Advances in Psychological Science ›› 2024, Vol. 32 ›› Issue (3): 486-498.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2024.00486
• Regular Articles • Previous Articles Next Articles
LEI Heya1,2, HUANG Wenxin1,2, CHEN Weicong1,3, HONG Jinzhu1,2, ZHENG Yan1,2, HUANG Liang1,2()
Received:
2023-08-06
Online:
2024-03-15
Published:
2024-01-19
CLC Number:
LEI Heya, HUANG Wenxin, CHEN Weicong, HONG Jinzhu, ZHENG Yan, HUANG Liang. The causes of eye effect instability: Subjective and objective factors and psychological related mechanisms[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2024, 32(3): 486-498.
影响因素 | 影响方式 | 启示 |
---|---|---|
眼睛的生理特征 | 1.影响意图推断、人格推论 2.影响吸引力评价 | 在同一实验中尽量保持刺激的生理特征相似, 避免选择生理特征两极化的眼睛。 |
眼睛的情绪类型 | 1.诱发的大脑激活模式 2.影响意图推断、人格推论 | 研究中考虑是否要将眼睛情绪类型作为额外因素进行控制, 避免将不同情绪产生的不同效果混为一谈。 |
眼睛的呈现时间 | 1.习惯化效应 2.监测机制产生作用 3.与被观察的感觉相分离 | 呈现时间过短会导致眼睛线索被忽略, 过长则会失效。应控制眼睛线索呈现时长, 也可在需要被试反应时再呈现眼睛刺激。 |
眼神的注视方向 | 1.影响认知加工过程 2.影响神经环路基础 | 若想更明显地改变行为, 直视的眼睛比转移视线的眼睛更有效。 |
实验的交互形式 | 影响匿名性:被观察的感觉 | 眼睛效应在互动条件下生效, 应让被试相信在进行真实互动。 |
行为成本 | 行为成本本身便是影响亲社会行为的重要因素, 行为成本低时各种操作更有效。 | 研究眼睛线索对亲社会行为的影响时, 控制低行为成本有利于改善眼睛效应的不稳定性。 |
周边人数 | 1.影响心理状态:责任分散 2.影响认知过程:匿名性 | 实验时控制周边人数, 同一场所中人数不宜过多, 应避免在过于拥挤场所进行实验。 |
噪音 | 影响匿名性:被观察的感觉 | 保持实验情境中噪音大小适度, 如远离施工现场、避开广播期间。 |
自我意识 | 特质自我意识影响对眼睛线索的感知, 继而不同程度地激活状态自我意识 | 可根据不同特质自我意识将人群分类, 分别研究眼睛效应的效果, 或将状态自我意识纳入调查范围。 |
群体身份 | 1.对声誉的关注 2.社会启发式假说 | 眼睛效应出现在内群体成员中。在群体中研究眼睛效应时, 或同时收集多名被试的数据时, 注意调查个体对群体身份的感知。 |
影响因素 | 影响方式 | 启示 |
---|---|---|
眼睛的生理特征 | 1.影响意图推断、人格推论 2.影响吸引力评价 | 在同一实验中尽量保持刺激的生理特征相似, 避免选择生理特征两极化的眼睛。 |
眼睛的情绪类型 | 1.诱发的大脑激活模式 2.影响意图推断、人格推论 | 研究中考虑是否要将眼睛情绪类型作为额外因素进行控制, 避免将不同情绪产生的不同效果混为一谈。 |
眼睛的呈现时间 | 1.习惯化效应 2.监测机制产生作用 3.与被观察的感觉相分离 | 呈现时间过短会导致眼睛线索被忽略, 过长则会失效。应控制眼睛线索呈现时长, 也可在需要被试反应时再呈现眼睛刺激。 |
眼神的注视方向 | 1.影响认知加工过程 2.影响神经环路基础 | 若想更明显地改变行为, 直视的眼睛比转移视线的眼睛更有效。 |
实验的交互形式 | 影响匿名性:被观察的感觉 | 眼睛效应在互动条件下生效, 应让被试相信在进行真实互动。 |
行为成本 | 行为成本本身便是影响亲社会行为的重要因素, 行为成本低时各种操作更有效。 | 研究眼睛线索对亲社会行为的影响时, 控制低行为成本有利于改善眼睛效应的不稳定性。 |
周边人数 | 1.影响心理状态:责任分散 2.影响认知过程:匿名性 | 实验时控制周边人数, 同一场所中人数不宜过多, 应避免在过于拥挤场所进行实验。 |
噪音 | 影响匿名性:被观察的感觉 | 保持实验情境中噪音大小适度, 如远离施工现场、避开广播期间。 |
自我意识 | 特质自我意识影响对眼睛线索的感知, 继而不同程度地激活状态自我意识 | 可根据不同特质自我意识将人群分类, 分别研究眼睛效应的效果, 或将状态自我意识纳入调查范围。 |
群体身份 | 1.对声誉的关注 2.社会启发式假说 | 眼睛效应出现在内群体成员中。在群体中研究眼睛效应时, 或同时收集多名被试的数据时, 注意调查个体对群体身份的感知。 |
[1] | 郭青青, 邓逊, 郭伟栋. (2020). 社会拥挤对金钱捐赠意愿的影响:自我表达需要的中介作用. 心理科学, 43(5), 1211-1219 |
[2] |
金花, 严世振, 单金磊, 刘婷. (2022). 面孔生命性加工的时间进程:ERP研究. 心理与行为研究, 20(5), 577-584+599.
doi: 10.12139/j.1672-0628.2022.05.001 |
[3] |
李雅宁, 田杨阳, 吴琦, 冷海州, 蒋重清, 杨伊生. (2021). 面孔社会知觉中的表情效应. 心理科学进展, 29(6), 1022-1029.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2021.01022 |
[4] |
时慧颖, 汤洁, 刘萍萍. (2022). 眼睛效应不稳定与感知规范:一个新视角. 心理科学进展, 30(12), 2718-2734.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2022.02718 |
[5] |
石荣, 刘昌. (2019). 基于直觉的亲社会性:来自社会启发式假设的思考. 心理科学进展, 27(8), 1468-1477.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2019.01468 |
[6] | 孙聪慧, 赵春黎, 王圣龙. (2020). 眼睛线索下公我意识启动对利他行为的影响. 心理发展与教育, 36(3), 283-288. |
[7] |
田珈源, 龙彦伶, 杨虹, 吴惠芳, 薛鹏, 蒋重清. (2022). 面孔-人格知觉中眼部线索及其效应. 心理科学进展, 30(12), 2735-2745.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2022.02735 |
[8] |
吴琴, 崔丽莹. (2020). 合作行为中的“眼睛效应”:解释机制与限制因素. 心理科学进展, 28(6), 994-1003.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2020.00994 |
[9] |
徐慧, 李美佳, 彭华茂. (2022). 眼睛注视线索对经济领域风险决策的影响:基于框架效应范式. 心理与行为研究, 20(1), 37-44.
doi: 10.12139/j.1672-0628.2022.01.006 |
[10] |
杨莎莎, 陈思静. (2022). 第三方惩罚中的规范错觉:基于公正世界信念的解释. 心理学报, 54(3), 281-299.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2022.00281 |
[11] |
占友龙, 肖啸, 谭千保, 李琎, 钟毅平. (2022). 声誉关注与社会距离对伤害困境中道德决策的影响:来自行为与ERPs的证据. 心理学报, 54(6), 613-627.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2022.00613 |
[12] |
张雪姣, 刘聪慧. (2017). 亲社会行为中的“眼睛效应”. 心理科学进展, 25(3), 475-485.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2017.00475 |
[13] | 赵亚军, 张智君. (2009). 眼睛注视线索提示效应:内源性注意还是外源性注意? 心理学报, 41(12), 1133-1142. |
[14] | 朱千, 寇慧, 毕泰勇. (2019). 面孔社会性线索的加工机制. 生理学报, 71(1), 73-85. |
[15] |
Andreoni, J., & Bernheim, B. D. (2009). Social image and the 50-50 norm: A theoretical and experimental analysis of audience effects. Econometrica, 77(5), 1607-1636.
doi: 10.3982/ECTA7384 URL |
[16] |
Ayal, S., Celse, J., & Hochman, G. (2021). Crafting messages to fight dishonesty: A field investigation of the effects of social norms and watching eye cues on fare evasion. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 166, 9-19.
doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2019.10.003 URL |
[17] |
Baillon, A., Selim, A., & van Dolder, D. (2013). On the social nature of eyes: The effect of social cues in interaction and individual choice tasks. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34(2), 146-154.
doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2012.12.001 URL |
[18] | Bateson, M., Robinson, R., Abayomi-Cole, T., Greenlees, J., O’Connor, A., & Nettle, D. (2015). Watching eyes on potential litter can reduce littering: Evidence from two field experiments. PeerJ, 3, Article e1443. https://doi.org/10.7717/peerj.1443 |
[19] | Belteki, Z., van den Boomen, C., & Junge, C. (2022). Face-to-face contact during infancy: How the development of gaze to faces feeds into infants’ vocabulary outcomes. Frontiers in Psychology, 13, Article 997186. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.997186 |
[20] |
Botto, S. V., & Rochat, P. (2019). Evaluative audience perception (EAP): How children come to care about reputation. Child Development Perspectives, 13(3), 180-185.
doi: 10.1111/cdep.2019.13.issue-3 URL |
[21] |
Brambilla, M., Biella, M., & Kret, M. E. (2019). Looking into your eyes: Observed pupil size influences approach- avoidance responses. Cognition and Emotion, 33(3), 616-622.
doi: 10.1080/02699931.2018.1472554 URL |
[22] |
Breil, C., Huestegge, L., & Böckler, A. (2022). From eye to arrow: Attention capture by direct gaze requires more than just the eyes. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 84(1), 64-75.
doi: 10.3758/s13414-021-02382-2 |
[23] |
Burra, N., Baker, S., & George, N. (2017). Processing of gaze direction within the N170/M170 time window: A combined EEG/MEG study. Neuropsychologia, 100, 207-219.
doi: S0028-3932(17)30157-4 pmid: 28450203 |
[24] |
Calvo, M. G., Krumhuber, E. G., & Fernández-Martín, A. (2019). Visual attention mechanisms in happiness versus trustworthiness processing of facial expressions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 72(4), 729-741.
doi: 10.1177/1747021818763747 URL |
[25] |
Cañigueral, R., & Hamilton, A. F. (2019). Being watched: Effects of an audience on eye gaze and prosocial behaviour. Acta Psychologica, 195, 50-63.
doi: S0001-6918(18)30377-9 pmid: 30878648 |
[26] |
Chatterjee, A., & Vartanian, O. (2014). Neuroaesthetics. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(7), 370-375.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.03.003 pmid: 24768244 |
[27] |
Chen, Y., Zhao, C., & Liu, R. (2022). Perceived gaze direction affects recollection processes in recognition of concrete and abstract words: Electrophysiological evidence. NeuroReport, 33(18), 791-798.
doi: 10.1097/WNR.0000000000001847 pmid: 36367796 |
[28] |
Conty, L., George, N., & Hietanen, J. K. (2016). Watching eyes effects: When others meet the self. Consciousness and Cognition, 45, 184-197.
doi: S1053-8100(16)30250-1 pmid: 27639066 |
[29] |
Cox, M. J., DiBello, A. M., Meisel, M. K., Ott, M. Q., Kenney, S. R., Clark, M. A., & Barnett, N. P. (2019). Do misperceptions of peer drinking influence personal drinking behavior? Results from a complete social network of first-year college students. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 33(3), 297-303.
doi: 10.1037/adb0000455 pmid: 30869918 |
[30] |
Dear, K., Dutton, K., & Fox, E. (2019). Do ‘watching eyes’ influence antisocial behavior? A systematic review & meta-analysis. Evolution and Human Behavior, 40(3), 269-280.
doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2019.01.006 URL |
[31] |
Duval, S. (1976). Conformity on a visual task as a function of personal novelty on attitudinal dimensions and being reminded of the object status of self. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 12(1), 87-98.
doi: 10.1016/0022-1031(76)90088-3 URL |
[32] |
Engel, C., & van Lange, P. A. M. (2021). Social mindfulness is normative when costs are low, but rapidly declines with increases in costs. Judgment and Decision Making, 16(2), 290-322.
doi: 10.1017/S1930297500008585 URL |
[33] |
Ernest-Jones, M., Nettle, D., & Bateson, M. (2011). Effects of eye images on everyday cooperative behavior: A field experiment. Evolution and Human Behavior, 32(3), 172-178.
doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.10.006 URL |
[34] | Everett, J. A., Faber, N. S., & Crockett, M. J. (2015). The influence of social preferences and reputational concerns on intergroup prosocial behaviour in gains and losses contexts. Royal Society Open Science, 2(12), Article 150546. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.150546 |
[35] |
Fenigstein, A., Scheier, M. F., & Buss, A. H. (1975). Public and private self-consciousness: Assessment and theory. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 43(4), 522-527.
doi: 10.1037/h0076760 URL |
[36] | Fenzl, T., & Brudermann, T. (2021). Eye cues increase cooperation in the dictator game under physical attendance of a recipient, but not for all. Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Economics, 94, Article 101748. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socec.2021.101748 |
[37] |
Govern, J. M., & Marsch, L. A. (2001). Development and validation of the situational self-awareness scale. Consciousness and Cognition, 10(3), 366-378.
pmid: 11697870 |
[38] |
Guy, N., & Pertzov, Y. (2023). The robustness of individual differences in gaze preferences toward faces and eyes across face-to-face experimental designs and its relation to social anxiety. Journal of Vision, 23(5), 15-15.
doi: 10.1167/jov.23.5.15 pmid: 37212783 |
[39] | Hadjikhani, N., Åsberg Johnels, J., Zürcher, N. R., Lassalle, A., Guillon, Q., Hippolyte, L.,... Gillberg, C. (2017). Look me in the eyes: Constraining gaze in the eye-region provokes abnormally high subcortical activation in autism. Scientific Reports, 7(1), Article 3163. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-03378-5 |
[40] |
Haley, K. J., & Fessler, D. M. T. (2005). Nobody's watching? Subtle cues affect generosity in an anonymous economic game. Evolution and Human Behavior, 26(3), 245-256.
doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2005.01.002 URL |
[41] |
Hassan, R., MacMillan, H. L., Tanaka, M., & Schmidt, L. A. (2020). Psychophysiological influences on personality trajectories in adolescent females exposed to child maltreatment. Development and Psychopathology, 32(4), 1390-1401.
doi: 10.1017/S0954579419001342 URL |
[42] |
Hessels, R. S. (2020). How does gaze to faces support face-to-face interaction? A review and perspective. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 27(5), 856-881.
doi: 10.3758/s13423-020-01715-w |
[43] |
Hietanen, J. K., Leppänen, J. M., Peltola, M. J., Linna-Aho, K., & Ruuhiala, H. J. (2008). Seeing direct and averted gaze activates the approach-avoidance motivational brain systems. Neuropsychologia, 46(9), 2423-2430.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.02.029 pmid: 18402988 |
[44] |
Hietanen, J. O., & Hietanen, J. K. (2017). Genuine eye contact elicits self-referential processing. Consciousness and Cognition, 51, 100-115.
doi: S1053-8100(16)30336-1 pmid: 28327346 |
[45] | Hietanen, J. O., Peltola, M. J., & Hietanen, J. K. (2020). Psychophysiological responses to eye contact in a live interaction and in video call. Psychophysiology, 57(6), Article e13587. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.13587 |
[46] |
House, B. R., Kanngiesser, P., Barrett, H. C., Broesch, T., Cebioglu, S., Crittenden, A. N., … Silk, J. B. (2020). Universal norm psychology leads to societal diversity in prosocial behaviour and development. Nature Human Behaviour, 4(1), 36-44.
doi: 10.1038/s41562-019-0734-z pmid: 31548679 |
[47] |
Jarick, M., & Bencic, R. (2019). Eye contact is a two-way street: Arousal is elicited by the sending and receiving of eye gaze information. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 1262. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01262
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01262 URL pmid: 31214077 |
[48] | Kelsey, C., Grossmann, T., & Vaish, A. (2018). Early reputation management: Three-year-old children are more generous following exposure to eyes. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, Article 698. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00698 |
[49] |
Kelsey, C., Vaish, A., & Grossmann, T. (2018). Eyes, more than other facial features, enhance real-world donation behavior. Human Nature, 29(4), 390-401.
doi: 10.1007/s12110-018-9327-1 |
[50] |
Kret, M. E., & de Dreu, C. K. (2019). The power of pupil size in establishing trust and reciprocity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 148(8), 1299-1311.
doi: 10.1037/xge0000508 URL |
[51] |
Kruglanski, A. W., & Webster, D. M. (1991). Group members’ reactions to opinion deviates and conformists at varying degrees of proximity to decision deadline and of environmental noise. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61(2), 212-225.
pmid: 1920063 |
[52] | Lapolla, N. J., Bishop, B. H., & Gahtan, E. (2023). Social context modulates autonomic responses to direct eye contact. Physiology and Behavior, 263, 114-119. |
[53] | Lau, W. K., Sauter, M., & Huckauf, A. (2022). Small pupils lead to lower judgements of a person’s characteristics for exaggerated, but not for realistic pupils. Behavioral Sciences, 12(8), Article 283. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs12080283 |
[54] | Liang, J., Zou, Y. Q., Liang, S. Y., Wu, Y. W., & Yan, W. J. (2021). Emotional gaze: The effects of gaze direction on the perception of facial emotions. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, Article 684357. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.684357 |
[55] | Lloyd, D. R., Medina, D. J., Hawk, L. W., Fosco, W. D., & Richards, J. B. (2014). Habituation of reinforcer effectiveness. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 7, Article 107. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnint.2013.00107 |
[56] | Lotti, L., Barile, L., & Manfredi, G. (2023). Improving recycling sorting behaviour with human eye nudges. Scientific Reports, 13(1), Article 10127. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-023-37019-x |
[57] |
Luo, Y., Zhang, S., Tao, R., & Geng, H. (2016). The power of subliminal and supraliminal eye contact on social decision making: An individual-difference perspective. Consciousness and Cognition, 40, 131-140.
doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2016.01.001 pmid: 26821242 |
[58] |
Ma, Y., & Tan, H. (2023). Representation of intergroup conflict in the human brain. Neuron, 111(11), 1692-1696.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2023.05.004 URL |
[59] | Manesi, Z., van Lange, P. A., & Pollet, T. V. (2016). Eyes wide open: Only eyes that pay attention promote prosocial behavior. Evolutionary Psychology, 14(2), 1-15. |
[60] |
Manesi, Z., van Lange, P. A. M., van Doesum, N. J., & Pollet, T. V. (2019). What are the most powerful predictors of charitable giving to victims of typhoon Haiyan: Prosocial traits, socio-demographic variables, or eye cues? Personality and Individual Differences, 146, 217-225.
doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2018.03.024 |
[61] |
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98(2), 224-253.
doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224 URL |
[62] |
Mifune, N., Hashimoto, H., & Yamagishi, T. (2010). Altruism toward in-group members as a reputation mechanism. Evolution and Human Behavior, 31(2), 109-117.
doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2009.09.004 URL |
[63] |
Myllyneva, A., & Hietanen, J. K. (2015). There is more to eye contact than meets the eye. Cognition, 134, 100-109.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.09.011 pmid: 25460383 |
[64] |
Nettle, D., Harper, Z., Kidson, A., Stone, R., Penton-Voak, I. S., & Bateson, M. (2013). The watching eyes effect in the dictator game: It’s not how much you give, it’s being seen to give something. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34(1), 35-40.
doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2012.08.004 URL |
[65] |
Niedźwiecka, A. (2020). Look me in the eyes: Mechanisms underlying the eye contact effect. Child Development Perspectives, 14(2), 78-82.
doi: 10.1111/cdep.v14.2 URL |
[66] |
Northover, S. B., Pedersen, W. C., Cohen, A. B., & Andrews, P. W. (2017). Artificial surveillance cues do not increase generosity: Two meta-analyses. Evolution and Human Behavior, 38(1), 144-153.
doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.07.001 URL |
[67] | Oda, R., Kato, Y., & Hiraishi, K. (2015). The watching-eye effect on prosocial lying. Evolutionary Psychology, 13(3). https://doi.org/10.1177/1474704915594959 |
[68] |
Oda, R., Niwa, Y., Honma, A., & Hiraishi, K. (2011). An eye-like painting enhances the expectation of a good reputation. Evolution and Human Behavior, 32(3), 166-171.
doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.11.002 URL |
[69] |
Okumura, Y., Hattori, T., Fujita, S., & Kobayashi, T. (2023). A robot is watching me!: Five-year-old children care about their reputation after interaction with a social robot. Child Development, 94(4), 865-873.
doi: 10.1111/cdev.13903 pmid: 36752147 |
[70] |
Okamura, Y., & Ura, M. (2020). Shapes of faces and eyeglasses influence the judgement of facial impressions in a metaphor-consistent manner. Current Psychology, 39(6), 2293-2297.
doi: 10.1007/s12144-018-9931-6 |
[71] |
Olszanowski, M., Parzuchowski, M., & Szymkow, A. (2019). When the smile is not enough: The interactive role of smiling and facial characteristics in forming judgments about trustworthiness and dominance. Roczniki Psychologiczne, 22(1), 35-52.
doi: 10.18290/rpsych.2019.22.1-3 URL |
[72] |
Park, I., Lee, D., & Sah, Y. J. (2022). Under watching eyes in news comment sections: Effects of audience cue on self-awareness and commenting behaviour. Behaviour and Information Technology, 42(13), 2279-2295.
doi: 10.1080/0144929X.2022.2114937 URL |
[73] |
Pfattheicher, S., & Keller, J. (2015). The watching eyes phenomenon: The role of a sense of being seen and public self-awareness. European Journal of Social Psychology, 45(5), 560-566.
doi: 10.1002/ejsp.v45.5 URL |
[74] |
Powell, K. L., Roberts, G., & Nettle, D. (2012). Eye images increase charitable donations: Evidence from an opportunistic field experiment in a supermarket. Ethology, 118(11), 1096-1101.
doi: 10.1111/eth.2012.118.issue-11 URL |
[75] |
Prinsen, J., & Alaerts, K. (2019). Eye contact enhances interpersonal motor resonance: Comparing video stimuli to a live two-person action context. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 14(9), 967-976.
doi: 10.1093/scan/nsz064 pmid: 31506688 |
[76] | Rotella, A., Sparks, A. M., Mishra, S., & Barclay, P. (2021). No effect of ‘watching eyes’: An attempted replication and extension investigating individual differences. Plos One, 16(10), Article e0255531. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0255531 |
[77] |
Russell, R., Batres, C., Courrèges, S., Kaminski, G., Soppelsa, F., Morizot, F., & Porcheron, A. (2019). Differential effects of makeup on perceived age. British Journal of Psychology, 110(1), 87-100.
doi: 10.1111/bjop.12337 pmid: 30073658 |
[78] |
Sénémeaud, C., Sanrey, C., Callé, N., Plainfossé, C., Belhaire, A., & Georget, P. (2017). The watching-eyes phenomenon and blood donation: Does exposure to pictures of eyes increase blood donation by young adults? Transfusion and Apheresis Science, 56(2), 168-170.
doi: S1473-0502(16)30168-9 pmid: 27887885 |
[79] |
Senju, A., & Johnson, M. H. (2009). The eye contact effect: Mechanisms and development. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(3), 127-134.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.11.009 pmid: 19217822 |
[80] |
Sparks, A., & Barclay, P. (2013). Eye images increase generosity, but not for long: The limited effect of a false cue. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34(5), 317-322.
doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2013.05.001 URL |
[81] |
Sparks, A., & Barclay, P. (2015). No effect on condemnation of short or long exposure to eye images. Letters on Evolutionary Behavioral Science, 6(2), 13-16.
doi: 10.5178/lebs.2015.35 URL |
[82] | Stephani, T., Driller, K. K., Dimigen, O., & Sommer, W. (2020). Eye contact in active and passive viewing: Event-related brain potential evidence from a combined eye tracking and EEG study. Neuropsychologia, 143, Article 107478. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2020.107478 |
[83] |
Sutherland, C. A. M., & Young, A. W. (2022). Understanding trait impressions from faces. British Journal of Psychology, 113(4), 1056-1078.
doi: 10.1111/bjop.12583 pmid: 35880691 |
[84] | Todorov, A., & Oh, D. (2021). The structure and perceptual basis of social judgments from faces. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 63, 189-245. |
[85] |
Vaish, A., Kelsey, C. M., Tripathi, A., & Grossmann, T. (2017). Attentiveness to eyes predicts generosity in a reputation-relevant context. Evolution and Human Behavior, 38(6), 729-733.
doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2017.07.004 URL |
[86] |
van Doesum, N. J., Karremans, J. C., Fikke, R. C., de Lange, M. A., & van Lange, P. A. (2018). Social mindfulness in the real world: The physical presence of others induces other-regarding motivation. Social Influence, 13(4), 209-222.
doi: 10.1080/15534510.2018.1544589 URL |
[87] | van Doesum, N. J., van der Wal, A. J., Boomsma, C., & Staats, H. (2021). Aesthetics and logistics in urban parks: Can moving waste receptacles to park exits decrease littering? Journal of Environmental Psychology, 77, Article 101669. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2021.101669 |
[88] | van Lange, P. A., & Manesi, Z. (2023). Reputation reminders: When do eye cues promote prosocial behavior? The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 26, Article e8. https://doi.org/10.1017/SJP.2023.4 |
[89] |
van Lange, P. A. M., & Rand, D. G. (2022). Human cooperation and the crises of climate change, COVID-19, and misinformation. Annual Review of Psychology, 73, 379-402.
doi: 10.1146/psych.2022.73.issue-1 URL |
[90] | Wang, R., Wang, Y., Chen, C., Huo, L., & Liu, C. (2023). How do eye cues affect behaviors? Two meta-analyses. Current Psychology, 1-18. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-023-04395-6 |
[91] |
Wang, Z., Quinn, P. C., Jin, H., Sun, Y.-H. P., Tanaka, J. W., Pascalis, O., & Lee, K. (2019). A regional composite-face effect for species-specific recognition: Upper and lower halves play different roles in holistic processing of monkey faces. Vision Research, 157, 89-96.
doi: S0042-6989(18)30047-6 pmid: 29653136 |
[92] |
Xin, Z., Liu, Y., Yang, Z., & Zhang, H. (2016). Effects of minimal social cues on trust in the investment game. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 19(3), 235-243.
doi: 10.1111/ajsp.v19.3 URL |
[93] |
Xu, X., Potters, J., & Suetens, S. (2020). Cooperative versus competitive interactions and in-group bias. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 179, 69-79.
doi: 10.1016/j.jebo.2020.08.041 URL |
[94] |
Zhang, X., Dalmaso, M., Castelli, L., Fiorese, A., Lan, Y., Sun, B., Fu, S., & Galfano, G. (2021). Social attention across borders: A cross-cultural investigation of gaze cueing elicited by same-and other-ethnicity faces. British Journal of Psychology, 112(3), 741-762.
doi: 10.1111/bjop.v112.3 URL |
[1] | CHEN Yanyan, WU Junhui, LUAN Shenghua. The mechanisms and promotion strategies of cooperation in the intergenerational dilemma [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2023, 31(9): 1728-1741. |
[2] | SHI Huiying, TANG Jie, LIU Pingping. Instability of the watching eyes effect and perceived norms: A new perspective [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(12): 2718-2734. |
[3] | LI Qinggong, WANG Zhenyan, SUN Jieyuan, SHI Yan. The influence of reputation and face trustworthiness on women’s trust judgment in car-hailing scene and the moderating effect of intuitive thinking [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2020, 28(5): 746-751. |
[4] | SUN Tuwei, LUO Nanfeng, SHI Wei, LI Hongyang. Psychological mechanisms underlying the negative effects of corporations’ high reputation [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2020, 28(3): 497-509. |
[5] | CHEN Sijing, YANG Shasha. Motives of altruistic punishment [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2020, 28(11): 1901-1910. |
[6] | ZHANG Xuejiao; LIU Conghui. The watching eyes effect on prosociality [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2017, 25(3): 475-485. |
[7] | TAN Chenhao; WANG Yibo; CUI Yichen; WANG Pei. The profit sacrificing tendency of reputation-profit game [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2016, 24(12): 1907-1916. |
[8] | YUAN Mingliang; ZHANG Mengyuan; KOU Yu. Prosocial reputation and prosocial behavior [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2016, 24(10): 1655-1662. |
[9] | LIU Guo-Fang;XIN Zi-Qiang. Reputation Mechanisms in Indirect Reciprocity: Image, Standing, Tag and Its Transmission [J]. , 2011, 19(2): 233-242. |
Viewed | ||||||
Full text |
|
|||||
Abstract |
|
|||||