心理学报 ›› 2020, Vol. 52 ›› Issue (5): 572-583.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2020.00572
收稿日期:
2019-09-09
发布日期:
2020-03-26
出版日期:
2020-05-25
通讯作者:
孟迎芳
E-mail:mengyf1978@126.com
基金资助:
HUANG Fajie, MENG Yingfang(), YAN Ying
Received:
2019-09-09
Online:
2020-03-26
Published:
2020-05-25
Contact:
MENG Yingfang
E-mail:mengyf1978@126.com
摘要:
以往研究关于提取阶段的干扰是否会影响内隐记忆存在着异义, 其中一个重要因素可能源于所采用的内隐记忆测验类型的差异。本研究采用学习-测验范式, 通过4个实验, 分别考察了提取干扰对识别式知觉内隐测验、识别式概念内隐测验、产生式知觉内隐测验和产生式概念内隐测验的影响, 以期对提取干扰和内隐记忆之间的关系有着更全面的了解。结果表明: (1)词汇判断任务(识别式知觉)和语义分类任务(识别式概念)的启动效应在提取干扰下消失了, (2)而产生式词汇判断任务(产生式知觉)和产生式语义分类任务(产生式概念)在提取干扰下仍发现了明显的启动效应, 但与无干扰条件相比, 启动效应量也有着明显的减少。由此可见, 不同类型的内隐记忆都会受到提取干扰的影响, 相比于产生式启动, 识别式启动更容易受到提取干扰的破坏。
中图分类号:
黄发杰, 孟迎芳, 严颖. (2020). 提取干扰对不同类型内隐记忆的影响. 心理学报, 52(5), 572-583.
HUANG Fajie, MENG Yingfang, YAN Ying. (2020). The effects of retrieval interference on different types of implicit memory. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 52(5), 572-583.
指标 | 条件 | 知觉编码旧词 | 概念编码旧词 | 新词 | 知觉编码启动量 | 概念编码启动量 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
反应时(ms) | 无干扰 | 639 ± 12 | 646 ± 9 | 681 ± 12 | 43 ± 8 | 35 ± 6 |
有干扰 | 1229 ± 74 | 1250 ± 71 | 1260 ± 68 | 32 ± 18 | 10 ± 19 | |
正确率(%) | 无干扰 | 0.99 ± 0.02 | 0.98 ± 0.03 | 0.95 ± 0.04 | 0.04 ± 0.04 | 0.04 ± 0.04 |
有干扰 | 0.96 ± 0.04 | 0.94 ± 0.06 | 0.94 ± 0.04 | 0.01 ± 0.05 | 0.01 ± 0.04 | |
ACs | 无干扰 | 86.92 ± 1.05 | 85.65 ± 1.16 | 77.83 ± 1.01 | 9.09 ± 1.25 | 7.82 ± 1.28 |
有干扰 | 80.30 ± 1.31 | 78.99 ± 1.64 | 77.79 ± 1.20 | 2.51 ± 1.51 | 1.20 ± 1.79 |
表1 词汇判断任务各条件下的反应时和正确率指标
指标 | 条件 | 知觉编码旧词 | 概念编码旧词 | 新词 | 知觉编码启动量 | 概念编码启动量 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
反应时(ms) | 无干扰 | 639 ± 12 | 646 ± 9 | 681 ± 12 | 43 ± 8 | 35 ± 6 |
有干扰 | 1229 ± 74 | 1250 ± 71 | 1260 ± 68 | 32 ± 18 | 10 ± 19 | |
正确率(%) | 无干扰 | 0.99 ± 0.02 | 0.98 ± 0.03 | 0.95 ± 0.04 | 0.04 ± 0.04 | 0.04 ± 0.04 |
有干扰 | 0.96 ± 0.04 | 0.94 ± 0.06 | 0.94 ± 0.04 | 0.01 ± 0.05 | 0.01 ± 0.04 | |
ACs | 无干扰 | 86.92 ± 1.05 | 85.65 ± 1.16 | 77.83 ± 1.01 | 9.09 ± 1.25 | 7.82 ± 1.28 |
有干扰 | 80.30 ± 1.31 | 78.99 ± 1.64 | 77.79 ± 1.20 | 2.51 ± 1.51 | 1.20 ± 1.79 |
指标 | 条件 | 知觉编码旧词 | 概念编码旧词 | 新词 | 知觉编码启动量 | 概念编码启动量 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
反应时(ms) | 无干扰 | 718 ± 29 | 701 ± 28 | 742 ± 29 | 24 ± 7 | 40 ± 8 |
有干扰 | 1186 ± 61 | 1197 ± 63 | 1134 ± 58 | -51 ± 19 | -62 ± 23 | |
正确率(%) | 无干扰 | 0.94 ± 0.01 | 0.91 ± 0.01 | 0.89 ± 0.01 | 0.05 ± 0.01 | 0.02 ± 0.02 |
有干扰 | 0.92 ± 0.02 | 0.91 ± 0.02 | 0.90 ± 0.02 | 0.02 ± 0.01 | 0.01 ± 0.01 | |
ACs | 无干扰 | 77.35 ± 1.52 | 74.93 ± 1.85 | 72.23 ± 1.62 | 5.12 ± 1.31 | 2.70 ± 1.93 |
有干扰 | 76.97 ± 2.16 | 75.65 ± 1.98 | 74.53 ± 2.09 | 2.43 ± 2.03 | 1.12 ± 1.65 |
表2 语义分类任务中各种条件下的平均反应时和正确率指标
指标 | 条件 | 知觉编码旧词 | 概念编码旧词 | 新词 | 知觉编码启动量 | 概念编码启动量 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
反应时(ms) | 无干扰 | 718 ± 29 | 701 ± 28 | 742 ± 29 | 24 ± 7 | 40 ± 8 |
有干扰 | 1186 ± 61 | 1197 ± 63 | 1134 ± 58 | -51 ± 19 | -62 ± 23 | |
正确率(%) | 无干扰 | 0.94 ± 0.01 | 0.91 ± 0.01 | 0.89 ± 0.01 | 0.05 ± 0.01 | 0.02 ± 0.02 |
有干扰 | 0.92 ± 0.02 | 0.91 ± 0.02 | 0.90 ± 0.02 | 0.02 ± 0.01 | 0.01 ± 0.01 | |
ACs | 无干扰 | 77.35 ± 1.52 | 74.93 ± 1.85 | 72.23 ± 1.62 | 5.12 ± 1.31 | 2.70 ± 1.93 |
有干扰 | 76.97 ± 2.16 | 75.65 ± 1.98 | 74.53 ± 2.09 | 2.43 ± 2.03 | 1.12 ± 1.65 |
指标 | 条件 | 知觉编码旧词 | 概念编码旧词 | 新词 | 知觉编码启动量 | 概念编码启动量 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
反应时(ms) | 无干扰 | 860 ± 23 | 851 ± 24 | 958 ± 31 | 98 ± 13 | 107 ± 12 |
有干扰 | 1227 ± 65 | 1236 ± 64 | 1279 ± 65 | 52 ± 15 | 43 ± 16 | |
正确率(%) | 无干扰 | 0.95 ± 0.01 | 0.95 ± 0.01 | 0.89 ± 0.01 | 0.06 ± 0.01 | 0.06 ± 0.01 |
有干扰 | 0.95 ± 0.01 | 0.96 ± 0.01 | 0.92 ± 0.01 | 0.03 ± 0.01 | 0.04 ± 0.01 | |
ACs | 无干扰 | 79.95 ± 1.57 | 80.01 ± 1.68 | 71.23 ± 1.16 | 8.72 ± 1.52 | 8.78 ± 1.28 |
有干扰 | 79.18 ± 1.34 | 81.94 ± 1.50 | 74.73 ± 1.21 | 4.46 ± 1.49 | 7.21 ± 1.63 |
表3 产生式词汇判断任务各种条件下的平均反应时和正确率指标
指标 | 条件 | 知觉编码旧词 | 概念编码旧词 | 新词 | 知觉编码启动量 | 概念编码启动量 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
反应时(ms) | 无干扰 | 860 ± 23 | 851 ± 24 | 958 ± 31 | 98 ± 13 | 107 ± 12 |
有干扰 | 1227 ± 65 | 1236 ± 64 | 1279 ± 65 | 52 ± 15 | 43 ± 16 | |
正确率(%) | 无干扰 | 0.95 ± 0.01 | 0.95 ± 0.01 | 0.89 ± 0.01 | 0.06 ± 0.01 | 0.06 ± 0.01 |
有干扰 | 0.95 ± 0.01 | 0.96 ± 0.01 | 0.92 ± 0.01 | 0.03 ± 0.01 | 0.04 ± 0.01 | |
ACs | 无干扰 | 79.95 ± 1.57 | 80.01 ± 1.68 | 71.23 ± 1.16 | 8.72 ± 1.52 | 8.78 ± 1.28 |
有干扰 | 79.18 ± 1.34 | 81.94 ± 1.50 | 74.73 ± 1.21 | 4.46 ± 1.49 | 7.21 ± 1.63 |
指标 | 条件 | 知觉编码旧词 | 概念编码旧词 | 新词 | 知觉编码启动量 | 概念编码启动量 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
反应时(ms) | 无干扰 | 992 ± 24 | 969 ± 21 | 1055 ± 26 | 63 ± 13 | 86 ± 10 |
有干扰 | 1347 ± 62 | 1320 ± 69 | 1403 ± 68 | 55 ± 19 | 83 ± 19 | |
正确率(%) | 无干扰 | 0.96 ± 0.01 | 0.94 ± 0.01 | 0.88 ± 0.01 | 0.08 ± 0.01 | 0.06 ± 0.01 |
有干扰 | 0.95 ± 0.01 | 0.94 ± 0.01 | 0.91 ± 0.01 | 0.05 ± 0.01 | 0.04 ± 0.01 | |
ACs | 无干扰 | 81.25 ± 1.39 | 77.77 ± 1.50 | 70.43 ± 1.15 | 10.82 ± 0.99 | 7.34 ± 1.20 |
有干扰 | 80.38 ± 1.65 | 78.59 ± 1.65 | 73.28 ± 1.37 | 7.10 ± 1.35 | 5.31 ± 1.60 |
表4 产生式语义分类任务各种条件下的平均反应时和正确率指标
指标 | 条件 | 知觉编码旧词 | 概念编码旧词 | 新词 | 知觉编码启动量 | 概念编码启动量 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
反应时(ms) | 无干扰 | 992 ± 24 | 969 ± 21 | 1055 ± 26 | 63 ± 13 | 86 ± 10 |
有干扰 | 1347 ± 62 | 1320 ± 69 | 1403 ± 68 | 55 ± 19 | 83 ± 19 | |
正确率(%) | 无干扰 | 0.96 ± 0.01 | 0.94 ± 0.01 | 0.88 ± 0.01 | 0.08 ± 0.01 | 0.06 ± 0.01 |
有干扰 | 0.95 ± 0.01 | 0.94 ± 0.01 | 0.91 ± 0.01 | 0.05 ± 0.01 | 0.04 ± 0.01 | |
ACs | 无干扰 | 81.25 ± 1.39 | 77.77 ± 1.50 | 70.43 ± 1.15 | 10.82 ± 0.99 | 7.34 ± 1.20 |
有干扰 | 80.38 ± 1.65 | 78.59 ± 1.65 | 73.28 ± 1.37 | 7.10 ± 1.35 | 5.31 ± 1.60 |
[1] |
Adams S. C., & Kiefer M . (2012). Testing the attentional boundary conditions of subliminal semantic priming: the influence of semantic and phonological task sets. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 1-12.
doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00001 URL pmid: 22279433 |
[2] |
Alipour A., Aerab-Sheybani K., & Akhondy N . (2012). Effects of handedness and depth of processing on the explicit and implicit memory. Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences, 32, 29-33.
doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2012.01.005 URL |
[3] |
Anderson S. F., Kelley K., & Maxwell S. E . (2017). Sample- size planning for more accurate statistical power: a method adjusting sample effect sizes for publication bias and uncertainty. Psychological Science, 28(11), 1547-1562.
doi: 10.1177/0956797617723724 URL pmid: 28902575 |
[4] |
Barnhardt T . (2005). Number of solutions effects in stem decision: support for the distinction between identification and production processes in priming. Memory, 13(7), 725-748.
doi: 10.1080/09658210444000368 URL pmid: 16191822 |
[5] |
Clarke A. J. B., & Butler L. T . (2008). Dissociating word stem completion and cued recall as a function of divided attention at retrieval. Memory, 16(7), 763-772.
doi: 10.1080/09658210802261116 URL pmid: 18720222 |
[6] |
D’Esposito M., Detre J. A., Alsop D. C., Shin R. K., Atlas S., & Grossman M . (1995). The neural basis of the central executive system of working memory. Nature, 378, 279-281.
doi: 10.1038/378279a0 URL pmid: 7477346 |
[7] |
Dew I. T. Z., & Cabeza R . (2011). The porous boundaries between explicit and implicit memory: behavioral and neural evidence. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1224(1), 174-190.
doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010.05946.x URL pmid: 21486300 |
[8] |
Gabrieli J. D. E., Vaidya C. J., Stone M., Francis W. S., Thompson-Schill S. L., Fleischman D. A., … Wilson R. S . (1999). Convergent behavioral and neuropsychological evidence for a distinction between identification and production forms of repetition priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128(4), 479-498.
doi: 10.1037//0096-3445.128.4.479 URL pmid: 10650584 |
[9] |
Geraci L . (2006). A test of the frontal lobe functioning hypothesis of age deficits in production priming. Neuropsychology, 20(5), 539-548.
doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.20.5.539 URL pmid: 16938016 |
[10] |
Just M. A., Keller T. A., & Cynkar J . (2008). A decrease in brain activation associated with driving when listening to someone speak. Brain Research, 1205, 70-80.
doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.12.075 URL pmid: 18353285 |
[11] |
Kiefer M . (2012). Executive control over unconscious cognition: attentional sensitization of unconscious information processing. Frontiers of Human Neuroscience, 6, 1-12.
doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00061 URL pmid: 22470329 |
[12] |
Kiefer M., & Martens U . (2010). Attentional sensitization of unconscious cognition: task sets modulate subsequent masked semantic priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139(3), 464-489.
doi: 10.1037/a0019561 URL pmid: 20677895 |
[13] |
LaVoie D. J., & Faulkner K. M . (2008). Production and identification repetition priming in amnesticmild cognitive impairment. Aging Neuropsychology and Cognition, 15(4), 523-544.
doi: 10.1080/13825580802051497 URL pmid: 18584343 |
[14] |
Leynes P. A., Bruett H., Krizan J., & Veloso A . (2017). What psychological process is reflected in the FN400 event-related potential component. Brain and Cognition, 113, 142-154.
doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2017.02.004 URL pmid: 28235696 |
[15] |
Lin J. Y., Meng Y. F., & Lin W. J . (in press). Conditional automaticity: interference effects on the implicit memory retrieval process. Psychological Research. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-019-01228-9.
doi: 10.1007/s00426-019-01228-9 URL pmid: 31302775 |
[16] | Lin W. J., Meng Y. F., & Lin J. Y . (2017). Effects of interference on retrieval process in implicit memory. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 49(7), 49-60. |
[ 林无忌, 孟迎芳, 林静远 . (2017). 提取干扰对内隐记忆的影响. 心理学报, 49(7), 49-60.] | |
[17] | Lozito J. P., & Mulligan N. W . (2010). Exploring the role of attention during implicit memory retrieval. Journal of Memory and Language, 63(3), 387-399. |
[18] |
Lucas H. D., Taylor J. R., Henson R. N., & Paller K. A . (2012). Many roads lead to recognition: electrophysiological correlates of familiarity derived from short-term masked repetition priming. Neuropsychologia, 50(13), 3041-3052.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.07.036 URL pmid: 23010141 |
[19] |
Marques V. R. S., Spataro P., Cestari V., Sciarretta A., & Rossi- Arnaud C . (2016). Testing the Identification/Production hypothesis of implicit memory in Schizophrenia: The role of response competition. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 22( 3), 314-321.
doi: 10.1017/S1355617715001198 URL pmid: 26689111 |
[20] |
Martens U., & Kiefer M . (2009). Specifying attentional top-down influences on subsequent unconscious semantic processing. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 5, 56-68.
doi: 10.2478/v10053-008-0067-3 URL pmid: 20523850 |
[21] | Meng Y. F., & Guo C. Y . (2007). The Asymmetric Effect of Interference at Encoding or Retrieval on Implicit and Explicit Memory. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 39(4), 579-588. |
[ 孟迎芳, 郭春彦 . (2007). 编码与提取干扰对内隐和外显记忆的非对称性影响. 心理学报, 39(4), 579-588.] | |
[22] |
Meng Y. F., & Guo C. Y . (2009). The Asymmetric Relationship Between Encoding and Retrieval in Implicit and Explicit Memory. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 41(8), 694-705.
URL pmid: 17039771 |
[ 孟迎芳, 郭春彦 . (2009). 内隐与外显记忆的编码与提取非对称性关系. 心理学报, 41(8), 694-705.]
pmid: 17039771 |
|
[23] | Meng Y. F., & Yu H. L . (2012). The dissociation between encoding and retrieval in implicit and explicit memory. Journal of South China Normal University (Social Science Edition), (3), 50-55. |
[ 孟迎芳, 于海莉 . (2012). 内隐记忆与外显记忆编码与提取加工的分离. 华南师范大学学报(社会科学版), (3), 50-55.] | |
[24] |
Miyoshi K., & Ashida H . (2014). Priming and implicit recognition depend on similar temporal changes in perceptual representations. Acta Psychologica, 148, 6-11.
doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.12.012 URL pmid: 24486801 |
[25] |
Moors A., & de Houwer J . (2006). Automaticity: a theoretical and conceptual analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 132(2), 297-326.
doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.2.297 URL pmid: 16536645 |
[26] |
Mulligan N. W., & Lozito J. P . (2006). An asymmetry between memory encoding and retrieval Revelation, generation, and transfer-appropriate processing. Psychological Science, 17(1), 7-11.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01657.x URL pmid: 16371137 |
[27] |
Newell B. R., Cavenett T., & Anderws S . (2008). On the immunity of perceptual implicit memory to manipulations of attention. Memory & Cognition, 36(4), 725-734.
doi: 10.3758/mc.36.4.725 URL pmid: 18604956 |
[28] |
Prull M. W., Lawless C., Marshall H. M., & Sherman A. T . (2016). Effects of divided attention at retrieval on conceptual implicit memory. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1-13.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00001 URL pmid: 26858668 |
[29] |
Prull M. W., & Spataro P . (2017). Editorial: The role of the distinctions between identification/production and perceptual/ conceptual processes in implicit memory: Findings from cognitive psychology, neuroscience and neuropsychology. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 8-10.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00008 URL pmid: 28194118 |
[30] |
Rasmussen I. A., Xu J., Antonsen I. K., Brunner J., Skandsen T., & Axelson D. E., … Haberg A . (2008). Simple dual tasking recruits prefrontal cortices in chronic severe traumatic brain injury patients, but not in controls. Journal of Neurotrauma, 25(9), 1057-1070.
doi: 10.1089/neu.2008.0520 URL pmid: 18729718 |
[31] | Sbicigo J. B., Janczura G. A., Salles. J . (2017). The role of attention in perceptual and conceptual priming. Psychology & Neuroscience, 10(2):117-131. |
[32] |
Sheldon S. A., & Moscovitch M . (2010). Recollective performance advantages for implicit memory tasks. Memory, 18(7), 681-97.
doi: 10.1080/09658211.2010.499876 URL pmid: 20721805 |
[33] |
Spaniol J., Davidson P. S. R., Kim A. S. N., Han H., Moscovitch M., & Grady C. L . (2009). Event-related fMRI studies of episodic encoding and retrieval: meta-analyses using activation likelihood estimation. Neuropsychologia, 47(8-9), 1765-1779.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.02.028 URL pmid: 19428409 |
[34] |
Spataro P., Cestari V., & Rossi-Arnaud C . (2011). The relationship between divided attention and implicit memory: a meta-analysis. Acta Psychologica, 136(3), 329-339.
doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.12.007 URL pmid: 21257140 |
[35] |
Spataro P., Mulligan N. W., & Rossi-Arnaud C . (2013). Divided attention can enhance memory encoding: the attentional boost effect in implicit memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 39(4), 1223-1231.
doi: 10.1037/a0030907 URL pmid: 23356238 |
[36] |
Spataro P., Saraulli D., Mulligan N. W., Cestari V., Costanzi M., & Rossi-Arnaud C . (2017). Not all identification tasks are born equal: testing the involvement of production processes in perceptual identification and lexical decision. Psychological Research, 82(4), 685-699.
doi: 10.1007/s00426-017-0852-z URL pmid: 28285363 |
[37] | Tan J. F., Wu S. S., Xu L., Wang L. J., & Chen A. T . (2013). Prefrontal cortex with executive functions involved in dual- task performance. Advances in Psychological Science, 21(12), 2127-2135. |
[ 谭金凤, 伍姗姗, 徐雷, 王丽君, 陈安涛 . (2013). 前额叶皮层与双任务加工执行功能. 心理科学进展, 21(12), 2127-2135.] | |
[38] |
Tombu M. N., Asplund C. L., Dux P. E., Godwin D., Martin J. W., & Marois R . (2011). A unified attentional bottleneck in the human brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108(33), 13426-13431.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1103583108 URL pmid: 21825137 |
[39] | Wang D., Wang T., Qin S., & Zhang J. J . (2019). Location effect of Chinese wordable components in the component priming paradigm. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 51(2), 163-176. |
[ 王丹, 王婷, 秦松, 张积家 . (2019). 部件启动范式下可成字部件的位置效应. 心理学报, 51(2), 163-176.] | |
[40] | Winer B. J., Brown D. R., & Michels K. M . (1971). Statistical principles in experimental design. New York: McGraw-Hill. |
[1] | 朱一鸣, 赵阳, 唐宁, 周吉帆, 沈模卫. 笔画节点在手写体汉字识别中的作用[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(12): 1903-1916. |
[2] | 孟迎芳, 董月晴, 陈荃. 概念内隐记忆中的注意促进效应[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(5): 469-480. |
[3] | 林静远, 林无忌, 孟迎芳. 尼古丁对内隐记忆与外显记忆的影响 *[J]. 心理学报, 2018, 50(9): 940-952. |
[4] | 林无忌, 孟迎芳, 林静远. 提取干扰对内隐记忆的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(7): 897-908. |
[5] | 叶晓红;陈幼贞;孟迎芳. 回想、熟悉性与启动在编码过程的认知神经机制[J]. 心理学报, 2015, 47(9): 1101-1110. |
[6] | 刘英杰;魏萍;丁锦红;郭春彦. 内隐重复效应影响外显工作记忆的年龄差异[J]. 心理学报, 2014, 46(3): 321-330. |
[7] | 孟迎芳. 内隐与外显记忆编码阶段脑机制的重叠与分离[J]. 心理学报, 2012, 44(1): 30-39. |
[8] | 王娟,张积家,谢书书,袁爱玲. 结合东巴文学习汉字对幼儿汉字字形记忆的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2011, 43(05): 519-533. |
[9] | 李月婷,李琦,郭春彦. 内隐和外显记忆测验中情绪词差异的ERP研究[J]. 心理学报, 2010, 42(07): 735-742. |
[10] | Soledad Ballesteros and Julia Mayas. 保留的跨通道启动与老化:对于近期观点的总结[J]. 心理学报, 2009, 41(11): 1063-1074. |
[11] | 孟迎芳, 郭春彦 . 内隐与外显记忆的编码与提取非对称性关系[J]. 心理学报, 2009, 41(08): 694-705. |
[12] | 孟迎芳,郭春彦. 编码与提取干扰对内隐和外显记忆的非对称性影响[J]. 心理学报, 2007, 39(04): 579-588. |
[13] | 孟迎芳,郭春彦. 内隐记忆和外显记忆的脑机制分离:面孔再认的ERP研究[J]. 心理学报, 2006, 38(01): 15-21. |
[14] | 林树. 对广告信息加工的性别差异[J]. 心理学报, 2005, 37(05): 694-701. |
[15] | 杨炯炯, ,翁旭初,管林初, ,匡培梓, ,张懋植, ,孙伟建,于生元5. 额叶在启动效应中的作用机制:对额叶损伤病人颜色命名和词命名任务的分析[J]. 心理学报, 2003, 35(05): 610-616. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||