Please wait a minute...
Advances in Psychological Science    2020, Vol. 28 Issue (6) : 935-944     DOI: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2020.00935
Regular Articles |
The multidimensional spatial representation of time: Dissociations on its ontogenetic origin and activation mechanism
HE Tingyu,DING Yi,LI Haokun,CHENG Xiaorong,FAN Zhao,DING Xianfeng()
School of Psychology, Central China Normal University, Wuhan 430079, China
Download: PDF(591 KB)   HTML
Export: BibTeX | EndNote | Reference Manager | ProCite | RefWorks    
Abstract  

A growing body of evidence suggested that time can be represented separately either on the lateral, sagittal or vertical axis. However, how these mental time lines originate and be activated remains controversial. Recent evidence supports that the lateral mental time line mainly originates from sensorimotor experience associated with reading/writing habits, and the activation may be a low-level sensorimotor mechanism. In contrast, the sagittal/vertical mental time line mainly originates from spatial metaphor in language, and the activation may be a high-level semantic mechanism. The future study should focus more on the diversity of origins, the gene basis, the neural mechanism and the analogy between mental time lines and mental number lines.

Keywords mental time line      origin      activation      embodied simulation      metaphor mapping     
ZTFLH:  B842  
Corresponding Authors: Xianfeng DING     E-mail: xianfengding@mail.ccnu.edu.cn
Issue Date: 22 April 2020
Service
E-mail this article
E-mail Alert
RSS
Articles by authors
Tingyu HE
Yi DING
Haokun LI
Xiaorong CHENG
Zhao FAN
Xianfeng DING
Cite this article:   
Tingyu HE,Yi DING,Haokun LI, et al. The multidimensional spatial representation of time: Dissociations on its ontogenetic origin and activation mechanism[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2020, 28(6): 935-944.
URL:  
http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlkxjz/EN/10.3724/SP.J.1042.2020.00935     OR     http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlkxjz/EN/Y2020/V28/I6/935
[1] 陈玲 . (2018). 虚拟感觉运动经验对不同维度心理时间线的分离影响 (硕士学位论文). 华中师范大学, 武汉.
[2] 冯宁 . (2015). 心理时间线:具身模拟和概念隐喻的双重进程? (硕士学位论文). 华中师范大学, 武汉.
[3] 刘丽虹, 张积家 . (2009). 时间的空间隐喻对汉语母语者时间认知的影响. 外语教学与研究:外国语文双月刊, 4, 266-271.
[4] 马佳 . (2017). 空间-时间联合编码效应:基于极性对应理论的解释 (硕士学位论文). 河北师范大学, 石家庄.
[5] 张恩涛, 方杰, 林文毅, 罗俊龙 . (2013). 抽象概念表征的具身认知观. 心理科学进展, 21(3), 429-436.
[6] Anelli, F., Peters-Founshtein, G., Shreibman, Y., Moreh, E., Forlani, C., Frassinetti, F., & Arzy, S . (2018). Nature and nurture effects on the spatiality of the mental time line. Scientific Reports, 8(1), 11710.
[7] Barsalou, L. W . (2008). Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59(1), 617-645.
[8] Barsalou, L. W . (2009). Simulation, situated conceptualization, and prediction. Philosophical Transactions: Biological Sciences, 364(1521), 1281-1289.
[9] Basso, G., Nichelli, P., Frassinetti, F., & Di Pellegrino, G . (1996). Time perception in a neglected space. Neuroreport, 7(13), 2111-2114.
[10] Bender, A., & Beller, S . (2014). Mapping spatial frames of reference onto time: A review of theoretical accounts and empirical findings. Cognition, 132(3), 342-382.
[11] Bonato, M., Zorzi, M., & Umiltà, C . (2012). When time is space: Evidence for a mental time line. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 36(10), 2257-2273.
[12] Boroditsky, L . (2000). Metaphoric structuring: Understanding time through spatial metaphors. Cognition, 75(1), 1-28.
[13] Boroditsky, L . (2001). Does language shape thought? Mandarin and English speakers' conceptions of time. Cognitive Psychology, 43(1), 1-22.
[14] Boroditsky, L . (2011). How languages construct time. In S. Dehaene & E. Brannon (Eds.), Space, time and number in the brain: Searching for the foundations of mathematical thought ( pp. 333-341). San Diego, CA: Elsevier.
[15] Boroditsky, L., Fuhrman, O., & McCormick, K . (2011). Do English and Mandarin speakers think about time differently? Cognition, 118(1), 123-129.
[16] Bottini, R., Crepaldi, D., Casasanto, D., Crollen, V., & Collignon, O . (2015). Space and time in the sighted and blind. Cognition, 141, 67-72.
[17] Bueti, D., & Walsh, V . (2009). The parietal cortex and the representation of time, space, number and other magnitudes. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 364(1525), 1831-1840.
[18] Buzsáki, G., & Llinás, R . (2017). Space and time in the brain. Science, 358(6362), 482-485.
[19] Casasanto, D . (2008). Who's afraid of the big bad Whorf? Crosslinguistic differences in temporal language and thought. Language Learning, 58, 63-79.
[20] Casasanto, D., & Boroditsky, L . (2008). Time in the mind: Using space to think about time. Cognition, 106(2), 579-593.
[21] Casasanto, D., & Bottini, R . (2014). Mirror reading can reverse the flow of time. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(2), 473-479.
[22] Chan, T. T., & Bergen, B . (2005). Writing direction influences spatial cognition. Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, 27, 412-417.
[23] Danckert, J., Ferber, S., Pun, C., Broderick, C., Striemer, C., Rock, S., & Stewart, D . (2007). Neglected time: Impaired temporal perception of multi-second intervals in unilateral neglect. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(10), 1706-1720.
[24] de la Fuente, J., Santiago, J., Román, A., Dumitrache, C., & Casasanto, D . (2014). When you think about it, your past is in front of you: How culture shapes spatial conceptions of time. Psychological Science, 25(9), 1682-1690.
[25] de la Vega, I., Eikmeier, V., Ulrich, R., & Kaup, B . (2016). The mental timeline in a crossed-hands paradigm. Experimental Psychology, 63(6), 326-332.
[26] Ding, X., Cheng, X., Fan, Z., & Liu, H . (2015). Is elapsing time really recoded into spatial linear representation in working memory? Experimental Psychology, 62(1), 1-9.
[27] Ding, X., Feng, N., Cheng, X., Liu, H., & Fan, Z . (2015). Are past and future symmetric in mental time line? Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 208-215.
[28] Eikmeier, V., Alex-Ruf, S., Maienborn, C., & Ulrich, R . (2015). How strongly linked are mental time and space along the left-right axis? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 41(6), 1878-1883.
url: http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/xlm0000129
[29] Eikmeier, V., Hoppe, D., & Ulrich, R . (2015). Response mode does not modulate the space-time congruency effect: Evidence for a space-time mapping at a conceptual level. Acta Psychologica, 156, 162-167.
[30] Eikmeier, V., Schröter, H., Maienborn, C., Alex-Ruf, S., & Ulrich, R . (2013). Dimensional overlap between time and space. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20(6), 1120-1125.
[31] Fischer, M. H . (2008). Finger counting habits modulate spatial-numerical associations. Cortex, 44(4), 386-392.
[32] Fuhrman, O., & Boroditsky, L . (2010). Cross-cultural differences in mental representations of time: Evidence from an implicit nonlinguistic task. Cognitive Science, 34(8), 1430-1451.
[33] Fuhrman, O., McCormick, K., Chen, E., Jiang, H., Shu, D., Mao, S., & Boroditsky, L . (2011). How linguistic and cultural forces shape conceptions of time: English and Mandarin time in 3D. Cognitive Science, 35(7), 1305-1328.
[34] Gallese, V., & Lakoff, G . (2005). The brain's concepts: The role of the sensory-motor system in conceptual knowledge. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 22(3/4), 455-479.
[35] Glenberg, A. M., & Kaschak, M. P . (2002). Grounding language in action. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9(3), 558-565.
[36] Hartmann, M., Martarelli, C. S., Mast, F. W., & Stocker, K . (2014). Eye movements during mental time travel follow a diagonal line. Consciousness and Cognition, 30, 201-209.
[37] Hartmann, M., & Mast, F. W . (2012). Moving along the mental time line influences the processing of future related words. Consciousness and Cognition, 21(3), 1558-1562.
[38] Haspelmath, M . (1997). From Space to Time: Temporal Adverbials in the World’s Languages. München: Newcastle.
[39] Kong, F., & You, X . (2012). Space-time compatibility effects in the auditory modality. Experimental Psychology, 59(2), 82-87.
[40] Lai, V. T., & Boroditsky, L .(2013). The immediate and chronic influence of spatio-temporal metaphors on the mental representations of time in English, Mandarin, and Mandarin-English speakers. Frontiers in Psychology, 4(5), 142-151.
[41] Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M . (1980). The metaphorical structure of the human conceptual system. Cognitive Science, 4(2), 195-208.
[42] Landau, M. J., Meier, B. P., & Keefer, L. A . (2010). A metaphor-enriched social cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 136(6), 1045-1067.
[43] Levinson, S. C., & Majid, A . (2013). The island of time: Yélî Dnye, the language of Rossel Island. Frontiers in Psychology, 4(2), 61-71.
[44] Martarelli, C. S., Mast, F. W., & Hartmann, M . (2017). Time in the eye of the beholder: Gaze position reveals spatial- temporal associations during encoding and memory retrieval of future and past. Memory & Cognition, 45(1), 40-48.
[45] Miles, L. K., Tan, L., Noble, G. D., Lumsden, J., & Macrae, C. N . (2011). Can a mind have two time lines? Exploring space-time mapping in Mandarin and English speakers. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18(3), 598-604.
[46] Nava, E., Rinaldi, L., Bulf, H., & Macchi Cassia, V . (2017). Visual and proprioceptive feedback differently modulate the spatial representation of number and time in children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 161, 161-177.
[47] Núñez, R. E., Cooperrider, K., Doan, D., & Wassmann, J . (2012). Contours of time: Topographic construals of past, present, and future in the Yupno valley of Papua New Guinea. Cognition, 124(1), 25-35.
[48] Núñez, R. E., & Sweetser, E . (2006). With the future behind them: Convergent evidence from Aymara language and gesture in the crosslinguistic comparison of spatial construals of time. Cognitive Science, 30, 401-450.
url: http://doi.wiley.com/10.1207/s15516709cog0000_62
[49] Oliveri, M., Koch, G., Salerno, S., Torriero, S., Gerfo, E. L., & Caltagirone, C . (2009). Representation of time intervals in the right posterior parietal cortex: Implications for a mental time line. NeuroImage, 46(4), 1173-1179.
url: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1053811909002791
[50] Ouellet, M., Román, A., & Santiago, J . (2012). A multisensory interaction effect in the conceptual realm of time. Experimental Psychology, 59(4), 236-242.
url: http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000148
[51] Ouellet, M., Santiago, J., Israeli, Z., & Gabay, S . (2010). Is the future the right time? Experimental Psychology, 57(4), 308-314.
[52] Pitt, B., & Casasanto, D . (2014). Experiential origins of the mental number line. Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, 36, 1174-1179.
[53] Pitt, B., Scales, K., & Casasanto, D . (2018). Time and numbers on the fingers: Dissociating the mental timeline and mental number line. Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, 40, 890-895.
[54] Proctor, R. W., & Cho, Y. S . (2006). Polarity correspondence: A general principle for performance of speeded binary classification tasks. Psychological Bulletin, 132(3), 416-442.
[55] Radden, G . (2003). The metaphor time as space across languages. Zeitschrift für Interkulturellen Fremdsprachenunterricht, 8(2/3), 226-239.
[56] Rinaldi, L., Brugger, P., Bockisch, C. J., Bertolini, G., & Girelli, L . (2015). Keeping an eye on serial order: Ocular movements bind space and time. Cognition, 142, 291-298.
[57] Rinaldi, L., Locati, F., Parolin, L., Bernardi, N. F., & Girelli, L . (2016). Walking on a mental time line: Temporal processing affects step movements along the sagittal space. Cortex, 78, 170-173.
[58] Rinaldi, L., Vecchi, T., Fantino, M., Merabet, L. B., & Cattaneo, Z . (2018). The ego-moving metaphor of time relies on visual experience: No representation of time along the sagittal space in the blind. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 147(3), 444-450.
[59] Rolke, B., Fernández, S. R., Schmid, M., Walker, M., Lachmair, M., López, J. J. R., .. Vázquez, C . (2013). Priming the mental time-line: Effects of modality and processing mode. Cognitive Processing, 14(3), 231-244.
url: http://link.springer.com/10.1007/s10339-013-0537-5
[60] Santiago, J., & Lakens, D . (2015). Can conceptual congruency effects between number, time, and space be accounted for by polarity correspondence? Acta Psychologica, 156, 179-191.
url: https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0001691814002637
[61] Sell, A. J., & Kaschak, M. P . (2011). Processing time shifts affects the execution of motor responses. Brain and Language, 117(1), 39-44.
url: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2010.07.003
[62] Tillman, K. A., Tulagan, N., Fukuda, E., & Barner, D . (2018). The mental timeline is gradually constructed in childhood. Developmental Science, 21, 3879-3884.
[63] Tversky, B . (2011). Visualizing thought. Topics in Cognitive Science, 3(3), 499-535.
url: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1756-8765.2010.01113.x
[64] Ulrich, R., Eikmeier, V., de la Vega, I., Fernández, S. R., Alex-Ruf, S., & Maienborn, C . (2012). With the past behind and the future ahead: Back-to-front representation of past and future sentences. Memory & Cognition, 40(3), 483-495.
[65] Ulrich, R., & Maienborn, C . (2010). Left-right coding of past and future in language: The mental timeline during sentence processing. Cognition, 117(2), 126-138.
[66] Vallesi, A., Weisblatt, Y., Semenza, C., & Shaki, S . (2014). Cultural modulations of space-time compatibility effects. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21(3), 666-669.
url: http://link.springer.com/10.3758/s13423-013-0540-y
[67] Vannucci, M., Pelagatti, C., Chiorri, C., & Brugger, P . (2019). Space-time interaction: Visuo-spatial processing affects the temporal focus of mind wandering. Psychological Research, 83, 698-709.
url: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-018-1080-x
[68] von Sobbe, L., Scheifele, E., Maienborn, C., & Ulrich, R . (2019). The space-time congruency effect: A meta- analysis. Cognitive Science, 43(1), e12709.
[69] Walker, E., Bergen, B., & Núñez, R . (2013). Later events lie behind her, but not behind you: Compatibility effects for temporal sequences along the sagittal axis depend on perspective. Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, 35, 3729-3734.
[70] Walker, E. J., Bergen, B. K., & Núñez, R . (2017). The spatial alignment of time: Differences in alignment of deictic and sequence time along the sagittal and lateral axes. Acta Psychologica, 175, 13-20.
[71] Winter, B., Matlock, T., Shaki, S., & Fischer, M. H . (2015). Mental number space in three dimensions. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 57, 209-219.
url: https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0149763415002407
[72] Xiao, C., Zhao, M., & Chen, L . (2017). Both earlier times and the future are “front”: The distinction between time- and ego-reference-points in Mandarin speakers’ temporal representation. Cognitive Science, 42(3), 1026-1040.
[1] ZHANG Jing,LI Weihe,SHI Yanwei,ZHANG Nan,MA Hongyu. Work-related rumination and its “double-edged sword” effect[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2020, 28(2): 358-367.
[2] LIU Yuxin,CHEN Chen,ZHU Nan,ZHANG Jianwei,WANG Shuai. How does “one takes on the attributes of one's associates”? The past, present, and future of Trait Activation Theory[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2020, 28(1): 161-177.
[3] WANG Hongyu,CUI Zhisong,ZOU Chunlong,YU Jiali,ZHAO Di. Loyal or rebel? Employee bootleg innovation in Chinese context[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2019, 27(6): 975-989.
[4] JING Wei, LIU Ziqin.  The diminished attention to eye in the face processing of individuals with autism spectrum disorder[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2018, 26(3): 476-487.
[5] XU Zhan-Fei, XI Ju-Zhe.  Attitudinal ambivalence: Origins and coping strategies[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2018, 26(2): 331-343.
[6] GAO Zhihua; LU Zhongyi; CUI Xinying. What is the mechanism of negation processes? Introduction and commentary on psychological theories on negation[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2017, 25(3): 413-423.
[7] ZHOU Linshu, ZHAO Huaiyang, JIANG Cunmei.  Neural plasticity to musical performance training: A meta-analysis study[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2017, 25(11): 1877-1887.
[8] JIANG Ronghuan; LI Xiaodong. Developmental Reversals in False Memory: Why the More Grown up the More Fallible?[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2015, 23(8): 1371-1379.
[9] YANG Qun1; ZHANG Qingfanf1,2. Aging of Speech Production: Behavioral and Neural Mechanisms[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2015, 23(12): 2072-2084.
[10] HE Wen;SUN Yawen;LUO Junlong. Meta-stereotype and Its Impacts on Intergroup Relations[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2014, 22(8): 1294-1302.
[11] LI Yu;SHU Hua. The Brain Mechanisms and Functional Hypothesis of Default Mode Network and Its Clinical Application[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2014, 22(2): 234-249.
[12] ZHANG Lingyan;JIN Tan;TIAN Zhaoxia. On the Time Axis of Morpho-semantic Activation: Form-then-meaning or Form-with-meaning?[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2013, 21(8): 1382-1389.
[13] WANG Caiyu. The Connotation, Mechanism and Effects of Self-Brand Connections[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2013, 21(5): 922-933.
[14] WANG Xiaojuan;YANG Jianfeng. The Dynamic Neural Network and Its Development of Visual Word Processing[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2013, 21(2): 199-204.
[15] YIN Rong;QU Fang-Bing;YE Hao-Sheng. Research on Embodied Theories of Conceptual Representation[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2012, 20(9): 1372-1381.
Viewed
Full text


Abstract

Cited

  Shared   
  Discussed   
Copyright © Advances in Psychological Science
Support by Beijing Magtech