Situational evolution of the relationship between warmth and competence in intergroup evaluation: Impact of evaluating intention and behavioral outcomes
Bin ZUO1,2,Fangfang WEN1,2(),Yang WU3,Taotao DAI1
1 School of Psychology, Center for Studies of Social Psychology, Central China Normal University, Wuhan 430079, China 2 Key Laboratory of Adolescent Cyberpsychology and Behavior, Ministry of Education, Central China Normal University, Wuhan 430079, China 3 School of Marxism, Huazhong University of Science and Technology, Wuhan 430074, China
Two critical dimensions, warmth and competence, feature prominently in people’s social cognitive processes, and there is a great deal of research examining the nature of these two dimensions and their relationships. Recently, researchers have become increasingly interested in the situational dependency that may characterize people’s perceptions of these two dimensions. However, past research has only considered the effect of simple and repeated interactions, leaving the nature and outcomes of the interactions unexamined. According to social interdependence theory, the competitive or cooperative interactions between groups may elicit disparate downstream effects on perceptions of group members. Various theoretical perspectives converged on the notion that the outcomes of previous interaction sessions may exercise a crucial but differential impact on the warmth and competence rating of outgroup members. The current work aimed to investigate the joint effect of the competitive/collaborative context and the interaction outcomes on people’s warmth and competence ratings as well as the relationship between the two dimensions.
The current research explored this problem in two studies. We investigated how the result of success or failure affects intergroup evaluation in different situations of competition and cooperation. A total of 496 undergraduates were recruited. In all four of our experiments, we used the Distant Planetary Paradigm and imagined a four-stage encounter with an alien prospecting team, with the characteristics and outcomes of each session manipulated according to experimental design. After each stage of the encounter, participants rated their perceptions of the warmth and competence of the alien groups.
The results showed that: (1) a positive relationship between warmth and competence ratings was observed when participants inferred competence from warmth-related information, and when they inferred warmth from competence-related information, the relationship reversed; (2) during the first stage of interaction, knowing the coaction intention of the outgroup exerted an overarching “centralizing” effect over people’s perception of the member of that outgroup, with the ratings on both dimensions showing a compensatory tendency; and (3) the effect of interaction outcome was contingent on the coaction contexts, such that in a competition context, ratings of warmth and competence exhibited an anti-parallel tendency as the four-stage interactions unfolded, such that a successful outcome may cause a rising-warmth-falling-competence tendency and a failure outcome may cause a warmth-falling-competence rising tendency, whereas in a cooperation context, ratings on the two dimensions evolved in a parallel fashion, such that a successful cooperation may cause an overall increase in ratings on both dimensions and a failed cooperation may cause overall falling ratings on both dimensions.
In conclusion, the current research is the first to explore the situational evolution of the relationship between warmth and competence ratings in intergroup evaluations, and has important implications for the future inter-group relationship research. This line of inquiry makes a novel contribution to the field by examining how social relationships within joint activities could influence behavior and intentions toward members of an outgroup.
佐斌,温芳芳,吴漾,代涛涛. (2018). 群际评价中热情与能力关系的情境演变:评价意图与结果的作用. 心理学报, 50(10): 1180-1196.
Bin ZUO,Fangfang WEN,Yang WU,Taotao DAI. (2018). Situational evolution of the relationship between warmth and competence in intergroup evaluation: Impact of evaluating intention and behavioral outcomes. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 50(10), 1180-1196.
Abele A. E., Bruckmüller S., & Wojciszke B . ( 2014). You are so kind-and I am kind and smart: Actor-observer differences in the interpretation of on-going behavior. Polish Psychological Bulletin, 45( 4), 394-401.
[2]
Abele A. E., Hauke N., Peters K., Louvet E., Szymkow A., & Duan Y . ( 2016). Facets of the fundamental content dimensions: agency with competence and assertiveness— communion with warmth and morality. Frontiers in Psychology,7, 1810.
[3]
Asch S.E . ( 1946). Forming impression of personality. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 41( 3), 258-290.
[4]
Bakan D .( 1966). The duality of human existence: an essay on psychology and religion. Chicago, IL: Rand McNally.
[5]
Bettelheim B., & Janowitz M . ( 1950). Dynamics of prejudice. New York: Harper
[6]
Bye H.H., &Herrebrøden H .( 2017). Emotions as mediators of the stereotype-discrimination relationship: A bias map replication. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 136843021769437.
[7]
Cacault M. P., Goette L., Lalive R., & Thoenig M . ( 2015). Do we harm others even if we don't need to? Frontiers in Psychology, 6( 729), 1-9.
pmid: 26082737
[8]
Capozza D., Bernardo G. A. D., & Falvo R . ( 2017). Intergroup contact and outgroup humanization: Is the causal relationship uni- or bidirectional? Plos One, 12( 1), e0170554.
pmid: 5261613
[9]
Chen Y., & Li S.X . ( 2009). Group identity and social preferences. American Economic Review, 99( 1), 431-457.
[10]
Cislak A., & Wojciszke B .( 2008). Agency and communion are inferred from actions serving interests of self or others. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38( 7), 1103-1110.
[11]
Clough P.D., & Bates J . ( 2017). Competent men and warm women: Gender stereotypes and backlash in image search results. Computer and Human Interaction, 5, 6620-6631.
[12]
Cuddy A. J. C., Fiske S. T., & Glick P . ( 2007). The BIAS map: behaviors from intergroup affect and stereotypes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,92( 4), 631-648.
pmid: 17469949
[13]
Dai T., Zuo B., & Wen F. F . ( 2014). The compensation effect between warmth and competence in social cognition. Advance in Psychological Science,22( 3), 502-511.
Dorrough A. R., Glöckner A., Hellmann D. M., & Ebert I . ( 2015). The development of ingroup favoritism in repeated social dilemmas. Frontiers in Psychology , 6: 476.
pmid: 4411968
[15]
Dovidio J. F., Love A., Schellhaas F. M. H., & Hewstone M . ( 2017). Reducing intergroup bias through intergroup contact: twenty years of progress and future directions. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 20( 5), 136843021771205.
[16]
Durrheim K., Quayle M., Tredoux C. G., Titlestad K., & Tooke L . ( 2016). Investigating the evolution of ingroup favoritism using a minimal group interaction paradigm: The effects of inter-and intragroup interdependence. Plos One,11( 11), e0165974.
[17]
Fiske S.T . ( 1993). Controlling other people: The impact of power on stereotyping. American Psychologist, 48( 6), 621-628
pmid: 8328729
[18]
Fiske S. T., Cuddy A. J. C., & Glick P . ( 2007). Universal dimensions of social cognition: Warmth and competence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11( 2), 77-83.
pmid: 17188552
[19]
Fiske S. T., Cuddy A. J. C., Glick P., & Xu J . ( 2002). A model of (often mixed) stereotype content: competence and warmth respectively follow from perceived status and competition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82( 6), 878-902.
[20]
Fiske S.T . ( 2018). Stereotype content: Warmth and competence endure. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 27( 2), 67-73.
[21]
Hayward L. E., Tropp L. R., Hornsey M. J., & Barlow F. K . ( 2017). Toward a comprehensive understanding of intergroup contact descriptions and mediators of positive and negative contact among majority and minority groups. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 43( 3), 347-364.
[22]
Hoffman C., & Hurst N .( 1990). Gender stereotypes: perception or rationalization? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58( 2), 197-208.
[23]
Holoien D.S., & Fiske S.T . ( 2013). Downplaying positive impressions: Compensation between warmth and competence in impression management. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49( 1), 33-41.
[24]
Johnson D.W .( 2003). Social interdependence: interrelationships among theory, research, and practice. American Psychologist, 58( 11), 934-945.
pmid: 14609388
[25]
Judd C. M., James-Hawkins L., Yzerbyt V., & Kashima Y . ( 2005). Fundamental dimensions of social judgment: Understanding the relations between judgments of competence and warmth. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89( 6), 899-913.
[26]
Kanas A., Scheepers P., & Sterkens C . ( 2017). Positive and negative contact and attitudes towards the religious out-group: testing the contact hypothesis in conflict and non-conflict regions of Indonesia and the Philippines. Social Science Research, 63, 95-110.
[27]
Kervyn N., Bergsieker H. B., & Fiske S. T . ( 2012). The innuendo effect: hearing the positive but inferring the negative. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48( 1), 77-85.
pmid: 26023243
[28]
Kervyn N., Yzerbyt V. Y., & Judd C. M . ( 2011). When compensation guides inferences: indirect and implicit measures of the compensation effect. European Journal of Social Psychology, 41( 2), 144-150.
[29]
Kervyn N., Yzerbyt V. Y., Judd C. M., & Nunes A . ( 2009). A question of compensation: The social life of the fundamental dimensions of social perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96( 4), 828-842.
pmid: 19309205
[30]
Meeusen C., Barlow F. K., & Sibley C. G . ( 2017). Generalized and specific components of prejudice: The decomposition of intergroup context effects. European Journal of Social Psychology, 47( 4), 443-456.
[31]
Rast III D. E., Gaffney A. M., & Yang F. G . ( 2017). The effect of stereotype content on intergroup uncertainty and interactions. The Journal of Social Psychology, 22, 1-10.
pmid: 29166218
[32]
Schaller M., Park J. H., & Faulkner J . ( 2003). Prehistoric dangers and contemporary prejudices. European Review of Social Psychology, 14( 1), 105-137.
[33]
Sherif M., Harvey O. J., White B. J., Hood W. R., & Sherif C. W . ( 1961). Intergroup cooperation and conflict: The robbers cave experiment. Norman, OK: University ofOklahoma Book Exchange.
[34]
Ufkes E. G., Otten S., van der Zee K. I., Giebels E., & Dovidio J. F . ( 2011). The effect of stereotype content on anger versus contempt in “day-to-day” conflicts. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 15( 1), 57-74.
[35]
Uskul A., & Over H . ( 2017). Culture, social interdependence, and ostracism. Current Directions in Psychological Science,26( 4), 371-376.
[36]
Vezzali L., Turner R., Capozza D., & Trifiletti E . ( 2018). Does intergroup contact affect personality? a longitudinal study on the bidirectional relationship between intergroup contact and personality traits. European Journal of Social Psychology, 48( 2), 159-173.
[37]
Wen F., Zuo B., Wu Y., Dong X., & Wang W . ( 2016). Reducing the effect of stereotype threat: The role of coaction contexts and regulatory fit. Social Psychology of Education, 19( 3), 607-626.
[38]
Wolgast A., & Fischer N .( 2017). You are not alone: Colleague support and goal-oriented cooperation as resources to reduce teachers’ stress. Social Psychology of Education, 20( 1), 97-114.
[39]
Yzerbyt V. Y., Kervyn N., & Judd C. M . ( 2008). Compensation versus halo: The unique relations between the fundamental dimensions of social judgment. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34( 8), 1110-1123.
[40]
Zuo B., Dai T. T., Wen F. F., & Suo Y.X . ( 2015). The big two model in social cognition. Journal of Psychological Science, 38( 4), 1019-1023.
Zuo B., Dai T. T., Wen F. F., & Teng T. T . ( 2014). The relationship between warmth and competence in social cognition. Advances in Psychological Science, 22( 9), 1467-1474.