Advances in Psychological Science ›› 2020, Vol. 28 ›› Issue (12): 2064-2075.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2020.02064
• Regular Articles • Previous Articles Next Articles
CUI Shuang, GAO Yaru, WANG Yangyang, HUANG Bijuan, SI Jiwei()
Received:
2020-04-03
Online:
2020-12-15
Published:
2020-10-26
Contact:
SI Jiwei
E-mail:sijiwei1974@126.com
CLC Number:
CUI Shuang, GAO Yaru, WANG Yangyang, HUANG Bijuan, SI Jiwei. Spontaneous focusing on numerosity (SFON) of children in informal learning environment[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2020, 28(12): 2064-2075.
[1] | 王一峰, 王荣燕, 李红. (2011). 感数的认知机制: 从注意到工作记忆. 心理科学进展, 19(7), 967-975. |
[2] | 杨伟星, 张明亮, 李红霞, 杨雅琳, 司继伟. (2017). 人类基本数学能力的进化证据. 心理科学进展, 25(5), 810-824. |
[3] |
Ann, D., Amar, S., & Yen, L. C. (2016). Mathematics anxiety: What have we learned in 60 years? Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 508. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00508
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00508 URL pmid: 27199789 |
[4] |
Anobile, G., Guerrini, G., Burr, D. C., Monti, M., del Lucchese, B., & Cicchini, G. M. (2019). Spontaneous perception of numerosity in pre-school children. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 286(1906), 20191245. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2019.1245
doi: 10.1098/rspb.2019.1245 URL pmid: 31288698 |
[5] |
Baroody, A. J., & Li, X. (2016). The construct and measurement of spontaneous attention to a number. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 13(2), 170-178.
doi: 10.1080/17405629.2016.1147345 URL |
[6] |
Baroody, A. J., Li, X., & Lai, M. -L. (2008). Toddlers’ spontaneous attention to number. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 10(3), 1-31.
doi: 10.1080/10986060701818610 URL |
[7] | Batchelor, S. (2014). Dispositional factors affecting children's early numerical development (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Loughborough University, Loughborough. |
[8] |
Batchelor, S., Inglis, M., & Gilmore, C. (2015). Spontaneous focusing on numerosity and the arithmetic advantage. Learning and Instruction, 40, 79-88.
doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2015.09.005 URL |
[9] |
Beilock, S. L., & Maloney,E. A. (2015). Math anxiety: A factor in math achievement not to be ignored. Policy Insights from the Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 2(1), 4-12.
doi: 10.1177/2372732215601438 URL |
[10] | Ben-Shachar, M. S., Lisson, S., Shotts-Peretz, D., Hannula- Sormunen, M., & Berger, A. (2020, February 18). Spontaneous focusing on numerosity is linked to numerosity discrimination in children and adults. Preprint. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/mt3n9 |
[11] |
Bojorque, G., Torbeyns, J., Hannula-Sormunen, M., van Nijlen, D., & Verschaffel, L. (2017). Development of SFON in Ecuadorian kindergartners. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 32(3), 449-462.
doi: 10.1007/s10212-016-0306-9 URL |
[12] |
Braham, E. J., Libertus, M. E., & McCrink, K. (2018). Children’s spontaneous focus on number before and after guided parent-child interactions in a children’s museum. Developmental Psychology, 54(8), 1492-1498.
doi: 10.1037/dev0000534 URL pmid: 30047774 |
[13] |
Chan, J. Y. -C., & Mazzocco,M. M. M. (2017). Competing features influence children’s attention to number. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 156, 62-81.
doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2016.11.008 URL pmid: 28039750 |
[14] |
Chan, J. Y. -C., Praus-Singh, T. L., & Mazzocco, M. M. M. (2020). Parents’ and young children’s attention to mathematical features varies across play materials. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 50, 65-77.
doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2019.03.002 URL |
[15] |
Chang,H., & Beilock,S. L. (2016). The math anxiety-math performance link and its relation to individual and environmental factors: A review of current behavioral and psychophysiological research. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 10, 33-38.
doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.04.011 URL |
[16] |
Choe, K. W., Jenifer, J. B., Rozek, C. S., Berman, M. G., & Beilock, S. L. (2019). Calculated avoidance: Math anxiety predicts math avoidance in effort-based decision-making. Science Advances, 5(11), eaay1062. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.aay1062
doi: 10.1126/sciadv.aay9969 URL pmid: 31807711 |
[17] |
Cicchini, G. M., Anobile, G., & Burr, D. C. (2016). Spontaneous perception of numerosity in humans. Nature Communications, 7, 12536. doi: 10.1038/ncomms12536
doi: 10.1038/ncomms12536 URL pmid: 27555562 |
[18] |
Daches Cohen, L., & Rubinsten, O. (2017). Mothers, intrinsic math motivation, arithmetic skills, and math anxiety in elementary school. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1939. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01939
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01939 URL pmid: 29180973 |
[19] |
Degrande, T., Verschaffel, L., & van Dooren, W. (2017). Spontaneous focusing on quantitative relations: Towards a characterization. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 19(4), 260-275.
doi: 10.1080/10986065.2017.1365223 URL |
[20] |
Edens, K. M., & Potter,E. F. (2013). An exploratory look at the relationships among math skills, motivational factors and activity choice. Early Childhood Educational Journal, 41(3), 235-243.
doi: 10.1007/s10643-012-0540-y URL |
[21] |
Fuhs, W. M., Nesbitt, T. K., & O’Rear, D. C. (2018). Approximate number system task performance: Associations with domain- general and domain-specific cognitive skills in young children. Journal of Numerical Cognition, 4(3), 590-612.
doi: 10.5964/jnc.v4i3.141 URL |
[22] | Geary, D. C., & vanMarle, K. (2016). Young children’s core symbolic and nonsymbolic quantitative knowledge in the prediction of later mathematics achievement. Developmental Psychology, 52(12), 21-30. |
[23] |
Gray, S. A., & Reeve,R. A. (2016). Number-specific and general cognitive markers of preschoolers’ math ability profiles. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 147, 1-21.
doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2016.02.004 URL pmid: 26985575 |
[24] |
Gunderson,E. A, & Levine,S. C. (2011). Some types of parent number talk count more than others: Relations between parents’ input and children’s cardinal-number knowledge. Developmental Science, 14(5), 1021-1032.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01050.x URL pmid: 21884318 |
[25] | Hannula, M. M. (2005). Spontaneous focusing on numerosity in the development of early mathematical skills (Unpublished Doctoral dissertation). University of Turku, Turku. |
[26] | Hannula, M. M., & Lehtinen, E. (2001). Spontaneous tendency to focus on numerosities in the development of cardinality. In M. Panhuizen-Van Heuvel (Ed.), Proceedings of 25th conference of the international group for the psychology of mathematics education (Vol 3, pp.113-120). Drukkerij Wilco, The Netherlands: Amersfoort. |
[27] |
Hannula, M. M., & Lehtinen, E. (2005). Spontaneous focusing on numerosity and mathematical skills of young children. Learning and Instruction, 15(3), 237-256.
doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2005.04.005 URL |
[28] |
Hannula, M. M., Lepola, J., & Lehtinen, E. (2010). Spontaneous focusing on numerosity as a domain-specific predictor of arithmetical skills. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 107(4), 394-406.
doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2010.06.004 URL pmid: 20643417 |
[29] | Hannula, M. M., Mattinen, A., & Lehtinen, E. (2005) . Does social interaction influence 3-year-old children’s tendency to focus on numerosity? A quasi-experimental study in day care. In L. Verschaffel, E. De Corte, G. Kanselaar, & M. Valcke (Eds.), Powerful environemnts for promoting deep conceptual and strategic learning (pp. 63-80). Leuven: Leuven University Press. |
[30] |
Hannula, M., Räsänen, P., & Lehtinen, E. (2007). Development of counting skills: Role of spontaneous focusing on numerosity and subitizing-based enumeration. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 9(1), 51-57.
doi: 10.1080/10986060709336605 URL |
[31] | Hannula-Sormunen, M. M.. (2015). Spontaneous focusing on numerosity and its relation to counting and arithmetic. In R. C. Kadosh and A. Dowker (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of numerical cognition (pp. 275-290). Oxford: Oxford University Press. |
[32] | Hannula-Sormunen, M. M., Grabner, R. H., & Lehtinen, E. (2009, March). Neural correlates of spontaneous focusing on numerosity (SFON) in a 9-year-longitudinal study of children’s mathematical skills. Paper presented in EARLI Advanced Study Colloquium on Cognitive Neuroscience Meets Mathematics Education. Brugge, Belgium. |
[33] |
Hannula-Sormunen, M. M., Lehtinen, E., & Räsänen, P. (2015). Preschool children’s spontaneous focusing on numerosity, subitizing, and counting skills as predictors of their mathematical performance 7 years later at school. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 17(2-3), 155-177.
doi: 10.1080/10986065.2015.1016814 URL |
[34] |
Hassinger-Das, B., Jordan, N. C., Glutting, J., Irwin, C., & Dyson, N. (2014). Domain-general mediators of the relation between kindergarten number sense and first-grade mathematics achievement. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 118, 78-92.
doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2013.09.008 URL |
[35] |
Jansen, B. R. J., Schmitz, E. A., & van der Maas, H. L. J. (2016). Affective and motivational factors mediate the relation between math skills and use of math in everyday life. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 513. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00513
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00513 URL pmid: 27148122 |
[36] |
Krajewski, K., & Schneider, W. (2009). Early development of quantity to number-word linkage as a precursor of mathematical school achievement and mathematical difficulties: Findings from a four-year longitudinal study. Learning and Instruction, 19(6), 513-526.
doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2008.10.002 URL |
[37] |
Kucian, K., Kohn, J., Hannula-Sormunen, M. M., Richtmann, V., McCaskey, U., Käser, T., … von Aster, M. (2012). Kinder mit Dyskalkulie fokussieren spontan weniger auf anzahligkeit/Children with developmental dyscalculia focus spontaneously less on numerosities. Lernen und Lernstörungen, 1(4), 241-253.
doi: 10.1024/2235-0977/a000024 URL |
[38] |
Lepola, J., & Hannula-Sormunen, M. (2019). Spontaneous focusing on numerosity and motivational orientations as predictors of arithmetical skills from kindergarten to grade 2. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 100(3), 251-269.
doi: 10.1007/s10649-018-9851-2 URL |
[39] |
Li,X., & Baroody,A. J. (2014). Young children's spontaneous attention to exact quantity and verbal quantification skills. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 11(5), 608-623.
doi: 10.1080/17405629.2014.896788 URL |
[40] |
Lira, C. J., Carver, M., Douglas, H., & LeFevre, J. -A. (2017). The integration of symbolic and non-symbolic representations of exact quantity in preschool children. Cognition, 166, 382-397.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2017.05.033 URL pmid: 28609720 |
[41] |
Lobato, J., Rhodehamel, B., & Hohensee, C. (2012). “Noticing” as an alternative transfer of learning process. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 21(3), 433-482.
doi: 10.1080/10508406.2012.682189 URL |
[42] | Maloney, E. A. (2016). Math anxiety: Causes, consequences, and remediation. In K. R. Wentzel & D. B. Miele (Eds.), Handbook of motivation at school (2nd ed., pp. 408-423). New York, NY: Routledge. |
[43] | McMullen, J., Chan, J. Y. -C., Mazzocco, M. M. M., & Hannula- Sormunen, M. M. (2019). Spontaneous mathematical focusing tendencies in mathematical development and education. In A. Norton, M. W. Alibali (Eds.), Constructing number (pp. 69-86). New York, Springer, Cham. |
[44] |
McMullen, J., Hannula-Sormunen, M. M., Laakkonen, E., & Lehtinen, E. (2016). Spontaneous focusing on quantitative relations as a predictor of the development of rational number conceptual knowledge. Journal of Educational Psychology, 108(6), 857-868.
doi: 10.1037/edu0000094 URL |
[45] | McMullen, J. A., Hannula-Sormunen, M. M., & Lehtinen, E. (2011). Young children’s spontaneous focusing on quantitative aspects and their verbalizations of their quantitative reasoning. In B. Ubuz (Ed.), Proceedings of the 35th conference of the international group for the psychology of mathematics education (pp. 217-224). Ankara, Turkey: PME. |
[46] |
McMullen, J. A., Hannula-Sormunen, M. M., & Lehtinen, E. (2013). Young children’s recognition of quantitative relations in mathematically unspecified settings. Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 32, 450-460.
doi: 10.1016/j.jmathb.2013.06.001 URL |
[47] |
Mcmullen, J., Hannula-Sormunen, M. M., & Lehtinen, E. (2015). Preschool spontaneous focusing on numerosity predicts rational number conceptual knowledge 6 years later. ZDM Mathematics Education, 47(5), 813-824.
doi: 10.1007/s11858-015-0669-4 URL |
[48] |
Melhuish, E. C., Phan, M. B., Sylva, K., Sammons, P., Siraj- Blatchford, I., & Taggart, B. (2008). Effects of the home learning environment and preschool center experience upon literacy and numeracy development in early primary school. Journal of Social Issues, 64(1), 95-114.
doi: 10.1111/josi.2008.64.issue-1 URL |
[49] |
Moll, K., Snowling, M. J., Göbel, S. M., & Hulme, C. (2015). Early language and executive skills predict variations in number and arithmetic skill in children at family-risk of dyslexia and typically developing controls. Learning and Instruction, 38, 53-62.
doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2015.03.004 URL pmid: 26412946 |
[50] |
Murayama, K., Pekrun, R., Lichtenfeld, S., & vom Hofe, R. (2013). Predicting long-term growth in students' mathematics achievement: The unique contributions of motivation and cognitive strategies. Child Development, 84(4), 1475-1490.
doi: 10.1111/cdev.12036 URL |
[51] |
Nanu, C. E., Mcmullen, J., Munck, P., & Hannula-Sormunen, M. (2017). Spontaneous focusing on numerosity in preschool as a predictor of mathematical skills and knowledge in the fifth grade. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 169, 42-58.
doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2017.12.011 URL pmid: 29331837 |
[52] | Pelland, J. C. (2020). Arithmetic, Culture, and Attention. In M. Zack, D. Schlimm (Eds.), Research in history and philosophy of mathematics (pp. 83-98). Cham, Birkhäuser. |
[53] |
Piantadosi, S. T., Jara-Ettinger, J., & Gibson, E. (2014). Children's learning of number words in an indigenous farming- foraging group. Developmental Science, 17(4), 553-563.
doi: 10.1111/desc.12078 URL pmid: 24766463 |
[54] |
Prast, E. J., van de Weijer-Bergsma, E., Miočević, M., Kroesbergen, E. H., & van Luit, J. E. H. (2018). Relations between mathematics achievement and motivation in students of diverse achievement levels. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 55, 84-96.
doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2018.08.002 URL |
[55] |
Purpura, D. J., & Ganley,C. M. (2014). Working memory and language: Skills-specific or domain-general relations to mathematics? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 122, 104-121.
doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2013.12.009 URL |
[56] |
Purpura, D. J., & Napoli,A. R. (2015). Early numeracy and literacy: Untangling the relation between specific components. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 17(2-3), 197-218.
doi: 10.1080/10986065.2015.1016817 URL |
[57] |
Rathé, S., Torbeyns, J., de Smedt, B., Hannula-Sormunen, M. M., & Verschaffel, L. (2018). Verbal and behavioral measures of kindergartners’ SFON and their associations with number-related utterances during picture book reading. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 88, 550-565.
doi: 10.1111/bjep.12201 URL pmid: 29152713 |
[58] |
Rathé, S., Torbeyns, J., de Smedt, B., & Verschaffel, L. (2019). Spontaneous focusing on Arabic number symbols and its association with early mathematical competencies. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 48, 111-121.
doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2019.01.011 URL |
[59] |
Rathé, S., Torbeyns, J., de Smedt, B., & Verschaffel, L. (2020). Are children’s spontaneous number focusing tendencies related to their home numeracy environment? ZDM, 52, 729-742.
doi: 10.1007/s11858-020-01127-z URL |
[60] | Rathé, S., Torbeyns, J., Hannula-Sormunen, M. M., de Smedt, B., & Verschaffel, L. (2016). Spontaneous focusing on numerosity: A review of recent research. Mediterranean Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 15, 1-25. |
[61] |
Sella, F., Berteletti, I., Lucangeli, D., & Zorzi, M. (2016). Spontaneous non-verbal counting in toddlers. Developmental Science, 19(2), 329-337.
doi: 10.1111/desc.12299 URL pmid: 25754974 |
[62] | Sharir, T., Mashal, N., & Mevarech, Z. R. (2015, August). To what extent can young children spontaneously recognize mathematical structures? Paper presented at the Biennial Conference of the European Association for Research on Learning and Instruction (EARLI), Limassol, Cyprus. |
[63] |
Shusterman, A., Cheung, P., Taggart, J., Bass, I., Berkowitz, T., Leonard, J. A., & Schwartz, A. (2017). Conceptual correlates of counting: Children’s spontaneous matching and tracking of large sets reflects their knowledge of the cardinal principle. Journal of Numerical Cognition, 3(1), 1-30.
doi: 10.5964/jnc.v3i1.65 URL |
[64] | Spelke, E. S. (2003). What makes us smart? Core knowledge and natural language. In D. Gentner, S. Goldin-Meadow (Eds.). Language in mind: Advances in the study of language and thought (pp. 277-311). Cambridge: MIT Press |
[65] |
Susperreguy, M. I., & Davis-Kean,P. E. (2016). Maternal math talk in the home and math skills in preschool children. Early Education and Development, 27(6), 841-857.
doi: 10.1080/10409289.2016.1148480 URL |
[66] | Tian, J., & Siegler, R. (2015, March). Spontaneous focusing on numerosity in early math development: A cross-national study of American and Chinese preschoolers. Poster presented at the Society for Research in Child Development Biennial Meeting, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA. |
[67] | Verschaffel, L., Rathé, S., Wijns, N., Degrande, T., van Dooren, W., de Smedt, B., & Torbeyns, J. (2020). Young children’s early mathematical competencies: The role of mathematical focusing tendencies. In T. Meaney, O. Helenius, M. L. Johansson, T. Lange, & A. Wernberg (Eds.) Mathematics education in the early years (pp. 23-42). New York: Springer, Cham. |
[68] |
Wang, M. -T., Ye, F., & Degol, J. L. (2017). Who chooses STEM careers? Using a relative cognitive strength and interest model to predict careers in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 46(8), 1805-1820.
doi: 10.1007/s10964-016-0618-8 URL pmid: 27975183 |
[69] |
Wang, Z., Shakeshaft, N., Schofield, K., & Malanchini, M. (2018). Anxiety is not enough to drive me away: A latent profile analysis on math anxiety and math motivation. PloS One, 13(2), e0192072. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0192072
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0192072 URL pmid: 29444137 |
[70] | Wigfield, A., Eccles, J. S., Fredricks, J. A., Simpkins, S., Roeser, R. W., & Schiefele, U. (2015). Development of achievement motivation and engagement. In M. L. Richard (Ed.) Handbook of child psychology and developmental science, (pp. 1-44). London: John Wiley & Sons, Inc. |
[71] |
Wijns, N., De Smedt, B., Verschaffel, L., & Torbeyns, J. (2019). Are preschoolers who spontaneously create patterns better in mathematics? British Journal of Educational Psychology, 90(3). doi: 10.1111/bjep.12329
doi: 10.1111/bjep.12315 URL pmid: 31513289 |
[1] | ZHANG Libin, ZHANG Qiwen, WANG Chenxu, ZHANG Yunyun. The co-varying relationship between children and adolescents’ peer networks and bullying-related behaviors from the perspective of social networks [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2023, 31(3): 416-427. |
[2] | JIANG Danying, YANG Yunmei, LI Jing. Children's distributive justice in group context [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(9): 2004-2019. |
[3] | JIANG Han. Developing an online model of educational rehabilitation for children with ADHD [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(8): 1747-1758. |
[4] | KANG Dan, LI Jiajia, CAI Shu. Preschool children’s sleep problem and language disorder [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(6): 1270-1281. |
[5] | LIU Xiaohan, CHEN Minglong, GUO Jing. Application of machine learning in prognosis and trajectory of post-traumatic stress disorder in children [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(4): 851-862. |
[6] | CHEN Jing, RAN Guangming, ZHANG Qi, NIU Xiang. The association between peer victimization and aggressive behavior in children and adolescents: A three-level meta-analysis [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(2): 275-290. |
[7] | WANG Hui, LI Guangzheng. Children’s gestures and the relationship with learning [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2021, 29(9): 1617-1627. |
[8] | WANG Lin, WANG Zhidan, WANG Hongjing. The neural mechanisms of developmental motor disorders in children with autism spectrum disorder [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2021, 29(7): 1239-1250. |
[9] | WANG Zhen. Children’s gender stereotype threat and its interventions [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2021, 29(2): 276-285. |
[10] | HE Qinghua, LI Dandan. Brain maturation and improvement in intertemporal choice in Chinese children and adolescents [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2020, 28(3): 381-389. |
[11] | CHEN Xiaoyan, WANG Yifang, ZHAO Jiayin, MENG Qi. The developmental characteristics and mechanism of children’s distinction between fantasy and reality [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2019, 27(7): 1232-1237. |
[12] | LIU Fang, LIU Wen, YU Tengxu. Relation between emotion regulation and child problem behaviors based on the perspective of temperament [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2019, 27(4): 646-656. |
[13] | ZHANG Qing, DONG Shuyang, WANG Zhengyan. The developmental mechanisms of self-regulation in young children [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2019, 27(2): 230-241. |
[14] | LIN Yunqiang, ZHU Huimin, LIAN Fuxin. Can children with autism spectrum disorder recover? Research evidence based on “optimal outcomes” and residual deficits of individuals in spectrum [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2018, 26(8): 1465-1474. |
[15] | SU Yi (ESTHER). Acquisition of core Chinese grammar in preschool children with autism spectrum disorders [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2018, 26(3): 391-399. |
Viewed | ||||||
Full text |
|
|||||
Abstract |
|
|||||