心理学报 ›› 2022, Vol. 54 ›› Issue (3): 221-235.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2022.00221
• 研究报告 • 下一篇
收稿日期:
2021-07-21
发布日期:
2022-01-25
出版日期:
2022-03-25
通讯作者:
傅世敏
E-mail:fusm@gzhu.edu.cn
基金资助:
ZHOU Zinuan, CHEN Yanzhang, FU Shimin()
Received:
2021-07-21
Online:
2022-01-25
Published:
2022-03-25
Contact:
FU Shimin
E-mail:fusm@gzhu.edu.cn
摘要:
以往研究表明, 预期机制和注意机制都能促进感知行为, 但两者以何种方式共同作用于感知行为仍然存在争议, 特别是, 对于预期主体在其中的作用尚不清楚。本研究采用空间提示以及视觉搜索相结合的范式, 通过4个实验, 考察了当被试对目标进行预期以及对分心物进行预期时, 空间预期对空间注意效应的不同影响。结果显示:(1)当目标为预期主体时, 预期对注意效应具有调节作用; (2)当分心物为预期主体时, 预期与注意的作用独立; (3)当目标为预期主体时, 通过刺激数增加而导致的任务难度变化不影响预期和注意之间的关系。这表明, 空间预期是否影响空间注意效应受制于预期主体——当预期主体为目标时, 预期和注意两者交互式地影响感知行为; 当预期主体为分心物时, 预期和注意独立地影响感知行为; 而且, 预期和注意之间的关系不受任务难度影响。
中图分类号:
周子暖, 陈颜璋, 傅世敏. (2022). 预期对注意的影响受制于被预期主体是目标还是分心物. 心理学报, 54(3), 221-235.
ZHOU Zinuan, CHEN Yanzhang, FU Shimin. (2022). The effects of expectation on attention are dependent on whether expectation is on the target or on the distractor?. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 54(3), 221-235.
图1 实验1流程图和实验条件示意图。a为实验1流程图, 在实验过程中先呈现500 ms中央注视点, 接着在左侧或右侧垂直呈现两个方格提示, 提示消失后在刺激屏呈现一个目标矩形与一个分心物矩形, 被试的任务为对目标矩形内的符号做辨别反应; b为实验条件的列举, 该例子中, 预期主体为目标, 目标为绿色矩形, 分心物为红色矩形, 注意提示出现在左侧, 且指导语引导被试预期上方空间(目标有80%的概率出现在上方两个位置之一)。
[1] |
Alink, A., Schwiedrzik, C. M., Kohler, A., Singer, W., & Muckli, L. (2010). Stimulus predictability reduces responses in primary visual cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience, 30(8), 2960-2966.
doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3730-10.2010 URL |
[2] |
Becker, S. I., Lewis, A. J., & Axtens, J. E. (2017). Top-down knowledge modulates onset capture in a feedforward manner. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 24(2), 436-446.
doi: 10.3758/s13423-016-1134-2 URL |
[3] | Beer, A. L., & Röder, B. (2004). Unimodal and crossmodal effects of endogenous attention to visual and auditory motion. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 4(2), 230-240. |
[4] |
Beer, A. L., & Röder, B. (2005). Attending to visual or auditory motion affects perception within and across modalities: An event-related potential study. European Journal of Neuroscience, 21(4), 1116-1130.
doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.03927.x URL |
[5] |
Carrasco, M. (2011). Visual attention: The past 25 years. Vision Research, 51(13), 1484-1525.
doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2011.04.012 pmid: 21549742 |
[6] |
Chang, S., & Egeth, H. E. (2019). Enhancement and suppression flexibly guide attention. Psychological Science, 30(12), 1724-1732.
doi: 10.1177/0956797619878813 pmid: 31693453 |
[7] | Cheadle, S., Egner, T., Wyart, V., Wu, C., & Summerfield, C. (2015). Feature expectation heightens visual sensitivity during fine orientation discrimination. Journal of Vision, 15(14), 14. |
[8] |
Chen, A., Wang, A., Wang, T., Tang, X., & Zhang, M. (2017). Behavioral oscillations in visual attention modulated by task difficulty. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1630.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01630 URL |
[9] |
Clark, A. (2013). Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(3), 181-204.
doi: 10.1017/S0140525X12000477 pmid: 23663408 |
[10] |
de Lange, F. P., Heilbron, M., & Kok, P. (2018). How do expectations shape perception?. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(9), 764-779.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2018.06.002 URL |
[11] |
Egly, R., Driver, J., & Rafal, R. D. (1994). Shifting visual attention between objects and locations: Evidence from normal and parietal lesion subjects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 123(2), 161-177.
doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.123.2.161 URL |
[12] | Feldman, H., & Friston, K. (2010). Attention, uncertainty, and free-energy. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 4, 215. |
[13] |
Gaspelin, N., & Luck, S. J. (2018). The role of inhibition in avoiding distraction by salient stimuli. Trends in Cognitive Science, 22(1), 79-92.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2017.11.001 URL |
[14] |
Gilbert, C. D., & Li, W. (2013). Top-down influences on visual processing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 14(5), 350-363.
doi: 10.1038/nrn3476 URL |
[15] |
Heilbron, M., & Chait, M. (2018). Great expectations: Is there evidence for predictive coding in auditory cortex? Neuroscience, 389, 54-73.
doi: S0306-4522(17)30547-X pmid: 28782642 |
[16] |
Huang, L., & Pashler, H. (2005). Attention capacity and task difficulty in visual search. Cognition, 94(3), B101-111.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2004.06.006 URL |
[17] | Jiang, J., Summerfield, C., & Egner, T. (2013). Attention sharpens the distinction between expected and unexpected percepts in the visual brain. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33(47), 18438-18447. |
[18] | Jiang, J., Summerfield, C., & Egner, T. (2016). Visual prediction error spreads across object features in human visual cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience, 36(50), 12746-12763. |
[19] |
John-Saaltink, E. S., Utzerath, C., Kok, P., Lau, H. C., & de Lange, F. P. (2015). Expectation suppression in early visual cortex depends on task set. PLoS ONE, 10(6), e0131172.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0131172 URL |
[20] |
Kok, P., Brouwer, G. J., van Gerven, M. A. J., & de Lange, F. P. (2013). Prior expectations bias sensory representations in visual cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33(41), 16275-16284.
doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0742-13.2013 URL |
[21] | Kok, P., Mostert, P., & de Lange, F. D. (2017). Prior expectations induce prestimulus sensory templates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 114(39), 10473-10478. |
[22] |
Kok, P., Rahnev, D., Jehee, J. F., Lau, H. C., & de Lange, F. P. (2012). Attention reverses the effect of prediction in silencing sensory signals. Cerebral Cortex, 22(9), 2197-2206.
doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr310 URL |
[23] |
Li, Z., Goschl, F., & Yang, G. (2020). Dissociated neural mechanisms of target and distractor processing facilitated by expectations. The Journal of Neuroscience, 40(10), 1997-1999.
doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2562-19.2020 URL |
[24] |
Marzecova, A., Schettino, A., Widmann, A., SanMiguel, I., Kotz, S. A., & Schroger, E. (2018). Attentional gain is modulated by probabilistic feature expectations in a spatial cueing task: ERP evidence. Scientific Reports, 8(1), 54.
doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-18347-1 URL |
[25] |
Marzecova, A., Widmann, A., SanMiguel, I., Kotz, S. A., & Schroger, E. (2017). Interrelation of attention and prediction in visual processing: Effects of task-relevance and stimulus probability. Biological Psychology, 125, 76-90.
doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2017.02.009 URL |
[26] | Petilli, M. A., Marini, F., & Daini, R. (2020). Distractor context manipulation in visual search: How expectations modulate proactive control. Cognition, 196, 104129. |
[27] |
Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32(1), 3-25.
doi: 10.1080/00335558008248231 URL |
[28] |
Rafal, R. D., Posner, M. I., Friedman, J. H., Inhoff, A. W., & Bernstein, E. (1988). Orienting of visual attention in progressive supranuclear palsy. Brain, 111(2), 267-280.
doi: 10.1093/brain/111.2.267 URL |
[29] |
Rauss, K., Schwartz, S., & Pourtois, G. (2011). Top-down effects on early visual processing in humans: A predictive coding framework. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(5), 1237-1253.
doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.12.011 URL |
[30] |
Reimer, C. B., & Schubert, T. (2019). More insight into the interplay of response selection and visual attention in dual-tasks: Masked visual search and response selection are performed in parallel. Psychological Research, 83(3), 459-475.
doi: 10.1007/s00426-017-0906-2 URL |
[31] |
Rungratsameetaweemana, N., Itthipuripat, S., Salazar, A., & Serences, J. T. (2018). Expectations do not alter early sensory processing during perceptual decision-making. The Journal of Neuroscience, 38(24), 5632-5648.
doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3638-17.2018 URL |
[32] |
Rungratsameetaweemana, N., & Serences, J. T. (2019). Dissociating the impact of attention and expectation on early sensory processing. Current Opinion in Psychology, 29, 181-186.
doi: S2352-250X(18)30175-1 pmid: 31022561 |
[33] | Smout, C. A., Garrido, M. I., & Mattingley, J. B. (2019). Global effects of feature-based attention depend on surprise. NeuroImage, 215, 116785. |
[34] | Su, Y., Huang, W., Yang, N., Yan, K., Ding, Y., & Qu, Z. (2020). Attentional capture by a color singleton is stronger at spatially relevant than irrelevant locations: Evidence from an ERP study. Psychophysiology, 57(10), e13640. |
[35] |
Summerfield, C., & de Lange, F. P. (2014). Expectation in perceptual decision making: Neural and computational mechanisms. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 15(11), 745-756.
doi: 10.1038/nrn3838 URL |
[36] |
Summerfield, C., & Egner, T. (2016). Feature-based attention and feature-based expectation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(6), 401-404.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2016.03.008 URL |
[37] |
van Moorselaar, D., Daneshtalab, N., & Slagter, H. A. (2021). Neural mechanisms underlying distractor inhibition on the basis of feature and/or spatial expectations. Cortex, 137, 232-250.
doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2021.01.010 pmid: 33640854 |
[38] |
van Moorselaar, D., & Slagter, H. A. (2019). Learning what is irrelevant or relevant: Expectations facilitate distractor inhibition and target facilitation through distinct neural mechanisms. The Journal of Neuroscience, 39(35), 6953-6967.
doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0593-19.2019 URL |
[39] |
Wang, B., & Theeuwes, J. (2018). How to inhibit a distractor location? Statistical learning versus active, top-down suppression. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 80(4), 860-870.
doi: 10.3758/s13414-018-1493-z URL |
[40] |
Wang, B., van Driel, J., Ort, E., & Theeuwes, J. (2019). Anticipatory distractor suppression elicited by statistical regularities in visual search. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 31(10), 1535-1548.
doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01433 URL |
[41] | Wyart, V., Nobre, A. C., & Summerfield, C. (2012). Dissociable prior influences of signal probability and relevance on visual contrast sensitivity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109(9), 3593-3598. |
[42] |
Yantis, S., & Jonides, J. (1984). Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: Evidence from visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10(5), 601-621.
doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.10.5.601 URL |
[43] |
Zareian, B., Maboudi, K., Daliri, M. R., Abrishami Moghaddam, H., Treue, S., & Esghaei, M. (2020). Attention strengthens across-trial pre-stimulus phase coherence in visual cortex, enhancing stimulus processing. Scientific Reports, 10(1), 4837.
doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-61359-7 pmid: 32179777 |
[44] | Zhang, F., Chen, A., Dong, B., Wang, A., & Zhang, M. (2021). Rapid disengagement hypothesis and signal suppression hypothesis of visual attentional capture. Advances in Psychological Science, 29(1), 44-45. |
[张帆, 陈艾睿, 董波, 王爱君, 张明. (2021). 视觉注意捕获的快速脱离假说与信号抑制假说. 心理科学进展, 29(1), 44-45.] | |
[45] |
Zuanazzi, A., & Noppeney, U. (2018). Additive and interactive effects of spatial attention and expectation on perceptual decisions. Scientific Reports, 8(1), 6732.
doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-24703-6 pmid: 29712941 |
[46] |
Zuanazzi, A., & Noppeney, U. (2019). Distinct neural mechanisms of spatial attention and expectation guide perceptual inference in a multisensory World. The Journal of Neuroscience, 39(12), 2301-2312.
doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2873-18.2019 URL |
[1] | 陈晓宇, 杜媛媛, 刘强. 积极情绪提高背景线索学习的适应性[J]. 心理学报, 2022, 54(12): 1481-1490. |
[2] | 唐晓雨, 崔鑫忠, 高敏, 袁梦莹. 视听时、空一致性对Pip-and-Pop效应的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2022, 54(11): 1310-1324. |
[3] | 李杰, 杨悦, 赵婧. 汉语发展性阅读障碍儿童视觉同时性加工技能子成分的发展及其与阅读的关系[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(8): 821-836. |
[4] | 孙俊才, 寻凤娇, 刘萍, 张文海. 高善良特质在情绪调节行动控制中的内隐优势[J]. 心理学报, 2019, 51(7): 781-794. |
[5] | 杨海波, 刘和珺, 章鹏, 李量. 掩蔽刺激对目标识别加工的作用:来自fNIRS的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2019, 51(11): 1187-1197. |
[6] | 刘璐, 闫国利. 聋人阅读中的副中央凹视觉注意增强效应——来自消失文本的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2018, 50(7): 715-726. |
[7] | 袁小钧, 崔晓霞, 曹正操, 阚红, 王晓, 汪亚珉. 虚拟仿真场景中威胁性视觉刺激搜索的注意偏向效应 *[J]. 心理学报, 2018, 50(6): 622-636. |
[8] | 李毕琴, 李玲, 王爱君, 张明. 言语工作记忆内容在语义水平的注意捕获[J]. 心理学报, 2018, 50(5): 483-493. |
[9] | 谭群, 尹月阳, 刘燊, 韩尚锋, 徐强, 张林. 自我积极表情加工优势效应:来自ERPs的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2018, 50(10): 1120-1130. |
[10] | 李杨卓, 钱浩悦, 朱敏, 高湘萍. 自我相关信息对视觉搜索主动抑制的易化作用[J]. 心理学报, 2018, 50(1): 28-35. |
[11] | 王健, 袁立伟, 张芷, 王诗忠. 视觉预期和注意指向对姿势和动作肌肉 预期和补偿姿势调节的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(7): 920-927. |
[12] | 张豹;胡岑楼;黄赛. 认知控制在工作记忆表征引导注意中的作用:来自眼动的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2016, 48(9): 1105-1118. |
[13] | 魏柳青;张学民;李永娜;马玉. 视听通道双任务对多目标追踪的影响:干扰还是促进?[J]. 心理学报, 2014, 46(6): 727-739. |
[14] | 牟兵兵;宛小昂. 视觉搜索中的情绪干扰项预习效应[J]. 心理学报, 2014, 46(11): 1603-1612. |
[15] | 林欧;王正科;孟祥芝. 汉语发展性阅读障碍儿童的视知觉学习[J]. 心理学报, 2013, 45(7): 762-772. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||