心理科学进展 ›› 2020, Vol. 28 ›› Issue (10): 1619-1630.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2020.01619
收稿日期:
2019-11-15
出版日期:
2020-10-15
发布日期:
2020-08-24
通讯作者:
颜文靖
E-mail:eagan-ywj@foxmail.com
基金资助:
LIANG Jing1, RUAN Qiannan2, LI He3, MA Mengqing1, YAN Wenjing2()
Received:
2019-11-15
Online:
2020-10-15
Published:
2020-08-24
Contact:
YAN Wenjing
E-mail:eagan-ywj@foxmail.com
摘要:
欺骗检测一直是心理学的重要研究问题。基于欺骗理论的认知视角, 研究者提出欺骗检测的认知负荷取向。采用隐瞒信息测试这一测谎范式, 通过操纵认知负荷影响个体在虚假反应时的记忆-反应冲突解决过程, 考察增加认知负荷对欺骗检测的影响, 以期更好地揭示欺骗检测的认知机制。在此基础上, 以普通人群和犯罪嫌疑人为被试探查基于记忆-反应冲突的欺骗检测的行为和生理指标, 并根据获得的行为和生理指标, 采用机器学习方法进行建模, 预测个体的欺骗行为。研究结果将服务于司法、安防和人际交往等领域的欺骗检测。
中图分类号:
梁静, 阮倩男, 李贺, 马梦晴, 颜文靖. (2020). 认知负荷取向下基于记忆-反应冲突的欺骗检测. 心理科学进展 , 28(10), 1619-1630.
LIANG Jing, RUAN Qiannan, LI He, MA Mengqing, YAN Wenjing. (2020). Deception detection based on memory-response conflict: A cognitive load approach. Advances in Psychological Science, 28(10), 1619-1630.
[1] | 白学军, 章鹏, 张琪涵, 宋璐, 杨宇. (2019). 功能性近红外光谱技术在说谎研究中的应用. 心理科学进展, 27(1), 160-170. |
[2] |
崔茜, 蒋军, 杨文静, 张庆林. (2013). 欺骗的神经机制和测谎应用: 来自fmri研究的证据. 心理科学进展, 21(9), 1629-1642.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2013.01629 URL |
[3] | 付翠. (2011). P300用于31例刑事在押人员的检测准确性研究. 刑事技术, 0(4), 3-8. |
[4] | 高军峰, 王沛, 郑崇勋. (2010). 基于p300和机器学习的测谎方法研究. 西安交通大学学报, 44(10), 120-124. |
[5] |
梁静, 李开云, 曲方炳, 陈宥辛, 颜文靖, 傅小兰. (2014). 说谎的非言语视觉线索. 心理科学进展, 22(6), 995-1005.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2014.00995 URL |
[6] | 刘鑫, 位东涛, 张庆林, 郭英慧. (2018). GKT范式在确定犯罪嫌疑人中的应用: 一项ERP测谎研究. 心理科学, 41(3), 660-666. |
[7] | 王艳梅, 郭德俊. (2008). 积极情绪对任务转换的影响. 心理学报, 40(3), 301-306. |
[8] |
肖玉琴, 张卓, 宋平, 杨波. (2014). 冷酷无情特质: 一种易于暴力犯罪的人格倾向. 心理科学进展, 22(9), 1456-1466.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2014.01456 URL |
[9] | 张亭玉, 张雨青. (2008). 说谎行为及其识别的心理学研究. 心理科学进展, 16(4), 651-660. |
[10] | 赵敏, 郑崇勋, 赵春临. (2010). 利用小波分解和支持向量机的心理意识真实性识别研究. 西安交通大学学报, 44(4), 119-124. |
[11] |
Abe, N. (2009). The neurobiology of deception: Evidence from neuroimaging and loss-of-function studies. Current Opinion in Neurology, 22(6), 594-600.
doi: 10.1097/WCO.0b013e328332c3cf URL pmid: 19786872 |
[12] |
Abe, N. (2011). How the brain shapes deception: An integrated review of the literature. The Neuroscientist, 17(5), 560-574.
doi: 10.1177/1073858410393359 URL pmid: 21454323 |
[13] |
Ambach, W., Bursch, S., Stark, R., & Vaitl, D. (2010). A Concealed Information Test with multimodal measurement. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 75(3), 258-267.
doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2009.12.007 URL pmid: 20026133 |
[14] |
Ambach, W., Stark, R., & Vaitl, D. (2011). An interfering n- back task facilitates the detection of concealed information with EDA but impedes it with cardiopulmonary physiology. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 80(3), 217-226.
doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2011.03.010 URL |
[15] |
Ambach, W., Stark, R., Peper, M., & Vaitl, D. (2008). An interfering Go/No-go task does not affect accuracy in a Concealed Information Test. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 68(1), 6-16.
doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2007.11.004 URL pmid: 18180065 |
[16] |
Aron, A. R., Robbins, T. W., & Poldrack, R. A. (2004). Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(4), 170-177.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.02.010 URL |
[17] | Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In Psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 8, pp. 47-89). Academic Press. |
[18] |
Ben-Shakhar, G. (2012). Current research and potential applications of the concealed information test: An overview. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 342.
URL pmid: 23060826 |
[19] |
Ben-Shakhar, G., & Elaad, E. (2003). The validity of psychophysiological detection of information with the guilty knowledge test: A meta-analytic review. The Journal of Applied Psychology, 88(1), 131-151.
doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.88.1.131 URL pmid: 12675401 |
[20] |
Bhutta, M. R., Hong, M. J., Kim, Y. H., & Hong, K. S. (2015). Single-trial lie detection using a combined fNIRS- polygraph system. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 709.
URL pmid: 26082733 |
[21] |
Blandón-Gitlin, I., Fenn, E., Masip, J., & Yoo, A. H. (2014). Cognitive-load approaches to detect deception: Searching for cognitive mechanisms. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(9), 441-444.
URL pmid: 25168448 |
[22] |
Booth, J. R., Burman, D. D., Meyer, J. R., Gitelman, D. R., Parrish, T. B., & Mesulam, M. M. (2004). Development of brain mechanisms for processing orthographic and phonologic representations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(7), 1234-1249.
doi: 10.1162/0898929041920496 URL pmid: 15453976 |
[23] |
Christ, S. E., van Essen, D. C., Watson, J. M., Brubaker, L. E., & McDermott, K. B. (2009). The contributions of prefrontal cortex and executive control to deception: Evidence from activation likelihood estimate meta-analyses. Cerebral Cortex, 19(7), 1557-1566.
URL pmid: 18980948 |
[24] |
Ciesielski, K. T., Harris, R. J., & Cofer, L. F. (2004). Posterior brain ERP patterns related to the go/no-go task in children. Psychophysiology, 41(6), 882-892.
doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2004.00250.x URL pmid: 15563341 |
[25] |
Davatzikos, C., Ruparel, K., Fan, Y., Shen, D. G., Acharyya, M., Loughead, J. W., … Langleben, D. D. (2005). Classifying spatial patterns of brain activity with machine learning methods: Application to lie detection. Neuroimage, 28(3), 663-668.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.08.009 URL pmid: 16169252 |
[26] |
Debey, E., de Houwer, J., & Verschuere, B. (2014). Lying relies on the truth. Cognition, 132(3), 324-334.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.04.009 URL |
[27] |
Debey, E., Liefooghe, B., de Houwer, J., & Verschuere, B. (2015). Lie, truth, lie: The role of task switching in a deception context. Psychological Research, 79(3), 478-488.
doi: 10.1007/s00426-014-0582-4 URL pmid: 24923778 |
[28] |
DePaulo, B. M., Lindsay, J. J., Malone, B. E., Muhlenbruck, L., Charlton, K., & Cooper, H. (2003). Cues to deception. Psychological Bulletin, 129(1), 74-118.
doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.129.1.74 URL pmid: 12555795 |
[29] |
Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 135-168.
doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750 URL |
[30] |
Eysenck, M. W., & Eysenck, M. C. (1979). Processing depth, elaboration of encoding, memory stores, and expended processing capacity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning & Memory, 5(5), 472-484.
doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.5.5.472 URL |
[31] | Gamer, M. (2011). Detecting of deception and concealed information using neuroimaging techniques. In B. Verschuere, G. Ben-Shakhar, & E. Meijer (Eds.), Memory detection: Theory and application of the Concealed Information Test (pp. 90-113). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. |
[32] |
Gamer, M. (2014). Mind reading using neuroimaging: Is this the future of deception detection?. European Psychologist, 19(19), 172-183.
doi: 10.1027/1016-9040/a000193 URL |
[33] |
Gamer, M., & Berti, S. (2010). Task relevance and recognition of concealed information have different influences on electrodermal activity and event‐related brain potentials. Psychophysiology, 47(2), 355-364.
doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00933.x URL pmid: 20003148 |
[34] |
Gamer, M., Verschuere, B., Crombez, G., & Vossel, G. (2008). Combining physiological measures in the detection of concealed information. Physiology & Behavior, 95(3), 333-340.
doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2008.06.011 URL pmid: 18638496 |
[35] |
Gronau, N., Ben-Shakhar, G., & Cohen, A. (2005). Behavioral and physiological measures in the detection of concealed information. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90(1), 147-158.
doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.90.1.147 URL pmid: 15641895 |
[36] |
Haider, S. K., Jiang, A., Jamshed, M. A., Pervaiz, H., & Mumtaz, S. (2018). Performance enhancement in p300 erp single trial by machine learning adaptive denoising mechanism. IEEE Networking Letters, 1(1), 26-29.
doi: 10.1109/LNET URL |
[37] |
Haxby, J. V. (2012). Multivariate pattern analysis of fMRI: The early beginnings. Neuroimage, 62(2), 852-855.
URL pmid: 22425670 |
[38] |
Hu, X., & Rosenfeld, J. P. (2012). Combining the P300- complex trial-based Concealed Information Test and the reaction time-based autobiographical Implicit Association Test in concealed memory detection. Psychophysiology, 49(8), 1090-1100.
doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2012.01389.x URL pmid: 22681260 |
[39] |
Hu, X., Evans, A., Wu, H., Lee, K., & Fu, G. (2013). An interfering dot-probe task facilitates the detection of mock crime memory in a reaction time (RT)-based concealed information test. Acta Psychologica, 142(2), 278-285.
doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2012.12.006 URL pmid: 23376139 |
[40] |
Hu, X., Pornpattananangkul, N., & Rosenfeld, J. P. (2013). N 200 and P 300 as orthogonal and integrable indicators of distinct awareness and recognition processes in memory detection. Psychophysiology, 50(5), 454-464.
doi: 10.1111/psyp.12018 URL |
[41] |
Hu, X., Wu, H., & Fu, G. (2011). Temporal course of executive control when lying about self-and other-referential information: An ERP study. Brain Research, 1369, 149-157.
doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.10.106 URL pmid: 21059343 |
[42] |
Kubo, K., & Nittono, H. (2009). The role of intention to conceal in the P300-based concealed information test. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 34(3), 227-235.
doi: 10.1007/s10484-009-9089-y URL pmid: 19499325 |
[43] |
Levine, T. R., Blair, J. P., & Carpenter, C. J. (2018). A critical look at meta-analytic evidence for the cognitive approach to lie detection: A re-examination of Vrij, Fisher, and Blank (2017). Legal and Criminological Psychology, 23(1), 7-19.
doi: 10.1111/lcrp.2018.23.issue-1 URL |
[44] |
Meijer, E. H., & Verschuere, B. (2017). Deception detection based on neuroimaging: Better than the polygraph?. Journal of Forensic Radiology and Imaging, 8, 17-21.
doi: 10.1016/j.jofri.2017.03.003 URL |
[45] |
Meijer, E. H., Selle, N. K., Elber, L., & Ben-Shakhar, G. (2014). Memory detection with the Concealed Information Test: A meta analysis of skin conductance, respiration, heart rate, and P300 data. Psychophysiology, 51(9), 879-904.
doi: 10.1111/psyp.12239 URL |
[46] |
Mertens, R., & Allen, J. J. B. (2008). The role of psychophysiology in forensic assessments: Deception detection, ERPs, and virtual reality mock crime scenarios. Psychophysiology, 45(2), 286-298.
URL pmid: 17995914 |
[47] |
Monsell, S. (2003). Task switching. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(3), 134-140.
doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(03)00028-7 URL pmid: 12639695 |
[48] | National Research Council. (2003). The Polygraph and Lie Detection. Committee to review the scientific evidence on the polygraph (2003), The National Academic Press, Washington, DC. |
[49] | Rosenfeld, J. P. (2011). P300 in detecting concealed information. In B. Verschuere, G. Ben-Shakhar, & E. Meijer (Eds.), Memory detection: Theory and application of the Concealed Information Test (pp. 63-89). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. |
[50] |
Rosenfeld, J. P., Labkovsky, E., Winograd, M., Lui, M. A., Vandenboom, C., & Chedid, E. (2008). The Complex Trial Protocol (CTP): A new, countermeasure-resistant, accurate, P300-based method for detection of concealed information. Psychophysiology, 45(6), 906-919.
doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00708.x URL pmid: 18823418 |
[51] |
Rosenfeld, J. P., Soskins, M., Bosh, G., & Ryan, A. (2004). Simple, effective countermeasures to P300-based tests of detection of concealed information. Psychophysiology, 41(2), 205-219.
doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2004.00158.x URL pmid: 15032986 |
[52] |
Sartori, G., Agosta, S., Zogmaister, C., Ferrara, S. D., & Castiello, U. (2008). How to accurately detect autobiographical events. Psychological Science, 19(8), 772-780.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02156.x URL pmid: 18816284 |
[53] |
Seymour, T. L., & Kerlin, J. R. (2008). Successful detection of verbal and visual concealed knowledge using an RT- based paradigm. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 22(4), 475-490.
doi: 10.1002/(ISSN)1099-0720 URL |
[54] |
Seymour, T. L., Seifert, C. M., Shafto, M. G., & Mosmann, A. L. (2000). Using response time measures to assess "guilty knowledge". Journal of Applied Psychology, 85(1), 30-37.
doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.85.1.30 URL pmid: 10740954 |
[55] | Simbolon, A. I., Turnip, A., Hutahaean, J., & Siagian, Y. (2015). An experiment of lie detection based EEG-P300 classified by SVM algorithm. 2015 International Conference on Automation, Cognitive Science, Optics, Micro Electro- Mechanical System, and Information Technology (ICACOMIT). IEEE. |
[56] |
Spence, S. A., Farrow, T. F., Herford, A. E., Wilkinson, I. D., Zheng, Y., & Woodruff, P. W. (2001). Behavioural and functional anatomical correlates of deception in humans. Neuroreport, 12(13), 2849-2853.
doi: 10.1097/00001756-200109170-00019 URL pmid: 11588589 |
[57] |
Sporer, S. L., & Schwandt, B. (2006). Paraverbal indicators of deception: A meta-analytic synthesis. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 20(4), 421-446.
doi: 10.1002/(ISSN)1099-0720 URL |
[58] |
Sporer, S. L., & Schwandt, B. (2007). Moderators of nonverbal indicators of deception: A meta-analytic synthesis. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 13(1), 1-34.
doi: 10.1037/1076-8971.13.1.1 URL |
[59] |
Suchotzki, K., Verschuere, B., van Bockstaele, B., Ben-Shakhar, G., & Crombez, G. (2017). Lying takes time: A meta-analysis on reaction time measures of deception. Psychological Bulletin, 143(4), 428-453.
doi: 10.1037/bul0000087 URL pmid: 28182460 |
[60] |
Thorell, B., Lindqvist, S., Bergman, S., Bohlin, G., & Klingberg, T. (2009). Training and transfer effects of executive functions in preschool children. Developmental Science, 12(1), 106-113.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00745.x URL pmid: 19120418 |
[61] |
Trommer, B. L., Hoeppner, J. A. B., Lorber, R., & Armstrong, K. J. (1988). The Go—No-Go paradigm in attention deficit disorder. Annals of Neurology, 24(5), 610-614.
doi: 10.1002/ana.410240504 URL pmid: 3202613 |
[62] | Verschuere, B., & de Houwer, J. (2011). Detecting concealed information in less than a second: Response latency-based measures. Memory detection: Theory and application of the Concealed Information Test, 46-62. |
[63] |
Verschuere, B., & Kleinberg, B. (2016). ID-Check: Online Concealed Information Test Reveals True Identity. Journal of Forensic Sciences, 61(Suppl 1), S237-S240.
doi: 10.1111/jfo.2016.61.issue-S1 URL |
[64] |
Verschuere, B., Köbis, N. C., Bereby-Meyer, Y., Rand, D., & Shalvi, S. (2018). Taxing the brain to uncover lying? Meta- analyzing the effect of imposing cognitive load on the reaction-time costs of lying. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 7(3), 462-469.
doi: 10.1016/j.jarmac.2018.04.005 URL |
[65] |
Verschuere, B., Rosenfeld, J. P., Winograd, M. R., Labkovsky, E., & Wiersema, R. (2009). The role of deception in P300 memory detection. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 14(2), 253-262.
doi: 10.1348/135532508X384184 URL |
[66] |
Vincent, A., & Furedy, J. J. (1992). Electrodermal differentiation of deception: Potentially confounding and influencing factors. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 13(2), 129-136.
doi: 10.1016/0167-8760(92)90052-d URL pmid: 1399752 |
[67] | Visu-Petra, G., Bus, I., & Miclea, M. (2011). Detecting concealed information from a mock crime scenario by using psychophysiological and RT-based measures. Cognition, Brain, Behavior, 15(1), 19-37. |
[68] |
Visu-Petra, G., Miclea, M., & Visu-Petra, L. (2012). Reaction time-based detection of concealed information in relation to individual differences in executive functioning. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 26(3), 342-351.
doi: 10.1002/acp.1827 URL |
[69] |
Visu-Petra, G., Varga, M., Miclea, M., & Visu-Petra, L. (2013). When interference helps: Increasing executive load to facilitate deception detection in the concealed information test. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 146.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00146 URL pmid: 23543918 |
[70] |
Vrij, A., Fisher, R. P., & Blank, H. (2017). A cognitive approach to lie detection: A meta-analysis. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 22(1), 1-21.
doi: 10.1111/lcrp.2017.22.issue-1 URL |
[71] |
Vrij, A., Fisher, R., Mann, S., & Leal, S. (2006). Detecting deception by manipulating cognitive load. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(4), 141-142.
URL pmid: 16516533 |
[72] |
Vrij, A., Granhag, P. A., & Porter, S. (2010). Pitfalls and opportunities in nonverbal and verbal lie detection. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 11(3), 89-121.
doi: 10.1177/1529100610390861 URL pmid: 26168416 |
[73] |
Vrij, A., Mann, S. A., Fisher, R. P., Leal, S., Milne, R., & Bull, R. (2008). Increasing cognitive load to facilitate lie detection: The benefit of recalling an event in reverse order. Law and Human Behavior, 32(3), 253-265.
URL pmid: 17694424 |
[74] | Vrij, A., Mann, S., Leal, S., & Fisher, R. (2010). ‘Look into my eyes’: Can an instruction to maintain eye contact facilitate lie detection?. Psychology, Crime & Law, 16(4), 327-348. |
[75] |
Woodward, T. S., Ruff, C. C., & Ngan, E. T. C. (2006). Short- and long-term changes in anterior cingulate activation during resolution of task-set competition. Brain Research, 1068(1), 161-169.
URL pmid: 16376861 |
[76] | Zuckerman, M., DePaulo, B. M., & Rosenthal, R. (1981). Verbal and nonverbal communication of deception. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 14, 1-59. |
[1] | 陈新文, 李鸿杰, 丁玉珑. 探究事件相关脑电/脑磁信号中的神经表征模式:基于分类解码和表征相似性分析的方法[J]. 心理科学进展, 2023, 31(2): 173-195. |
[2] | 刘笑晗, 陈明隆, 郭静. 机器学习在儿童创伤后应激障碍识别及转归预测中的应用[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(4): 851-862. |
[3] | 侯婷婷, 陈潇, 孔德彭, 邵秀筠, 林丰勋, 李开云. 机器学习在自闭症儿童早期识别和诊断领域的应用[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(10): 2321-2337. |
[4] | 苏悦, 刘明明, 赵楠, 刘晓倩, 朱廷劭. 基于社交媒体数据的心理指标识别建模: 机器学习的方法[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(4): 571-585. |
[5] | 郑泓, 蒲城城, 王毅, 陈楚侨. 基于脑结构像的精神分裂症机器学习分类[J]. 心理科学进展, 2020, 28(2): 252-265. |
[6] | 董健宇, 韦文棋, 吴珂, 妮娜, 王粲霏, 付莹, 彭歆. 机器学习在抑郁症领域的应用[J]. 心理科学进展, 2020, 28(2): 266-274. |
[7] | 区健新, 吴寅, 刘金婷, 李红. 计算精神病学:抑郁症研究和临床应用的新视角[J]. 心理科学进展, 2020, 28(1): 111-127. |
[8] | 何丽萍, 申寻兵, 陈振彩, 卓芮芮, 李科定, 刘振南. 老年人的微表情识别与欺骗检测[J]. 心理科学进展, 2019, 27(suppl.): 155-155. |
[9] | 刘振南, 申寻兵, 杨何晨, 何丽萍, 李科定, 卓芮芮. 微表情识别与欺骗检测能力的跨文化比较[J]. 心理科学进展, 2019, 27(suppl.): 157-157. |
[10] | 王燕青, 王福兴, 谢和平, 陈佳雪, 李文静, 胡祥恩. 一图抵千言:多媒体学习中的自我生成绘图策略[J]. 心理科学进展, 2019, 27(4): 623-635. |
[11] | 车敬上, 孙海龙, 肖晨洁, 李爱梅. 为什么信息超载损害决策?基于有限认知资源的解释[J]. 心理科学进展, 2019, 27(10): 1758-1768. |
[12] | 陈佳雪, 谢和平, 王福兴, 周丽, 李文静. 诱发的积极情绪会促进多媒体学习吗?[J]. 心理科学进展, 2018, 26(10): 1818-1830. |
[13] | 谢和平; 王福兴; 王玉鑫; 安婧. 越难读意味着学得越好?学习过程中的不流畅效应[J]. 心理科学进展, 2016, 24(7): 1077-1090. |
[14] | 张冬梅;路海东;祖雅桐. 认知负荷视角下的知识反转效应[J]. 心理科学进展, 2016, 24(4): 501-509. |
[15] | 王福兴;段朝辉;周宗奎. 线索在多媒体学习中的作用[J]. 心理科学进展, 2013, 21(8): 1430-1440. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||