Advances in Psychological Science ›› 2022, Vol. 30 ›› Issue (6): 1230-1241.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2022.01230
• Meta-Analysis • Previous Articles Next Articles
ZHAO Ning1,2, LIU Xin1,2, LI Shu1,2, ZHENG Rui1,2()
Received:
2021-08-05
Online:
2022-06-15
Published:
2022-04-26
Contact:
ZHENG Rui
E-mail:zhengrui@psych.ac.cn
CLC Number:
ZHAO Ning, LIU Xin, LI Shu, ZHENG Rui. Nudging effect of default options: A meta-analysis[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(6): 1230-1241.
作者(年份) | 被试量 | 文化 | Cohens’d | 作者(年份) | 被试量 | 文化 | Cohens’d |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Abhyankar等( | 83 | 1 | 0.02 | Kühne等( | 158 | 1 | 0.74 |
Apathy & Holmgren ( | 1663 | 1 | -0.20 | Kühne等( | 158 | 1 | 0.43 |
Araña & León ( | 1680 | 1 | 0.24 | Kühne等( | 158 | 1 | 0.66 |
Bartholomew等( | 1059 | 1 | 1.32 | Lai & Hui ( | 32 | 1 | 0.48 |
Bergman等( | 3478 | 1 | 0.05 | Lai & Hui ( | 32 | 1 | 0.89 |
Briscese ( | 215 | 1 | 1.72 | Li等( | 4050 | 1 | 0.49 |
Briscese ( | 213 | 1 | 1.81 | Loeb等( | 62 | 1 | 1.91 |
Bruns等( | 498 | 1 | 0.39 | Loeb等( | 58 | 1 | 1.45 |
Chapman等( | 480 | 1 | 0.25 | Löfgren等( | 160 | 1 | 0.08 |
Cheung等( | 203 | 0 | -0.09 | Madrian & Shea ( | 10050 | 1 | 1.06 |
Dinner等( | 209 | 1 | 0.51 | Mehta等( | 314 | 1 | 0.51 |
Dinner等( | 126 | 1 | 0.49 | Michaelsen等( | 208 | 1 | 0.10 |
Ebeling & Lotz ( | 41952 | 1 | 1.42 | Michaelsen等( | 288 | 1 | 0.36 |
Elkington等( | 121 | 1 | 0.82 | Michaelsen等( | 698 | 1 | 0.44 |
Erkal, Gangadharan, & Xiao ( | 129 | 1 | 0.31 | Moncreiff ( | 348 | 1 | 0.39 |
Evans等( | 124 | 1 | 0.29 | Moncreiff ( | 328 | 1 | 0.49 |
Evans等( | 141 | 1 | 0.40 | Montoy等( | 3172 | 1 | 0.57 |
Everett等( | 177 | 1 | 0.34 | Narula 等 ( | 81 | 1 | -0.53 |
Everett等( | 150 | 1 | 0.32 | Pichert & Katsikopoulos ( | 152 | 1 | 0.55 |
Everett等( | 152 | 1 | 1.33 | Pichert & Katsikopoulos ( | 65 | 1 | 1.03 |
Everett等( | 136 | 1 | <0.01 | Pichert & Katsikopoulos ( | 88 | 1 | 0.45 |
樊亚凤 等( | 63 | 0 | -0.85 | Probst等( | 144 | 1 | 0.61 |
樊亚凤 等( | 78 | 0 | -0.17 | Reiter等( | 121 | 1 | -0.48 |
Fowlie等( | 1706 | 1 | 1.58 | Reiter等( | 283 | 1 | -0.14 |
Fowlie等( | 3058 | 1 | 1.71 | Robinson等( | 797 | 1 | 0.34 |
Halpern等( | 89 | 1 | 0.71 | Schubert等( | 107 | 1 | 0.02 |
Haward等( | 291 | 1 | 0.86 | Shevchenko等( | 67 | 1 | 1.55 |
何铨, 江程铭( | 65 | 0 | -0.12 | Steffel等( | 363 | 1 | 0.47 |
何铨, 江程铭( | 65 | 0 | 0.93 | Steffel等( | 429 | 1 | 0.86 |
Hedlin & Sunstein ( | 691 | 1 | 0.28 | Steffel等( | 209 | 1 | 2.31 |
黄元娜 等( | 121 | 0 | 0.36 | Steffel等( | 779 | 1 | 1.23 |
江程铭 等( | 289 | 0 | 0.39 | Steffel等( | 817 | 1 | 1.67 |
江程铭 等( | 291 | 0 | 0.51 | Steffel等( | 680 | 1 | 1.01 |
Jin ( | 197 | 0 | 0.84 | Steffel等( | 390 | 1 | 0.90 |
Jin ( | 124 | 0 | -0.02 | Strugnell等( | 5316 | 1 | 1.04 |
Jin ( | 112 | 0 | 0.10 | Theotokis & Manganari ( | 71 | 1 | 0.29 |
Johnson & Goldstein ( | 107 | 1 | 0.86 | Theotokis & Manganari ( | 73 | 1 | 1.01 |
Johnson等( | 136 | 1 | 0.63 | Theotokis & Manganari ( | 118 | 1 | 0.43 |
Johnson, Bellman & Lohse ( | 138 | 1 | 1.22 | Trevena等( | 152 | 1 | 0.40 |
Johnson, Bellman & Lohse ( | 156 | 1 | 0.74 | van Dalen & Henkens ( | 979 | 1 | 0.24 |
Kaiser等( | 17111 | 1 | 0.05 | Wang等( | 548 | 1 | 0.96 |
Keller等( | 54 | 1 | 0.14 | Wootton等( | 280 | 1 | 0.03 |
Keller等( | 54 | 1 | 0.14 | Or等( | 207 | 1 | 0.32 |
Kressel & Chapman ( | 128 | 1 | 0.47 | Young等( | 118 | 1 | 0.52 |
Kressel & Chapman ( | 51 | 1 | 0.68 | Young等( | 78 | 1 | 1.20 |
Kressel & Chapman ( | 75 | 1 | 0.40 | Zarghamee等( | 187 | 1 | 0.88 |
作者(年份) | 被试量 | 文化 | Cohens’d | 作者(年份) | 被试量 | 文化 | Cohens’d |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Abhyankar等( | 83 | 1 | 0.02 | Kühne等( | 158 | 1 | 0.74 |
Apathy & Holmgren ( | 1663 | 1 | -0.20 | Kühne等( | 158 | 1 | 0.43 |
Araña & León ( | 1680 | 1 | 0.24 | Kühne等( | 158 | 1 | 0.66 |
Bartholomew等( | 1059 | 1 | 1.32 | Lai & Hui ( | 32 | 1 | 0.48 |
Bergman等( | 3478 | 1 | 0.05 | Lai & Hui ( | 32 | 1 | 0.89 |
Briscese ( | 215 | 1 | 1.72 | Li等( | 4050 | 1 | 0.49 |
Briscese ( | 213 | 1 | 1.81 | Loeb等( | 62 | 1 | 1.91 |
Bruns等( | 498 | 1 | 0.39 | Loeb等( | 58 | 1 | 1.45 |
Chapman等( | 480 | 1 | 0.25 | Löfgren等( | 160 | 1 | 0.08 |
Cheung等( | 203 | 0 | -0.09 | Madrian & Shea ( | 10050 | 1 | 1.06 |
Dinner等( | 209 | 1 | 0.51 | Mehta等( | 314 | 1 | 0.51 |
Dinner等( | 126 | 1 | 0.49 | Michaelsen等( | 208 | 1 | 0.10 |
Ebeling & Lotz ( | 41952 | 1 | 1.42 | Michaelsen等( | 288 | 1 | 0.36 |
Elkington等( | 121 | 1 | 0.82 | Michaelsen等( | 698 | 1 | 0.44 |
Erkal, Gangadharan, & Xiao ( | 129 | 1 | 0.31 | Moncreiff ( | 348 | 1 | 0.39 |
Evans等( | 124 | 1 | 0.29 | Moncreiff ( | 328 | 1 | 0.49 |
Evans等( | 141 | 1 | 0.40 | Montoy等( | 3172 | 1 | 0.57 |
Everett等( | 177 | 1 | 0.34 | Narula 等 ( | 81 | 1 | -0.53 |
Everett等( | 150 | 1 | 0.32 | Pichert & Katsikopoulos ( | 152 | 1 | 0.55 |
Everett等( | 152 | 1 | 1.33 | Pichert & Katsikopoulos ( | 65 | 1 | 1.03 |
Everett等( | 136 | 1 | <0.01 | Pichert & Katsikopoulos ( | 88 | 1 | 0.45 |
樊亚凤 等( | 63 | 0 | -0.85 | Probst等( | 144 | 1 | 0.61 |
樊亚凤 等( | 78 | 0 | -0.17 | Reiter等( | 121 | 1 | -0.48 |
Fowlie等( | 1706 | 1 | 1.58 | Reiter等( | 283 | 1 | -0.14 |
Fowlie等( | 3058 | 1 | 1.71 | Robinson等( | 797 | 1 | 0.34 |
Halpern等( | 89 | 1 | 0.71 | Schubert等( | 107 | 1 | 0.02 |
Haward等( | 291 | 1 | 0.86 | Shevchenko等( | 67 | 1 | 1.55 |
何铨, 江程铭( | 65 | 0 | -0.12 | Steffel等( | 363 | 1 | 0.47 |
何铨, 江程铭( | 65 | 0 | 0.93 | Steffel等( | 429 | 1 | 0.86 |
Hedlin & Sunstein ( | 691 | 1 | 0.28 | Steffel等( | 209 | 1 | 2.31 |
黄元娜 等( | 121 | 0 | 0.36 | Steffel等( | 779 | 1 | 1.23 |
江程铭 等( | 289 | 0 | 0.39 | Steffel等( | 817 | 1 | 1.67 |
江程铭 等( | 291 | 0 | 0.51 | Steffel等( | 680 | 1 | 1.01 |
Jin ( | 197 | 0 | 0.84 | Steffel等( | 390 | 1 | 0.90 |
Jin ( | 124 | 0 | -0.02 | Strugnell等( | 5316 | 1 | 1.04 |
Jin ( | 112 | 0 | 0.10 | Theotokis & Manganari ( | 71 | 1 | 0.29 |
Johnson & Goldstein ( | 107 | 1 | 0.86 | Theotokis & Manganari ( | 73 | 1 | 1.01 |
Johnson等( | 136 | 1 | 0.63 | Theotokis & Manganari ( | 118 | 1 | 0.43 |
Johnson, Bellman & Lohse ( | 138 | 1 | 1.22 | Trevena等( | 152 | 1 | 0.40 |
Johnson, Bellman & Lohse ( | 156 | 1 | 0.74 | van Dalen & Henkens ( | 979 | 1 | 0.24 |
Kaiser等( | 17111 | 1 | 0.05 | Wang等( | 548 | 1 | 0.96 |
Keller等( | 54 | 1 | 0.14 | Wootton等( | 280 | 1 | 0.03 |
Keller等( | 54 | 1 | 0.14 | Or等( | 207 | 1 | 0.32 |
Kressel & Chapman ( | 128 | 1 | 0.47 | Young等( | 118 | 1 | 0.52 |
Kressel & Chapman ( | 51 | 1 | 0.68 | Young等( | 78 | 1 | 1.20 |
Kressel & Chapman ( | 75 | 1 | 0.40 | Zarghamee等( | 187 | 1 | 0.88 |
调节变量 | b | SE | t | p | 95% CI |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
截距 | 0.28 | 0.45 | 0.62 | 0.539 | -0.62, 1.17 |
文化 | |||||
东方/西方 | 0.44 | 0.20 | 2.25 | 0.027 | 0.05, 0.84 |
情境领域 | |||||
环保情境 | -0.32 | 0.17 | -1.85 | 0.067 | -0.66, 0.02 |
金钱情境 | 0.43 | 0.14 | 3.12 | 0.002 | 0.15, 0.70 |
健康情境 | -0.15 | 0.13 | -1.20 | 0.233 | -0.42, 0.10 |
动机 | |||||
利己/利他 | 0.19 | 0.12 | 1.56 | 0.122 | -0.05, 0.44 |
实际金钱变动 | 0.10 | 0.12 | 0.82 | 0.416 | -0.15, 0.36 |
实验设置 | |||||
因变量类型 | 0.04 | 0.13 | 0.27 | 0.788 | -0.23, 0.30 |
发表时间 | -0.10 | 0.15 | -0.68 | 0.498 | -0.40, 0.19 |
样本量 | <0.01 | < 0.01 | 1.19 | 0.236 | <0.01 |
是否在线实验 | -0.03 | 0.13 | -0.20 | 0.842 | -0.29, 0.24 |
是否现场实验 | -0.03 | 0.15 | -0.20 | 0.841 | -0.33, 0.27 |
研究数量 | 92 | ||||
R2 | 0.159 |
调节变量 | b | SE | t | p | 95% CI |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
截距 | 0.28 | 0.45 | 0.62 | 0.539 | -0.62, 1.17 |
文化 | |||||
东方/西方 | 0.44 | 0.20 | 2.25 | 0.027 | 0.05, 0.84 |
情境领域 | |||||
环保情境 | -0.32 | 0.17 | -1.85 | 0.067 | -0.66, 0.02 |
金钱情境 | 0.43 | 0.14 | 3.12 | 0.002 | 0.15, 0.70 |
健康情境 | -0.15 | 0.13 | -1.20 | 0.233 | -0.42, 0.10 |
动机 | |||||
利己/利他 | 0.19 | 0.12 | 1.56 | 0.122 | -0.05, 0.44 |
实际金钱变动 | 0.10 | 0.12 | 0.82 | 0.416 | -0.15, 0.36 |
实验设置 | |||||
因变量类型 | 0.04 | 0.13 | 0.27 | 0.788 | -0.23, 0.30 |
发表时间 | -0.10 | 0.15 | -0.68 | 0.498 | -0.40, 0.19 |
样本量 | <0.01 | < 0.01 | 1.19 | 0.236 | <0.01 |
是否在线实验 | -0.03 | 0.13 | -0.20 | 0.842 | -0.29, 0.24 |
是否现场实验 | -0.03 | 0.15 | -0.20 | 0.841 | -0.33, 0.27 |
研究数量 | 92 | ||||
R2 | 0.159 |
[1] | * 樊亚凤, 蒋晶, 崔稳权. (2019). 网络公益平台默认选项设置对个人捐赠意愿的影响及作用机制. 心理学报, 51(4), 415-427. |
[2] | 何贵兵, 李纾, 梁竹苑. (2018). 以小拨大: 行为决策助推社会发展. 心理学报, 50(8), 803-813. |
[3] | * 何铨, 江程铭. (2020). 默认选项助推老年人意外伤害险在线购买. 心理科学, 43(2), 430-437. |
[4] | * 黄元娜, 宋星云, 邵洋, 李纾, 梁竹苑. (2018). 以小拨大: 默认选项和反应模式效应助推中国器官捐献登记. 心理学报, 50(8), 868-879. |
[5] | * 江程铭, 马家涛, 孙红月. (2019). 助推爱心: 利用默认选项促进捐赠行为. 心理科学, 42(5), 1174-1179. |
[6] |
* Abadie A., & Gay S. (2006). The impact of presumed consent legislation on cadaveric organ donation: A cross- country study. Journal of Health Economics, 25(4), 599- 620.
doi: 10.1016/j.jhealeco.2006.01.003 URL |
[7] |
* Abhyankar P., Summers B. A., Velikova G., & Bekker H. L. (2014). Framing options as choice or opportunity: Does the frame influence Decisions? Medical Decision Making, 34(5), 567-582.
doi: 10.1177/0272989X14529624 pmid: 24732048 |
[8] |
Allcott H., & Rogers T. (2014). The short-run and long-run effects of behavioral interventions: Experimental evidence from energy conservation. American Economic Review, 104(10), 3003-3037.
doi: 10.1257/aer.104.10.3003 URL |
[9] |
* Apathy N. C., & Holmgren A. J. (2020). Opt-in consent policies: potential barriers to hospital health information exchange. American Journal of Managed Care, 26(1), e14-e20.
doi: 10.37765/ajmc.2020.42148 URL |
[10] |
* Araña J. E., & León C. J. (2013). Can defaults save the climate? Evidence from a field experiment on carbon offsetting programs. Environmental and Resource Economics, 54(4), 613-626.
doi: 10.1007/s10640-012-9615-x URL |
[11] | * Bartholomew T. S., Tookes H. E., Serota D. P., Behrends C. N., Forrest D. W., & Feaster D. J. (2020). Impact of routine opt-out HIV/HCV screening on testing uptake at a syringe services program: An interrupted time series analysis. International Journal of Drug Policy, 84, 102875. |
[12] |
Benartzi S., Beshears J., Milkman K. L., Sunstein C. R., Thaler R. H., Shankar M.,... Galing S. (2017). Should governments invest more in nudging? Psychological Science, 28(8), 1041-1055.
doi: 10.1177/0956797617702501 pmid: 28581899 |
[13] |
* Bergman P., Lasky-Fink J., & Rogers T. (2020). Simplification and defaults affect adoption and impact of technology, but decision makers do not realize it. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 158, 66-79.
doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2019.04.001 URL |
[14] |
Bolderdijk J. W., Steg L., Geller E. S., Lehman P., & Postmes T. (2013). Comparing the effectiveness of monetary versus moral motives in environmental campaigning. Nature Climate Change, 3(4), 413-416.
doi: 10.1038/nclimate1767 URL |
[15] |
Bonini B., Hadjichristidis C., & Graffeo M. (2018). Green nudging. Acta Psychologica Sinica (心理学报), 50(8), 814-826.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2018.00814 URL |
[16] | * Briscese G. (2019). Generous by default: A field experiment on designing defaults that align with past behaviour on charitable giving. Journal of Economic Psychology, 74, Article 102187. |
[17] |
* Bruns H., Kantorowicz-Reznichenko E., Klement K., Jonsson M. L., & Rahali B. (2018). Can nudges be transparent and yet effective? Journal of Economic Psychology, 65, 41-59.
doi: 10.1016/j.joep.2018.02.002 URL |
[18] |
Bucher T., Collins C., Rollo M. E., McCaffrey T. A., de Vlieger N., van der Bend D.,... Perez-Cueto F. J. (2016). Nudging consumers towards healthier choices: A systematic review of positional influences on food choice. British Journal of Nutrition, 115(12), 2252-2263.
doi: 10.1017/S0007114516001653 URL |
[19] |
Cadario R., & Chandon P. (2020). Which healthy eating nudges work best? A meta-analysis of field experiments. Marketing Science, 39(3), 465-486.
doi: 10.1287/mksc.2018.1128 URL |
[20] | * Chapman G. B., Li M., Colby H., & Yoon H. (2010). Opting in vs opting out of influenza vaccination. Journal of the American Medical Association, 304(1), 43-44. |
[21] |
Chernev A., Böckenholt U., & Goodman J. (2015). Choice overload: A conceptual review and meta-analysis. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 25(2), 333-358.
doi: 10.1016/j.jcps.2014.08.002 URL |
[22] |
* Cheung T., Cheng T., & Wong L. (2018). Willingness for deceased organ donation under different legislative systems in Hong Kong: Population-based cross-sectional survey. Hong Kong Medical Journal, 24(2), 119-127.
doi: 10.12809/hkmj176831 pmid: 29633715 |
[23] |
Cohen J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112(1), 155-159.
doi: 10.1037//0033-2909.112.1.155 pmid: 19565683 |
[24] |
Cohen J., Dunbar K., & Mcclelland J. (1988). On the control of automatic processes: A parallel distributed processing model of the Stroop effect. Psychological Review, 97(3), 332-361.
doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.97.3.332 URL |
[25] |
Davidai S., & Shafir E. (2020). Are ‘nudges’ getting a fair shot? Joint versus separate evaluation. Behavioural Public Policy, 4(3), 273-291.
doi: 10.1017/bpp.2018.9 URL |
[26] |
Dietz T. (2015). Altruism, self-interest, and energy consumption. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112(6), 1654-1655.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1423686112 URL |
[27] |
* Dinner I., Johnson E. J., Goldstein D. G., & Liu K. (2011). Partitioning default effects: Why people choose not to choose. Journal of Experimental Psychology Applied, 17(4), 332-341.
doi: 10.1037/a0024354 URL |
[28] |
* Ebeling F., & Lotz S. (2015). Domestic uptake of green energy promoted by opt-out tariffs. Nature Climate Change, 5, 868-871.
doi: 10.1038/NCLIMATE2681 |
[29] |
Egger M., Smith G. D., Schneider M., & Minder C. (1997). Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. British Medical Journal, 315(7109), 629-634.
doi: 10.1136/bmj.315.7109.629 URL |
[30] |
* Elkington J., Stevenson M., Haworth N., & Sharwood L. (2014). Using police crash databases for injury prevention research - A comparison of opt-out and opt-in approaches to study recruitment. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Public Health, 38(3), 286-289.
doi: 10.1111/1753-6405.12237 pmid: 24890488 |
[31] | * Erkal N., Gangadharan L., & Xiao E. (2021). Leadership selection: Can changing the default break the glass ceiling?. The Leadership Quarterly, 101563. |
[32] | * Evans A. M., Dillon K. D., Goldin G., & Krueger J. I. (2011). Trust and self-control: The moderating role of the default. Judgment and Decision Making, 6(7), 697-705. |
[33] |
* Everett J. A. C., Caviola L., Kahane G., Savulescu J., & Faber N. S. (2015). Doing good by doing nothing? The role of social norms in explaining default effects in altruistic contexts. European Journal of Social Psychology, 45(2), 230-241.
doi: 10.1002/ejsp.2080 URL |
[34] | * Fowlie M., Wolfram C., Spurlock C. A., Todd A., Baylis P., & Cappers P. (2017). Default effects and follow-on behavior: Evidence from an electricity pricing program (No. w23553). National Bureau of Economic Research. |
[35] |
Hagman W., Andersson D., Västfjäll D., & Tinghög G. (2015). Public views on policies involving nudges. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 6(3), 439-453.
doi: 10.1007/s13164-015-0263-2 URL |
[36] | * Halpern D. (2015). Inside the nudge unit: How small changes can make a big difference. London, England: WH Allen. |
[37] |
* Haward M. F., Murphy R. O., & Lorenz J. M. (2012). Default options and neonatal resuscitation decisions. Journal of Medical Ethics, 38(12), 713-718.
doi: 10.1136/medethics-2011-100182 URL |
[38] | * Hedlin S., & Sunstein C. R. (2016). Does active choosing promote green energy use? Experimental evidence. Ecology Law Quarterly, 43(1), 107-141. |
[39] | Hohle S. M. (2014). Nudging sustainable food choices. The role of defaults, frames, habits and nature relatedness (Unpublished master’s thesis, University of Oslo, Sweden). Retrieved from http://urn.nb.no/URN:NBN:no-44588 |
[40] |
Huedo-Medina T. B., Sánchez-Meca J., Marín-Martínez F., & Botella J. (2006). Assessing heterogeneity in meta- analysis: Q statistic or I² index? Psychological Methods, 11(2), 193-206.
pmid: 16784338 |
[41] |
Jachimowicz J., Duncan S., Weber E. U., & Johnson E. J. (2018). When and why defaults influence decisions: A meta-analysis of default effects. Behavioural Public Policy, 3(2), 159-186.
doi: 10.1017/bpp.2018.43 URL |
[42] |
Jachimowicz J. M., Hauser O. P., O’Brien J. D., Sherman E., & Galinsky A. D. (2018). The critical role of second- order normative beliefs in predicting energy conservation. Nature Human Behaviour, 2(10), 757-764.
doi: 10.1038/s41562-018-0434-0 pmid: 31406290 |
[43] |
* Jin L. (2011). Improving response rates in web surveys with default setting: The effects of default on web survey participation and permission. International Journal of Market Research, 53(1), 75-94.
doi: 10.2501/IJMR-53-1-075-094 URL |
[44] |
* Johnson E. J., Bellman S., & Lohse G. L. (2002). Defaults, framing and privacy: Why opting in-opting out 1. Marketing Letters, 13(1), 5-15.
doi: 10.1023/A:1015044207315 URL |
[45] |
* Johnson E. J., & Goldstein D. (2003). Do Defaults Save Lives? Science, 302(5649), 1338-1339.
pmid: 14631022 |
[46] |
* Johnson E. J., Hershey J. C., Meszaros J., & Kunreuther H. (1993). Framing, probability distortions, and insurance decisions. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 7(1), 35-51.
doi: 10.1007/BF01065313 URL |
[47] | Jung J. Y., & Mellers B. A. (2016). American attitudes toward nudges. Judgment & Decision Making, 11(1), 62- 74. |
[48] |
Just D., & Price J. (2013). Default options, incentives and food choices: Evidence from elementary-school children. Public Health Nutrition, 16(12), 2281-2288.
doi: 10.1017/S1368980013001468 URL |
[49] | * Kaiser M., Bernauer M., Sunstein C. R., & Reisch L. A. (2020). The power of green defaults: the impact of regional variation of opt-out tariffs on green energy demand in Germany. Ecological Economics, 174, Article 106685. |
[50] |
* Keller P. A., Harlam B., Loewenstein G., & Volpp K. G. (2011). Enhanced active choice: A new method to motivate behavior change. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 21(4), 376-383.
doi: 10.1016/j.jcps.2011.06.003 URL |
[51] |
* Kressel L. M., & Chapman G. B. (2007). The default effect in end-of-life medical treatment preferences. Medical Decision Making, 27(3), 299-310.
pmid: 17545499 |
[52] |
Kühberger A. (1998). The influence of framing on risky decisions: A meta-analysis. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 75(1), 23-55.
doi: 10.1006/obhd.1998.2781 URL |
[53] |
* Kühne S. J., Reijnen E., & Crameri A. (2020). When too few is bad for the environment choice set size and default effects for electricity products. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 79(1), 35-41.
doi: 10.1024/1421-0185/a000232 URL |
[54] | * Lai Y. L., & Hui K. L. (2004). Opting-in or opting-out on the internet: Does it really matter? In ICIS 2004:Twenty-Fifth International Conference on Information Systems (pp.781-792). Retrieved from http://aisel.aisnet.org/icis2004/63 |
[55] |
* Li D., Hawley Z., & Schnier K. (2013). Increasing organ donation via changes in the default choice or allocation rule. Journal of Health Economics, 32(6), 1117-1129.
doi: 10.1016/j.jhealeco.2013.09.007 URL |
[56] |
Li X., Dusseldorp E., & Meulman J. J. (2017). Meta‐CART: A tool to identify interactions between moderators in meta‐analysis. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 70(1), 118-136.
doi: 10.1111/bmsp.12088 URL |
[57] | Lipsey M. W., & Wilson D. B. (2001). Practical meta- analysis. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. |
[58] |
* Loeb K. L., Radnitz C., Keller K., Schwartz M. B., Marcus S., Pierson R. N.,... DeLaurentis D. (2017). The application of defaults to optimize parents' health-based choices for children. Appetite, 113, 368-375.
doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2017.02.039 URL |
[59] |
* Löfgren S., Ring E., Brömssen C. V., Sørensen R., & Högbom L. (2009). Short-term effects of clear-cutting on the water chemistry of two boreal streams in northern sweden: A paired catchment study. Ambio, 38(7), 347-356.
doi: 10.1579/0044-7447-38.7.347 URL |
[60] |
Madden S., Collett D., Walton P., Empson K., Forsythe J., Ingham A.,... Gardiner D. (2020). The effect on consent rates for deceased organ donation in Wales after the introduction of an opt‐out system. Anaesthesia, 75(9), 1146-1152.
doi: 10.1111/anae.15055 pmid: 32372409 |
[61] |
* Madrian B. C., & Shea D. F. (2001). The power of suggestion: Inertia in 401 (k) participation and savings behavior. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 116(4), 1149-1187.
doi: 10.1162/003355301753265543 URL |
[62] |
* Mehta S. J., Khan T., Guerra C., Reitz C., McAuliffe T., Volpp K. G.,... Doubeni C. A. (2018). A randomized controlled trial of opt-in versus opt-out colorectal cancer screening outreach. American Journal of Gastroenterology, 113(12), 1848-1854.
doi: 10.1038/s41395-018-0151-3 URL |
[63] | * Michaelsen P., Nyström L., Luke T. J., Johansson L. O., & Hedesström M. (2020). Are default nudges deemed fairer when they are more transparent? People's judgments depend on the circumstances of the evaluation. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/5knx4 |
[64] | * Moncreiff H. (2017). Green energy defaults: Too effective or not effective enough? (Unpublished master's thesis). The International Institute for Industrial Environmental Economics, Swedish. |
[65] | * Montoy J. C. C., Dow W. H., & Kaplan B. C. (2016). Patient choice in opt-in, active choice, and opt-out HIV screening: randomized clinical trial. British Medical Journal, 352, Article h6895. |
[66] |
* Narula T., Ramprasad C., Ruggs E. N., & Hebl M. R. (2014). Increasing colonoscopies? A psychological perspective on opting in versus opting out. Health Psychology, 33(11), 1426-1429.
doi: 10.1037/a0034260 URL |
[67] |
Nguyen N. T., Maxwell A. P., Donnelly M., & O’Neill C. (2020). The role of motivational and legal contexts in understanding support for tissue donation across 27 European countries. European Journal of Public Health, 31(2), 259-264.
doi: 10.1093/eurpub/ckaa148 URL |
[68] | * Or A., Baruch Y., Tadger S., & Barak Y. (2014). Real-life decision making of serious mental illness patients: Opt-in and opt-out research participation. Israel Journal of Psychiatry and Related Sciences, 51(3), 199-203. |
[69] | Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development. (2017). Behavioural insights and public policy: Lessons from around the world. Paris: Author |
[70] |
Patel M. S., Volpp K. G., Small D. S., Wynne C., Zhu J., Yang L.,... Day S. C. (2017). Using active choice within the electronic health record to increase influenza vaccination rates. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 32(7), 790-795.
doi: 10.1007/s11606-017-4046-6 URL |
[71] | Paunov Y., Wänke M., & Vogel T. (2019). Ethical defaults: Which transparency components can increase the effectiveness of default nudges? Social Influence, 14(3-4), 104-116. |
[72] |
* Pichert D., & Katsikopoulos K. V. (2008). Green defaults: Information presentation and pro-environmental behaviour. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 28(1), 63-73.
doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2007.09.004 URL |
[73] |
* Probst C. A., Shaffer V. A., & Chan Y. R. (2013). The effect of defaults in an electronic health record on laboratory test ordering practices for pediatric patients. Health Psychology, 32(9), 995-1002.
doi: 10.1037/a0032925 pmid: 24001250 |
[74] |
* Reiter P. L., McRee A.-L., Pepper J. K., Chantala K., & Brewer N. T. (2012). Improving HPV vaccine delivery: A national study of parents and their adolescent sons. Journal of Adolescent Health, 51(1), 32-37.
doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2012.01.006 URL |
[75] |
* Robinson C. D., Pons G. A., Duckworth A. L., & Rogers T. (2018). Some middle school students want behavior commitment devices (but take-up does not affect their behavior). Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 206.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00206 pmid: 29541040 |
[76] |
Rosenthal R. (1979). The ‘file drawer problem’ and tolerance for null results. Psychological Bulletin, 86(3), 638-461.
doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.86.3.638 URL |
[77] | Rutgers. (2017). Printing. Retrieved from https://oit-nb.rutgers.edu/service/printing/printgreen |
[78] |
Scheibehenne B., Greifeneder R., & Todd P. M. (2010). Can there ever be too many options? A meta-analytic review of choice overload. Journal of Consumer Research, 37(3), 409-425.
doi: 10.1086/651235 URL |
[79] | * Schubert R., Schmitz J., Grieder M., & Ghesla C. (2017). Green by default-welfare effects of green default electricity contracts: Final report 7th November 2017. ETH Zurich. |
[80] | * Shevchenko Y., von Helversen B., & Scheibehenne B. (2014). Change and status quo in decisions with defaults: The effect of incidental emotions depends on the type of default. Judgment and Decision Making, 9(3), 287-296. |
[81] |
* Steffel M., Williams E. F., & Pogacar R. (2016). Ethically deployed defaults: Transparency and consumer protection through disclosure and preference articulation. Journal of Marketing Research, 53(5), 865-880.
doi: 10.1509/jmr.14.0421 URL |
[82] |
* Strugnell C., Orellana L., Hayward J., Millar L., Swinburn B., & Allender S. (2018). Active (opt-in) consent underestimates mean BMI-z and the prevalence of overweight and obesity compared to passive (opt-out) consent. Evidence from the healthy together victoria and childhood obesity study. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 15(4), 747.
doi: 10.3390/ijerph15040747 URL |
[83] | Sunstein C. R. (2016a). Do people like nudges. Administrative Law Review, 68(2), 177-232. |
[84] | Sunstein C. R. (2016b). The ethics of influence: Government in the age of behavioral science. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. |
[85] | Sunstein C. R., Reisch L. A., & Rauber J. (2018). A worldwide consensus on nudging? Not quite, but almost. Regulation & Governance, 12(1), 3-22. |
[86] | Szucs D., & Ioannidis J. P. A. (2017). Empirical assessment of published effect sizes and power in the recent cognitive neuroscience and psychology literature. Plos Biology, 19(3), e3001151. |
[87] |
* Theotokis A., & Manganari E. (2015). The Impact of Choice Architecture on Sustainable Consumer Behavior: The Role of Guilt. Journal of Business Ethics, 131(2), 423-437.
doi: 10.1007/s10551-014-2287-4 URL |
[88] | Toft M. B., Schuitema G., & Thøgersen J. (2014). The importance of framing for consumer acceptance of the Smart Grid: A comparative study of Denmark, Norway and Switzerland. Energy Research & Social Science, 3, 113-123. |
[89] |
* Trevena L., Irwig L., & Barratt A. (2006). Impact of privacy legislation on the number and characteristics of people who are recruited for research: A randomised controlled trial. Journal of Medical Ethics, 32(8), 473- 477.
pmid: 16877628 |
[90] |
* Van Dalen H. P., & Henkens K. (2014). Comparing the effects of defaults in organ donation systems. Social Science & Medicine, 106, 137-142.
doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2014.01.052 URL |
[91] | Van Gestel L. C., Adriaanse M. A., & de Ridder D. T. D. (2020). Do nudges make use of automatic processing? Unraveling the effects of a default nudge under type 1 and type 2 processing. Comprehensive Results in Social Psychology, Advance online publication. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1080/23743603.2020.1808456 |
[92] | Viechtbauer W. (2010). Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package. Journal of Statistical Software, 36(3), 1-48. |
[93] | * Wang W., Ida T., & Shimada H. (2020). Default effect versus active decision: Evidence from a field experiment in Los Alamos. European Economic Review, 128, 103498. |
[94] |
Wilson A. L., Buckley E., Buckley J. D., & Bogomolova S. (2016). Nudging healthier food and beverage choices through salience and priming. Evidence from a systematic review. Food Quality and Preference, 51, 47-64.
doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2016.02.009 URL |
[95] | * Wootton S. H., Blackwell S. C., Saade G., Berens P. D., Hutchinson M., Green C. E.,... Tyson J. E. (2018). Randomized quality improvement trial of opting-in versus opting-out to increase influenza vaccination rates during pregnancy. AJP Reports, 8(3), e161-e167. |
[96] | Yan H., & Yates J. F. (2019). Improving acceptability of nudges: Learning from attitudes towards opt-in and opt-out policies. Judgment and Decision Making, 14(1), 26-39. |
[97] |
* Young S. D., Monin B., & Owens D. (2009). Opt-out testing for stigmatized diseases: A social psychological approach to understanding the potential effect of recommendations for routine HIV testing. Health Psychology, 28(6), 675-681.
doi: 10.1037/a0016395 pmid: 19916635 |
[98] |
* Zarghamee H. S., Messer K. D., Fooks J. R., Schulze W. D., Wu S., & Yan J. (2017). Nudging charitable giving: Three field experiments. Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Economics, 66, 137-149.
doi: 10.1016/j.socec.2016.04.008 URL |
Viewed | ||||||
Full text |
|
|||||
Abstract |
|
|||||