心理科学进展 ›› 2023, Vol. 31 ›› Issue (6): 1055-1067.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2023.01055
收稿日期:
2022-06-07
出版日期:
2023-06-15
发布日期:
2023-03-07
通讯作者:
陈文锋
E-mail:wchen@ruc.edu.cn
基金资助:
HE Tingting, WANG Yiman, CHEN Wenfeng()
Received:
2022-06-07
Online:
2023-06-15
Published:
2023-03-07
Contact:
CHEN Wenfeng
E-mail:wchen@ruc.edu.cn
摘要:
决策领域存在一个经典的联合评估的偏好反转现象(又称评估模式效应), 其解释力最强的广义可评估性理论(general evaluability theory, GET)强调了评估模式(即背景信息是否存在)的作用。社会认知领域也存在社会比较和社会评价的背景效应, 其著名的包含排除模型(Inclusion/ Exclusion Model, IEM)也强调背景信息的作用, 尤其是目标-背景相似性的作用。因此, 背景效应可以类比于评估模式效应, 也可以理解为一种“偏好” (上行同化和下行对比)及其“反转” (上行对比和下行同化)现象。基于从联合评估这一新的视角对背景效应进行的解释和拓展, 未来研究应该关注背景效应的测量标准、影响因素之间的交互作用、跨维度作用及其实践应用等方面, 以促进对背景效应的深化理解和两个领域的理论融合和实践结合。
中图分类号:
何婷婷, 王一漫, 陈文锋. (2023). 社会比较和社会评价的背景效应——来自联合评估视角的启示. 心理科学进展 , 31(6), 1055-1067.
HE Tingting, WANG Yiman, CHEN Wenfeng. (2023). Contextual effect of social comparison and social evaluation: Insights from the perspective of joint evaluation. Advances in Psychological Science, 31(6), 1055-1067.
[1] | 龚莹莹. (2019). 幼儿选择性信任的内群体偏好效应及其影响因素 (硕士学位论文). 吉林大学, 长春. |
[2] | 侯文霞. (2021). 面孔吸引力评价的背景效应—基于分类视角 (硕士学位论文). 中国人民大学, 北京. |
[3] |
侯文霞, 田欣然, 刘立志, 易冰, 欧玉晓, 陈文锋, 尚俊辰. (2021). 面孔吸引力同化的连续性效应. 心理科学进展, 29(7), 1210-1215.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2021.01210 |
[4] |
寇慧, 苏艳华, 张妍, 孔繁昌, 胡媛艳, 王洋, 陈红. (2013). 面孔吸引力的影响因素:观察者假设. 心理科学进展, 21(12), 2144-2153.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2013.02144 |
[5] |
路西, Hsee, C. K. (2018). 联合评估和单独评估:富有潜力的助推手段. 心理学报, 50(8), 827-839.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2018.00827 |
[6] | 潘思存, 黄希庭. (2022). 人类择偶复制现象的性别差异探析. 心理研究, 15(2), 160-166. |
[7] | 任乃馨, 陈文锋, 傅小兰. (2015). 负向情感化学习及其形成机制 (摘要). 见中国心理学会 (编), 第十八届全国心理学学术会议摘要集——心理学与社会发展 (pp. 405-406). 天津. |
[8] | 盛青. (2004). 如何使招聘面试更有效. 中国人力资源开发, (10), 49-53. |
[9] |
宋仕婕, 佐斌, 温芳芳, 谭潇. (2020). 群体认同对群际敏感效应及其行为表现的影响. 心理学报, 52(8), 993-1003.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2020.00993 |
[10] | 王一漫, 何婷婷, 陈文锋. (2023). 面孔可信度评价的跨维度同化效应: 社会经济地位调节下的内群体偏好不对称性. 科学通报, 68(2-3), 229-237. |
[11] | 邢淑芬, 俞国良. (2005). 社会比较研究的现状与发展趋势. 心理科学进展, 13(1), 78-84. |
[12] | 邢淑芬, 俞国良. (2006). 社会比较:对比效应还是同化效应? 心理科学进展, 14(6), 944-949. |
[13] | 严荣建. (2019). 成人对贫富成人及儿童的面孔表情刻板印象 (硕士学位论文). 西北师范大学, 兰州. |
[14] | 周春燕, 黄海, 刘陈陵, 郭永玉, 贺金波. (2015). 评价者与被评价者的社会阶层对阶层刻板印象的影响. 心理与行为研究, 13(4), 511-515. |
[15] | Arnold, A. J., Kappes, H. B., Klinenberg, E., & Winkielman, P. (2021). The role of comparisons in judgments of loneliness. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, Article 498305. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.498305 |
[16] |
Barker, P., Dotsch, R., & Imhoff, R. (2020). Assimilation and contrast in spontaneous comparisons: Heterogeneous effects of standard extremity in facial evaluations. International Review of Social Psychology, 33(1), 11, 1-17.
doi: 10.5334/irsp.291 URL |
[17] | Barker, P., & Imhoff, R. (2021). The dynamic interactive pattern of assimilation and contrast: Accounting for standard extremity in comparative evaluations. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 97, Article 104190. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2021.104190 |
[18] | Blanton, H. (2001). Evaluating the self in the context of another:The three-selves model of social comparison assimilation and contrast. In GMoskowitz (Ed.), Cognitive social psychology: The Princeton Symposium and the legacy and future of social cognition (pp. 75-88). Mahwah, New Jersey: Erlbaum. |
[19] |
Bless, H., & Schwarz, N. (1998). Context effects in political judgement: Assimilation and contrast as a function of categorization processes. European Journal of Social Psychology, 28(2), 159-172.
doi: 10.1002/(ISSN)1099-0992 URL |
[20] | Bless, H., & Schwarz, N. (2010). Chapter 6-Mental construal and the emergence of assimilation and contrast effects: The inclusion/exclusion model. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 319-373. |
[21] |
Brambilla, M., Masi, M., Mattavelli, S., & Biella, M. (2021). Faces and sounds becoming one: Cross-modal integration of facial and auditory cues in judging trustworthiness. Social Cognition, 39(3), 315-327.
doi: 10.1521/soco.2021.39.3.315 URL |
[22] |
Busemeyer, J. R., Gluth, S., Rieskamp, J., & Turner, B. M. (2019). Cognitive and neural bases of multi-attribute, multi-alternative, value-based decisions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 23(3), 251-263.
doi: S1364-6613(18)30284-5 pmid: 30630672 |
[23] | Cetre, S., Algan, Y., Grimalda, G., Murtin, F., Putterman, L., Schmidt, U., & Siegerink, V. (2020). Ethnic bias, economic success and trust: Findings from large sample experiments in Germany and the United States through the trustlab platform. OECD Statistics Working Papers, 2020(4), 2-16. |
[24] | Chang, L. W., Gershman, S. J., & Cikara, M. (2019). Comparing value coding models of context-dependence in social choice. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 85, Article 103847. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2019.103847 |
[25] | Charbonneau, I., Robinson, K., Blais, C., & Fiset, D. (2020). Implicit race attitudes modulate visual information extraction for trustworthiness judgments. PLoS One, 15(9), Article e0239305. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0239305 |
[26] | Chen, D. (2022, June). Social conformity updates the neural representation of facial attractiveness. Symposium conducted at the meeting of the 2022 National Forum for Outstanding PhD Students in Psychology, Canton, China. |
[27] |
Cogan, E., Parker, S., & Zellner, D. A. (2013). Beauty beyond compare: Effects of context extremity and categorization on hedonic contrast. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception Performance, 39(1), 16-22.
doi: 10.1037/a0031020 URL |
[28] | Damon, F., Méary, D., Quinn, P. C., Lee, K., Simpson, E., Paukner, A., … Pascalis, O. (2017). Preference for facial averageness: Evidence for a common mechanism in human and macaque infants. Scientific Reports, 7, Article 46303. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep46303 |
[29] |
DeBruine, L. M., Jones, B. C., Little, A. C., & Perrett, D. I. (2008). Social perception of facial resemblance in humans. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 37(1), 64-77.
pmid: 18157627 |
[30] |
Geiselman, R. E., Haight, N. A., & Kimata, L. G. (1984). Context effects on the perceived physical attractiveness of faces. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 20(5), 409-424.
doi: 10.1016/0022-1031(84)90035-0 URL |
[31] |
Gesa, W., Janina, J., Mareen, J. N., & Alexander, H. J. (2021). The impact of social comparisons of job demands and job control on well-being. Applied Psychology-Health and Well-being, 13(2), 419-436.
doi: 10.1111/aphw.12257 pmid: 33943008 |
[32] |
Haddock, G., Macrae, C. N., & Fleck, S. (2002). Syrian science and smart supermodels: On the when and how of perception-behavior effects. Social Cognition, 20, 461-481.
doi: 10.1521/soco.20.6.461.22976 URL |
[33] |
Hsee, C. K. (1996). The evaluability hypothesis: An explanation for preference reversals between joint and separate evaluations of alternatives. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 67(3), 247-257.
doi: 10.1006/obhd.1996.0077 URL |
[34] |
Hsee, C. K. (1998). Less is better: When low-value options are valued more highly than high-value options. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 11(2), 107-121.
doi: 10.1002/(ISSN)1099-0771 URL |
[35] |
Hsee, C. K., & Zhang, J. (2010). General evaluability theory. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5(4), 343-355.
doi: 10.1177/1745691610374586 pmid: 26162182 |
[36] |
Kahneman, D., & Ritov, I. (1994). Determinants of stated willingness to pay for public goods: A study in the headline method. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 9(1), 5-37.
doi: 10.1007/BF01073401 pmid: 32288199 |
[37] |
Kenrick, D. T., & Gutierres, S. E. (1980). Contrast effects and judgments of physical attractiveness: When beauty becomes a social problem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38(1), 131-140.
doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.38.1.131 URL |
[38] |
Komori, M., Kawamura, S., & Ishihara, S. (2009). Averageness or symmetry: Which is more important for facial attractiveness? Acta Psychologica, 131(2), 136-142.
doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2009.03.008 pmid: 19394585 |
[39] |
Marsh, H. W., Kong, C. K., & Hau, K. T. (2000). Longitudinal multilevel models of the big-fish-little-pond effect on academic self-concept: Counterbalancing contrast and reflected-glory effects in Hong Kong schools. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(2), 337-349.
pmid: 10707339 |
[40] |
Morgan, L. K., & Kisley, M. A. (2014). The effects of facial attractiveness and perceiver's mate value on adaptive allocation of central processing resources. Evolution and Human Behavior, 35(2), 96-102.
doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2013.11.002 URL |
[41] |
Mullen, B., Bryant, B., & Driskell, J. E. (1997). Presence of others and arousal: An integration. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 1(1), 52-64.
doi: 10.1037/1089-2699.1.1.52 URL |
[42] |
Mussweiler, T. (2003). Comparison processes in social judgment: Mechanisms and consequences. Psychological Review, 110(3), 472-489.
doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.110.3.472 pmid: 12885111 |
[43] | Neumann, J. V., & Morgenstern, O. (1944). Theory of games and economic behavior (60th Anniversary Commemorative Edition). Princeton: Princeton University Press. |
[44] |
Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84(3), 231-259.
doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.84.3.231 URL |
[45] |
Oh, D., Shafir, E., & Todorov, A. (2020). Economic status cues from clothes affect perceived competence from faces. Nature Human Behaviour, 4(3), 287-293.
doi: 10.1038/s41562-019-0782-4 pmid: 31819209 |
[46] | Qi, Y., Li, Q., & Du, F. (2018). Are rich people perceived as more trustworthy? Perceived socioeconomic status modulates judgments of trustworthiness and trust behavior based on facial appearance. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, Article 512. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00512 |
[47] |
Rennels, J. L., Bronstad, P. M., & Langlois, J. H. (2008). Are attractive men’s faces masculine or feminine? The importance of type of facial stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34(4), 884-893.
doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.34.4.884 URL |
[48] |
Ruys, K. I., Spears, R., Gordijn, E. H., & Vries, N. K. D. (2006). Two faces of (dis)similarity in affective judgments of persons: Contrast or assimilation effects revealed by morphs. Journal of Personality Social Psychology, 90(3), 399-411.
doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.90.3.399 URL |
[49] | Sakano, Y., Wada, A., Ikeda, H., Saheki, Y., Tagai, K., & Ando, H. (2021). Human brain activity reflecting facial attractiveness from skin reflection. Scientific Reports, 11(1), Article 3421. https://doi.org/10.1038/S41598-021-82601-W |
[50] | Schwarz, N., & Bless, H. (1992). Constructing reality and its alternatives:An inclusion/exclusion model of assimilation and contrast effects in social judgment. In L. L.Martin & A.Tesser (Eds.), The construction of social judgments (pp. 217-245). Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum. |
[51] |
Sevdalis, N., & Harvey, N. (2006). Determinants of willingness to pay in separate and joint evaluations of options: Context matters. Journal of Economic Psychology, 27(3), 377-385.
doi: 10.1016/j.joep.2005.07.001 URL |
[52] |
Sutherland, C. A. M., Young, A. W., & Rhodes, G. (2017). Facial first impressions from another angle: How social judgements are influenced by changeable and invariant facial properties. British Journal of Psychology, 108(2), 397-415.
doi: 10.1111/bjop.12206 pmid: 27443971 |
[53] |
Thorndike, E. L. (1920). A constant error in psychological ratings. Journal of Applied Psychology, 4, 25-29.
doi: 10.1037/h0071663 URL |
[54] | Todorov, A., & Oh, D. (2021). Chapter Four-The structure and perceptual basis of social judgments from faces. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 63, 189-245. |
[55] | Tomeo, O. B., Ungerleider, L. G., & Liu, N. (2017). Preference for averageness in faces does not generalize to non-human primates. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 11, Article 129. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00129 |
[56] | Tversky, A., & Gati, I. (1978). Studies of similarity. In E.Rosch & B.Lloys (Eds.), Cognition and categorization (pp. 81-98). New Jersey: Erlbaum. |
[1] | 石冠峰, 吴玉莹, 庞惠伟, 刘朝辉, 谢智慧. 职场错失焦虑的结构测量、多维效应与形成机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2023, 31(8): 1374-1388. |
[2] | 顾潇, 王玉慧, 雷雳. 社交媒体中自拍相关行为对身体映像的影响[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(8): 1497-1507. |
[3] | 刘得格, 黄晓治, 陈文晶, 李文东. 被妒忌:一种矛盾体验[J]. 心理科学进展, 2018, 26(1): 118-133. |
[4] | 刘得格;李文东;陈文晶. 恶意妒忌和善意妒忌的影响因素与作用机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2017, 25(2): 342-357. |
[5] | 何幸;崔丽莹. 他人财富情绪:妒忌的社会评估比较与认知加工分析[J]. 心理科学进展, 2016, 24(9): 1485-1495. |
[6] | 丁桂凤; 候亮; 张露; 张丽; 王曼; 古茜茜. 创业失败与再创业意向的作用机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2016, 24(7): 1009-1019. |
[7] | 谢晓非;陆静怡. 风险决策中的双参照点效应[J]. 心理科学进展, 2014, 22(4): 571-579. |
[8] | 李爱梅;赵丹;熊冠星;谭飞;王笑天;凌文辁. 等待是一种折磨?等待时间知觉及其导致的非理性决策行为[J]. 心理科学进展, 2014, 22(11): 1679-1690. |
[9] | 孙青青;郑丽军;郑涌. 性客体化与女性自我客体化[J]. 心理科学进展, 2013, 21(10): 1794-1802. |
[10] | 吴宝沛;张雷. 妒忌:一种带有敌意的社会情绪[J]. 心理科学进展, 2012, 20(9): 1467-1478. |
[11] | 刘艳. 自我建构研究的现状与展望[J]. 心理科学进展, 2011, 19(3): 427-439. |
[12] | 李静;郭永玉. 收入与幸福的关系及其现实意义[J]. 心理科学进展, 2010, 18(7): 1073-1080. |
[13] | 田宏杰;王福兴;徐菲菲;申继亮. 场景知觉中物体加工的背景效应[J]. 心理科学进展, 2010, 18(6): 878-886. |
[14] | 周浩;龙立荣. 公平感社会比较的参照对象选择研究述评[J]. 心理科学进展, 2010, 18(6): 948-954. |
[15] | 杜岩英; 雷雳; 马晓辉. 身体映像影响因素的生态系统分析[J]. 心理科学进展, 2010, 18(3): 480-486. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||