |
[104] | * Lambrecht, J., Bogda, K., Koch, H., Nottbusch, G., & Sporer, N. (2019). Comparing the effect of home and institutional learning environment on children’s vocabulary in primary school. Journal for Educational Research Online, 11(2), 86-115. |
[105] |
Laufer, B. (1998). The development of passive and active vocabulary in a second language: Same or different? Applied Linguistics, 19(2), 255-271.
doi: 10.1093/applin/19.2.255 URL |
[106] | * Lehrl, S., Ebert, S., & Rossbach, H. (2013). Facets of preschoolers' home literacy environments: What contributes to reading literacy in primary school? In M. Pfost, C. Artelt, & S, Weinert (Eds.), The development of reading literacy from early childhood to adolescence: Empirical findings from the Bamberg BiKS longitudinal studies (pp.35-61). University of Bamberg press. |
[107] | Leichter, H. J. (1984). Families as environments for literacy. In H. Goelman, A. Oberg, & F. Smith (Eds.), Awakening to literacy (pp.38-50). London: Heinemann. |
[108] |
* Lee, Y.-J., Lee, J.-S., & Lee, J.-W. (1997). The role of the play environment in young children’s language development. Early Child Development and Care, 139, 49-71.
doi: 10.1080/0300443971390105 URL |
[109] | Li, J. (2012). Cultural foundations of learning: East and west. New Youk, NY: Cambridge University Press. |
[110] |
Liu, C., Georgiou, G. K., & Manolitsis, G. (2018). Modeling the relationships of parents’ expectations, family’s SES, and home literacy environment with emergent literacy skills and word reading in Chinese. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 43, 1-10.
doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2017.11.001 URL |
[111] |
* Lohndorf, R. T., Vermeer, H. J., Cárcamo, R. A., & Mesman, J. (2018). Preschoolers’ vocabulary acquisition in Chile: The roles of socioeconomic status and quality of home environment. Journal of Child Language, 45(3), 559-580.
doi: 10.1017/S0305000917000332 pmid: 28931450 |
[112] |
* Manolitsis, G., Georgiou, G. K., & Parrila, R. (2011). Revisiting the home literacy model of reading development in an orthographically consistent language. Learning and Instruction, 21(4), 496-505.
doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2010.06.005 URL |
[113] |
* Manolitsis, G., Georgiou, G., Stephenson, K., & Parrila, R. (2009). Beginning to read across languages varying in orthographic consistency: Comparing the effects of non-cognitive and cognitive predictors. Learning and Instruction, 19(6), 466-480.
doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2008.07.003 URL |
[114] |
Marulis, L. M, & Neuman, S. B. (2013). How vocabulary interventions affect young children at risk: A meta- analytic review. Journal of Research on Educational Effectiveness, 6(3), 223-262.
doi: 10.1080/19345747.2012.755591 URL |
[115] | Mayer, R. E. (2003). The promise of multimedia learning: Using the same instructional design methods across different media. Learning & Instruction, 13(2), 125-139. |
[116] | McQuillan, J. (2006). The effects of print access and print exposure on English vocabulary acquisition of language minority students. The Reading Matrix, 6(1), 41-51. |
[117] | * McTaggart, J. A. (2004). The role of children's interest and home literacy environment in the development of early literacy skills (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Guelph. |
[118] |
* Meng, C. (2015). Home literacy environment and Head Start children’s language development: The role of approaches to learning. Early Education and Development, 26(1), 106-124.
doi: 10.1080/10409289.2015.957614 URL |
[119] |
Mol, S. E., & Bus, A. G. (2011). To read or not to read: A meta-analysis of print exposure from infancy to early adulthood. Psychological Bulletin, 137(2), 267-296.
doi: 10.1037/a0021890 URL |
[120] |
Mol, S. E., Bus, A. G., de Jong, M. T., & Smeets, D. J. H. (2008). Added value of dialogic parent-child book readings: A meta-analysis. Early Education and Development, 19(1), 7-26.
doi: 10.1080/10409280701838603 URL |
[121] |
* Mol, S. E., & Neuman, S. B. (2014). Sharing information books with kindergartners: The role of parents’ extra- textual talk and socioeconomic status. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 29(4), 399-410.
doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2014.04.001 URL |
[122] |
Montag, J. L., Jones, M. N., & Smith, L. B. (2015). The words children hear: Picture books and the statistics for language learning. Psychological Science, 26(9), 1489- 1496.
doi: 10.1177/0956797615594361 URL |
[123] |
Niklas, F., Nguyen, C., Cloney, D. S., Tayler, C., & Adams, R. (2016). Self-report measures of the home learning environment in large scale research: Measurement properties and associations with key developmental outcomes. Learning Environments Research, 19(2), 181-202.
doi: 10.1007/s10984-016-9206-9 URL |
[124] |
Niklas, F., & Schneider, W. (2013). Home literacy environment and the beginning of reading and spelling. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 38(1), 40-50.
doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2012.10.001 URL |
[125] | Pagani, L. S., Fitzpatrick, C., Barnett, T. A., & Dubow, E.. (2010). Prospective associations between early childhood television exposure and academic, psychosocial, and physical well-being by middle childhood. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 164(5), 425-431. |
[126] |
Parmar, P., Harkness, S., & Super, C. M. (2004). Asian and Euro-American parents' ethnotheories of play and learning: Effects on preschool children's home routines and school behaviour. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 28, 97-104.
doi: 10.1080/01650250344000307 URL |
[127] |
* Payne, A. C., Whitehurst, G. J., & Angell, A. L. (1994). The role of home literacy environment in the development of language ability in preschool children from low-income families. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 9(3-4), 427-440.
doi: 10.1016/0885-2006(94)90018-3 URL |
[128] |
Pellegrini, A. D., Galda, L., Jones, I., & Perlmutter, J. (1995). Joint reading between mothers and their head start children: Vocabulary development in two text formats. Discourse Processes, 19(3), 441-463.
doi: 10.1080/01638539509544926 URL |
[129] |
Peterson, R. A., & Brown, S. P. (2005). On the use of beta coefficients in meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90(1), 175-181.
pmid: 15641898 |
[130] |
Puglisi, M. L., Hulme, C., Hamilton, L. G., & Snowling, M. J. (2017). The home literacy environment is a correlate, but perhaps not a cause, of variations in children’s language and literacy development. Scientific Studies of Reading, 21(6), 498-514.
doi: 10.1080/10888438.2017.1346660 pmid: 29930486 |
[131] |
Raikes, H., Luze, G., Brooks-Gunn, J., Raikes, H. A., Pan, B. A., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., ... Rodriguez, E. T. (2006). Mother-child book reading in low-income families: Correlates and outcomes during the first three years of life. Child Development, 77(4), 924-953.
pmid: 16942498 |
[132] |
* Ren, L., Hu, B. Y., & Wu, Z. (2019). Profiles of literacy skills among Chinese preschoolers: Antecedents and consequences of profile membership. Learning and Individual Differences, 69(2), 22-32.
doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2018.11.008 URL |
[133] |
* Ren, L., Hu, B. Y., & Zhang, X. (2021). Disentangling the relations between different components of family socioeconomic status and Chinese preschoolers’ school readiness. Family Process, 60(1), 216-234.
doi: 10.1111/famp.v60.1 URL |
[134] | Rideout, V. (2017). The Common-sense census: Media use by kids age zero to eight. San Francisco, CA: Common Sense Media. Retrieved May 3, 2020, from https://www.commonsensemedia.org/sites/default/files/uploads/research/csm_zerotoeight_fullreport_release_2.pdf |
[135] |
* Roberts, J., Jergens, J., & Burchinal, M. (2005). The role of home literacy practices in preschool children’s language and emergent literacy skills. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48(2), 345-359.
pmid: 15989397 |
[136] | * Rodriguez, E. T. (2008). The home literacy environment: Predictors of trajectories across the first five years and relations to children’s school readiness at pre-kindergarten (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). New York University. |
[137] |
* Rodriguez, E. T., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Spellmann, M. E., Pan, B. A., Raikes, H., Lugo-Gil, J., & Luze, G. (2009). The formative role of home literacy experiences across the first three years of life in children from low-income families. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 30(6), 677-694.
doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2009.01.003 URL |
[138] |
* Roth, F. P., Speece, D. L., & Cooper, D. H. (2002). A longitudinal analysis of the connection between oral language and early reading. Journal of Educational Research, 95(5), 259-272.
doi: 10.1080/00220670209596600 URL |
[139] | Rothstein, H. R., Sutton, A. J., & Borenstein, M. (2006). Publication bias in meta-analysis: Prevention, assessment and adjustments. Chichester, West Sussex: John Wiley & Sons. |
[140] |
Scarborough, H., & Dobrich, W. (1994). On the efficacy of reading to preschoolers. Developmental Review, 14, 245- 302.
doi: 10.1006/drev.1994.1010 URL |
[141] | * Schlesinger, M. A., Flynn, R. M., & Richert, R. A. (2019). Do parents care about TV? How parent factors mediate US children’s media exposure and receptive vocabulary. Journal of Children and Media, 13(4), 395-414. |
[142] |
* Schmerse, D., Anders, Y., Flöter, M., Wieduwilt, N., Roßbach, H., & Tietze, W. (2018). Differential effects of home and preschool learning environments on early language development. British Educational Research Journal, 44(2), 338-357.
doi: 10.1002/berj.2018.44.issue-2 URL |
[143] | * See, H. M. (2008). The protective role of home learning activities in the development of Head Start children's school readiness skills: A longitudinal analysis of learning growth rates from preschool through first grade (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Maryland. |
[144] |
* Sénéchal, M. (2006). Testing the home literacy model. Scientific Studies of Reading, 10(1), 59-87.
doi: 10.1207/s1532799xssr1001_4 URL |
[145] |
* Sénéchal, M., & LeFevre, J. A. (2002). Parental involvement in the development of children’s reading skill: A five-year longitudinal study. Child Development, 73(2), 445-460.
pmid: 11949902 |
[146] |
* Sénéchal, M., & LeFevre, J. A. (2014). Continuity and change in the home literacy environment as predictors of growth in vocabulary and reading. Child Development, 85(4), 1552-1568.
doi: 10.1111/cdev.12222 pmid: 24467656 |
[147] |
* Sénéchal, M., LeFevre, J. A., Hudson, E., & Lawson, E. P. (1996). Knowledge of storybooks as a predictor of young children’s vocabulary. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88(3), 520-536.
doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.88.3.520 URL |
[148] |
* Sénéchal, M., LeFevre, J.-A., Thomas, E. M., & Daley, K. E. (1998). Differential effects of home literacy experiences on the development of oral and written language. Reading Research Quarterly, 33(1), 96-116.
doi: 10.1598/RRQ.33.1.5 URL |
[149] |
* Sénéchal, M., Thomas, E., & Monker, J. A. (1995). Individual differences in 4-year-old children’s acquisition of vocabulary during storybook reading. Journal of Educational Psychology, 87(2), 218-229.
doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.87.2.218 URL |
[150] |
* Shahaeian, A., Wang, C., Tucker-Drob, E., Geiger, V., Bus, A. G., & Harrison, L. J. (2018). Early shared reading, socioeconomic status, and children’s cognitive and school competencies: Six years of longitudinal evidence. Scientific Studies of Reading, 22(6), 485-502.
doi: 10.1080/10888438.2018.1482901 URL |
[151] | Shu, H., Li, W., Richard, A., Ku, Y.-M., & Yue, X. (2002). The role of home-literacy environment in learning to read Chinese. In W. Li, J. S. Gaffney, & J. L. Packard (Eds.), Chinese children’s reading acquisition: Theoretical and pedagogical issues (pp.207-224). Boston, MA: Kluwer Academic Publishers. |
[152] |
* Silinskas, G., Torppa, M., Lerkkanen, M., & Nurmi, J. (2020). The home literacy model in a highly transparent orthography. School Effectiveness and School Improvement, 31(1), 80-101.
doi: 10.1080/09243453.2019.1642213 URL |
[153] |
* Skwarchuk, S. L., Sowinski, C., & LeFevre, J. A. (2014). Formal and informal home learning activities in relation to children’s early numeracy and literacy skills: The development of a home numeracy model. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 121(1), 63-84.
doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2013.11.006 URL |
[154] |
Sosa, A. V. (2015). Association of the type of toy used during play with the quantity and quality of parent-infant communication. Jama Pediatrics, 170(2), 132-137.
doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2015.3753 URL |
[155] |
* Sparks, A., & Reese, E. (2013). From reminiscing to reading: Home contributions to children’s developing language and literacy in low-income families. First Language, 33(1), 89-109.
doi: 10.1177/0142723711433583 URL |
[156] | Standing, E. M. (1957). Maria Montessori: Her life and work. New York, NY: Plume. |
[157] |
Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (1989). Exposure to print and orthographic processing. Reading Research Quarterly, 24(4), 402-433.
doi: 10.2307/747605 URL |
[158] |
* Stephenson, K. A., Parrila, R. K., Georgiou, G. K., & Kirby, J. R. (2008). Effects of home literacy, parents’ beliefs, and children’s task-focused behavior on emergent literacy and word reading skills. Scientific Studies of Reading, 12(1), 24-50.
doi: 10.1080/10888430701746864 URL |
[159] |
Stipek, D., Milburn, S., Clements, D., & Daniels, D. H. (1992). Parents’ beliefs about appropriate education for young children. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 13(3), 293-310.
doi: 10.1016/0193-3973(92)90034-F URL |
[160] |
* Suggate, S. P., Schaughency, E. A., & Reese, E. (2011). The contribution of age and reading instruction to oral narrative and pre-reading skills. First Language, 31(4), 379-403.
doi: 10.1177/0142723710395165 URL |
[161] |
Super, C. M., & Harkness, S. (2002). Culture structures the environment for development. Human Development, 45, 270-274.
doi: 10.1159/000064988 URL |
[162] | Switzer, G. E, Wisniewski, S. R, Belle, S., Dew, M. A, Dew, M. A, & Schultz, R. (1999). Selecting, developing, and evaluating research instruments. Social Psychiatry & Psychiatric Epidemiology, 34(8), 399-409. |
[163] |
Taylor, R. (1995). Functional uses of reading and shared literacy activities in Icelandic homes: A monograph in family literacy. Reading Research Quarterly, 30, 194-219.
doi: 10.2307/748032 URL |
[164] | Taylor, R. (1996). The Familia Inventory users' manual. Grandview, MO: Family Reading Resources. |
[165] | Taylor, R. (2000). The Familia Inventory users' manual and infant/toddler supplement. Grandview, MO: Family Reading Resources. |
[166] | Teale, W. H., & Sulzby, E. (1986). Emergent literacy as a perspective for examining how young children become writers and readers. In W. H. Teale, & E. Sulzby (Eds.). Emergent literacy: Writing and reading (pp.vii-xxv). Norwood, NJ: Ablex. |
[167] |
Teepe, R. C., Molenaar, I., Oostdam, R., Fukkink, R., & Verhoeven, L. (2017). Children’s executive and social functioning and family context as predictors of preschool vocabulary. Learning and Individual Differences, 57(7), 1-8.
doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2017.05.012 URL |
[168] |
Tomopoulos, S., Dreyer, B. P., Tamis-lemonda, C., Flynn, V., Rovira, I., Tineo, W., & Mendelsohn, A. L. (2006). Books, toys, parent-chikd interaction, and Development in young Latino children. Ambulatory Pediatrics, 6(2), 72-78.
pmid: 16530142 |
[169] |
* Torppa, M., Parrila, R., Niemi, P., Lerkkanen, M. K., Poikkeus, A. M., & Nurmi, J. E. (2013). The double deficit hypothesis in the transparent Finnish orthography: A longitudinal study from kindergarten to Grade 2. Reading and Writing, 26(8), 1353-1380.
doi: 10.1007/s11145-012-9423-2 URL |
[170] | U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics. (2019). American time use survey: Average hours per day parents spent caring for and helping household children as their main activity. Retrieved July 3, 2020, from https://www-bls-gov.proxy.lib.umich.edu/charts/american-time-use/activity-by-parent.htm |
[171] |
van Ijzendoorn, M. H., Dijkstra, J., & Bus, A. G. (1995). Attachment, intelligence, and language: A meta-analysis. Social Development, 4(2), 115-128.
doi: 10.1111/sode.1995.4.issue-2 URL |
[172] | * Wang, H. H. (2015). Home literacy environment, the quality of mother child book reading interactions and Taiwanese children’s early literacy development (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Syracuse University. New York. |
[173] |
Wasik, B. A., Hindman, A. H., & Snell, E. K. (2016). Book reading and vocabulary development: A systematic review. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 37, 39-57.
doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2016.04.003 URL |
[174] | Weinberger, J. (1996). Literacy goes to school: The parents' role in young children's literacy learning. London: Paul Chapman Publishing Ltd. |
[175] | Whitehurst, G. J. (1993). The Stony Brook family reading survey. Stony Brook, NY: Author. |
[176] |
* Whitehurst, G. J., Arnold, D. S., Epstein, J. N., Angell, A. L., Smith, M., & Fischel, J. E. (1994). A picture book reading intervention in day care and home for children from low-income families. Developmental Psychology, 30(5), 679-689.
doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.30.5.679 URL |
[177] |
Whitehurst, G. J., & Lonigan, C. J. (1998). Child development and emergent literacy. Child Development, 69(3), 848- 872.
pmid: 9680688 |
[178] | Wilkins, D. A. (1972). Linguistics in language teaching. London: Edward Amold. |
[179] |
* Willard, J. A., Agache, A., Jäkel, J., Glück, C. W., & Leyendecker, B. (2015). Family factors predicting vocabulary in Turkish as a heritage language. Applied Psycholinguistics, 36(4), 875-898.
doi: 10.1017/S0142716413000544 URL |
[180] |
* Williams, K. E., Barrett, M. S., Welch, G. F., Abad, V., & Broughton, M. (2015). Associations between early shared music activities in the home and later child outcomes: Findings from the longitudinal study of Australian children. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 31, 113- 124.
doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2015.01.004 URL |
[181] | World Bank. (2021). School enrollment, preprimary. Retrieved July 3, 2021, from https://data.worldbank.org/indicator/SE.PRE.ENRR |
[182] | * Wu, C.C. (2007). Reading beliefs and strategies of Taiwanese mothers with preschoolers in relation to the children’s emergent literacy (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Syracuse University. New York. |
[183] | * Xiao, N., Che, Y., Zhang, X., Song, Z., Zhang, Y., & Yin, S. (2020). Father-child literacy teaching activities as a unique predictor of Chinese preschool children's word reading skills. Infant and Child Development, 29(4), 1-16. |
[184] | Xu, Y., & Liu, L. (2020). Examining sociocultural factors in assessing vocabulary knowledge of children from low socio-economic background. Early Child Development and Care, 195(15), 1-11. |
[185] |
* Zhang, S. Z., Georgiou, G. K., Xu, J., Liu, J. M., Li, M., & Shu, H. (2018). Different measures of print exposure predict different aspects of vocabulary. Reading Research Quarterly, 53(4), 443-454.
doi: 10.1002/rrq.2018.53.issue-4 URL |
[186] |
Zucker, T. A., Cabell, S. Q., Justice, L. M., Pentimonti, J. M., & Kaderavek, J. N. (2013). The role of frequent, interactive prekindergarten shared reading in the longitudinal development of language and literacy skills. Developmental Psychology, 49(8), 1425-1439.
doi: 10.1037/a0030347 URL |
[1] | 李亚丹, 杜颖, 谢聪, 刘春宇, 杨毅隆, 李阳萍, 邱江. 语义距离与创造性思维关系的元分析[J]. 心理科学进展, 2023, 31(4): 519-534. |
[2] | 曾润喜, 李游. 自我效能感与网络健康信息搜寻关系的元分析[J]. 心理科学进展, 2023, 31(4): 535-551. |
[3] | 吴佳桧, 傅海伦, 张玉环. 感知社会支持与学生学业成就关系的元分析:学习投入的中介作用[J]. 心理科学进展, 2023, 31(4): 552-569. |
[4] | 郭英, 田鑫, 胡东, 白书琳, 周蜀溪. 羞愧对亲社会行为影响的三水平元分析[J]. 心理科学进展, 2023, 31(3): 371-385. |
[5] | 陈必忠, 孙晓军. 中国内地大学生时间管理倾向的时代变迁:1999~2020[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(9): 1968-1980. |
[6] | 杜宇飞, 欧阳辉月, 余林. 隔代抚养与老年人抑郁水平:一项基于东西方文化背景的元分析[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(9): 1981-1992. |
[7] | 赵宁, 刘鑫, 李纾, 郑蕊. 默认选项设置的助推效果:来自元分析的证据[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(6): 1230-1241. |
[8] | 黄潇潇, 张亚利, 俞国良. 2010~2020中国内地小学生心理健康问题检出率的元分析[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(5): 953-964. |
[9] | 张亚利, 靳娟娟, 俞国良. 2010~2020中国内地初中生心理健康问题检出率的元分析[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(5): 965-977. |
[10] | 于晓琪, 张亚利, 俞国良. 2010~2020中国内地高中生心理健康问题检出率的元分析[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(5): 978-990. |
[11] | 陈雨濛, 张亚利, 俞国良. 2010~2020中国内地大学生心理健康问题检出率的元分析[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(5): 991-1004. |
[12] | 王佳燕, 蓝媛美, 李超平. 二元工作压力与员工创新关系的元分析[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(4): 761-780. |
[13] | 林新奇, 栾宇翔, 赵锴, 赵国龙. 领导风格与员工创新绩效关系的元分析:基于自我决定视角[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(4): 781-801. |
[14] | 刘俊材, 冉光明, 张琪. 不同情绪载体的神经活动及其异同——脑成像研究的ALE元分析[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(3): 536-555. |
[15] | 陈静, 冉光明, 张琪, 牛湘. 儿童和青少年同伴侵害与攻击行为关系的三水平元分析[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(2): 275-290. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||