Advances in Psychological Science ›› 2023, Vol. 31 ›› Issue (11): 2050-2062.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2023.02050
• Regular Articles • Previous Articles Next Articles
Received:
2023-02-06
Online:
2023-11-15
Published:
2023-08-28
CLC Number:
WEI Yipu. Visual world paradigm reveals the time course of spoken language processing[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2023, 31(11): 2050-2062.
[1] | 林桐, 王娟. (2018). 基于视觉情境范式的口语词汇理解研究进展. 心理技术与应用, 6(9), 570-576. |
[2] | 邱丽景, 王穗苹, 关心. (2009). 口语理解的视觉-情境范式研究. 华南师范大学学报, (1), 130-136. |
[3] |
Allopenna, P. D., Magnuson, J. S., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (1998). Tracking the time course of spoken word recognition using eye movements: Evidence for continuous mapping models. Journal of Memory and Language, 38(4), 419-439. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1997.2558
doi: 10.1006/jmla.1997.2558 URL |
[4] |
Altmann, G. T. M. (2004). Language-mediated eye movements in the absence of a visual world: The “blank screen paradigm”. Cognition, 93(2), 79-87. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2004.02.005
URL pmid: 15147941 |
[5] |
Altmann, G. T. M., & Kamide, Y. (1999). Incremental interpretation at verbs: Restricting the domain of subsequent reference. Cognition, 73(3), 247-264. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0010-0277(99)00059-1
doi: 10.1016/s0010-0277(99)00059-1 URL pmid: 10585516 |
[6] |
Altmann, G. T. M., & Kamide, Y. (2007). The real-time mediation of visual attention by language and world knowledge: Linking anticipatory (and other) eye movements to linguistic processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 57(4), 502-518. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2006.12.004
doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2006.12.004 URL |
[7] |
Altmann, G. T. M., & Mirković, J. (2009). Incrementality and prediction in human sentence processing. Cognitive Science, 33(4), 583-609. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1551-6709.2009.01022.x
doi: 10.1111/j.1551-6709.2009.01022.x URL pmid: 20396405 |
[8] | Arnold, J. E., Eisenband, J. G., Brown-Schmidt, S., & Trueswell, J. C. (2000). The rapid use of gender information: Evidence of the time course of pronoun resolution from eyetracking. Cognition, 76(1), B13-B26. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0010-0277(00)00073-1 |
[9] |
Barr, D. J., Jackson, L., & Phillips, I. (2014). Using a voice to put a name to a face: The psycholinguistics of proper name comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(1), 404-413. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031813
doi: 10.1037/a0031813 URL |
[10] |
Canseco-Gonzalez, E., Brehm, L., Brick, C. A., Brown- Schmidt, S., Fischer, K., & Wagner, K. (2010). Carpet or cárcel: The effect of age of acquisition and language mode on bilingual lexical access. Language and Cognitive Processes, 25(5), 669-705. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960903474912
doi: 10.1080/01690960903474912 URL |
[11] |
Chambers, C. G., Tanenhaus, M. K., Eberhard, K. M., Filip, H., & Carlson, G. N. (2002). Circumscribing referential domains during real-time language comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 47(1), 30-49. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.2001.2832
doi: 10.1006/jmla.2001.2832 URL |
[12] |
Chambers, C. G., Tanenhaus, M. K., & Magnuson, J. S. (2004). Actions and affordances in syntactic ambiguity resolution. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30(3), 687-696. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.30.3.687
doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.30.3.687 URL |
[13] |
Chow, W. Y., & Chen, D. (2020). Predicting (in)correctly: Listeners rapidly use unexpected information to revise their predictions. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 35(9), 1149-1161. https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2020.1733627
doi: 10.1080/23273798.2020.1733627 URL |
[14] |
Cooper, R. M. (1974). The control of eye fixation by the meaning of spoken Language. Cognitive Psychology, 6(1), 84-107. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(74)90005-x
doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(74)90005-X URL |
[15] |
Corps, R. E., Brooke, C., & Pickering, M. J. (2021). Prediction involves two stages: Evidence from visual-world eye-tracking. Journal of Memory and Language, 122, 104298. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2021.104298
doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2021.104298 URL |
[16] |
Cozijn, R., Commandeur, E., Vonk, W., & Noordman, L. G.. (2011). The time course of the use of implicit causality information in the processing of pronouns: A visual world paradigm study. Journal of Memory and Language, 64(4), 381-403. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2011.01.001
doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2011.01.001 URL |
[17] |
Dahan, D., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2004). Continuous mapping from sound to meaning in spoken-language comprehension: Immediate effects of verb-based thematic constraints. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30(2), 498-513. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.30.2.498
doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.30.2.498 URL |
[18] |
Degen, J., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2015). Processing scalar implicature: A constraint-based approach. Cognitive Science, 39(4), 667-710. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12171
doi: 10.1111/cogs.12171 URL pmid: 25265993 |
[19] |
Degen, J., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2016). Availability of alternatives and the processing of scalar implicatures: A visual world eye-tracking study. Cognitive Science, 40(1), 172-201. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12227
doi: 10.1111/cogs.12227 URL pmid: 25807866 |
[20] |
DeLong, K. A., Urbach, T. P., & Kutas, M. (2005). Probabilistic word pre-activation during language comprehension inferred from electrical brain activity. Nature Neuroscience, 8(8), 1117-1121. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1504
URL pmid: 16007080 |
[21] |
Eichert, N., Peeters, D., & Hagoort, P. (2018). Language- driven anticipatory eye movements in virtual reality. Behavior Research Methods, 50(3), 1102-1115. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-017-0929-z
doi: 10.3758/s13428-017-0929-z URL pmid: 28791625 |
[22] |
Ferreira, F., Foucart, A., & Engelhardt, P. E. (2013). Language processing in the visual world: Effects of preview, visual complexity, and prediction. Journal of Memory and Language, 69(3), 165-182. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2013.06.001
doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2013.06.001 URL |
[23] | Frazier, L. (1987). Sentence processing:A tutorial review. In M.Coltheart (Ed.), Attention and performance XII: The psychology of reading (pp. 559-586). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. |
[24] | Gardner, B., Dix, S., Lawrence, R., Morgan, C., Sullivan, A., & Kurumada, C. (2021). Online pragmatic interpretations of scalar adjectives are affected by perceived speaker reliability. PLoS ONE, 16(2), e0245130. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0245130 |
[25] |
Garnham, A., Traxler, M., Oakhill, J., & Gernsbacher, M. A. (1996). The locus of implicit causality effects in comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 35(4), 517-543. https://doi.org/doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1996.0028
URL pmid: 26221059 |
[26] |
Grüter, T., Lau, E., & Ling, W. (2020). How classifiers facilitate predictive processing in L1 and L2 Chinese: The role of semantic and grammatical cues. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 35(2), 221-234. https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2019.1648840
doi: 10.1080/23273798.2019.1648840 URL |
[27] |
Hanna, J. E., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2004). Pragmatic effects on reference resolution in a collaborative task: Evidence from eye movements. Cognitive Science, 28(1), 105-115. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogsci.2003.10.002
doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog2801_5 URL |
[28] | Henderson, J. M., & Ferreira, F. (2004). Scene perception for psycholinguists. In J. M.Henderson & F.Ferreira (Eds.), The interface of language, vision, and action: Eye movements and the visual world (pp. 1-58). Psychology Press. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203488430 |
[29] |
Henry, N., Jackson, C. N., & Hopp, H. (2022). Cue coalitions and additivity in predictive processing: The interaction between case and prosody in L2 German. Second Language Research, 38(3), 397-422. https://doi.org/10.1177/0267658320963151
doi: 10.1177/0267658320963151 URL |
[30] |
Heyselaar, E., Peeters, D., & Hagoort, P. (2020). Do we predict upcoming speech content in naturalistic environments. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 36(4), 440-461. https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2020.1859568
doi: 10.1080/23273798.2020.1859568 URL |
[31] |
Huang, Y., & Snedeker, J. (2020). Evidence from the visual world paradigm raises questions about unaccusativity and growth curve analyses. Cognition, 200, 104251. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104251
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104251 URL |
[32] |
Huang, Y. T., & Snedeker, J. (2009). Semantic meaning and pragmatic interpretation in 5-year-olds: Evidence from real-time spoken language comprehension. Developmental Psychology, 45(6), 1723-1739. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016704
doi: 10.1037/a0016704 URL pmid: 19899927 |
[33] |
Huang, Y. T., & Snedeker, J. (2011). Logic and conversation revisited: Evidence for a division between semantic and pragmatic content in real-time language comprehension. Language and Cognitive Processes, 26(8), 1161-1172. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2010.508641
doi: 10.1080/01690965.2010.508641 URL |
[34] |
Huettig, F., & Guerra, E. (2019). Effects of speech rate, preview time of visual context, and participant instructions reveal strong limits on prediction in language processing. Brain Research, 1706, 196-208. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2018.11.013
doi: S0006-8993(18)30565-1 URL pmid: 30439351 |
[35] |
Huettig, F., & McQueen, J. M. (2007). The tug of war between phonological, semantic and shape information in language-mediated visual search. Journal of Memory and Language, 57(4), 460-482. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.001
doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.001 URL |
[36] |
Huettig, F., Olivers, C. N. L., & Hartsuiker, R. J. (2011a). Looking, language, and memory: Bridging research from the visual world and visual search paradigms. Acta Psychologica, 137(2), 138-150. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.07.013
doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.07.013 URL |
[37] |
Huettig, F., Rommers, J., & Meyer, A. S. (2011b). Using the visual world paradigm to study language processing: A review and critical evaluation. Acta Psychologica, 137(2), 151-171. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.11.003
doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.11.003 URL |
[38] | Ito, A., & Knoeferle, P. (2022). Analysing data from the psycholinguistic visual-world paradigm: Comparison of different analysis methods. Behavior Research Methods. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-022-01969-3 |
[39] |
Ito, A., Pickering, M. J., & Corley, M. (2018). Investigating the time-course of phonological prediction in native and non-native speakers of English: A visual world eye- tracking study. Journal of Memory and Language, 98, 1-11. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2017.09.002
doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2017.09.002 URL |
[40] | Kaan, E., & Grüter, T. (2021). Prediction in second language processing and learning:Advances and directions. In E.Kaan & T.Grüter (Eds.), Prediction in second language processing and learning (pp. 1-24). John Benjamins. |
[41] | Kaiser, E. (2016). Discourse-level Processing. In P.Knoeferle, P.Pyykkönen-Klauck, & M. W.Crocker (Eds.), Visually situated language comprehension (pp. 151-184). John Benjamins Publishing. |
[42] |
Kamide, Y., Scheepers, C., & Altmann, G. T. M. (2003). Integration of syntactic and semantic information in predictive processing: Cross-linguistic evidence from German and English. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 32(1), 37-55. https://doi.org/10.1023/a:1021933015362
URL pmid: 12647562 |
[43] |
Karimi, H., Brothers, T., & Ferreira, F. (2019). Phonological versus semantic prediction in focus and repair constructions: No evidence for differential predictions. Cognitive Psychology, 112, 25-47. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2019.04.001
doi: S0010-0285(18)30233-0 URL pmid: 31078824 |
[44] |
Knoeferle, P., & Crocker, M. W. (2006). The coordinated interplay of scene, utterance, and world knowledge: Evidence from eye tracking. Cognitive Science, 30(3), 481-529. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog0000_65
doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog0000_65 URL pmid: 21702823 |
[45] |
Knoeferle, P., & Crocker, M. W. (2007). The influence of recent scene events on spoken comprehension: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Memory and Language, 57(4), 519-543. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2007.01.003
doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.01.003 URL |
[46] |
Knoeferle, P., Crocker, M. W., Scheepers, C., & Pickering, M. J. (2005). The influence of the immediate visual context on incremental thematic role-assignment: Evidence from eye-movements in depicted events. Cognition, 95(1), 95-127. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2004.03.002
URL pmid: 15629475 |
[47] |
Koornneef, A. W., & van Berkum, J. J. A. (2006). On the use of verb-based implicit causality in sentence comprehension: Evidence from self-paced reading and eye tracking. Journal of Memory and Language, 54, 445-465. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2005.12.003
doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2005.12.003 URL |
[48] |
Koring, L., Mak, P., & Reuland, E. (2012). The time course of argument reactivation revealed: Using the visual world paradigm. Cognition, 123(3), 361-379. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2012.02.011
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.02.011 URL pmid: 22475295 |
[49] |
Kukona, A. (2020). Lexical constraints on the prediction of form: Insights from the visual world paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 46(11), 2153-2162. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000935
doi: 10.1037/xlm0000935 URL |
[50] |
Kuperberg, G. R., & Jaeger, T. F. (2016). What do we mean by prediction in language comprehension. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 31(1), 32-59. https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2015.1102299
URL pmid: 27135040 |
[51] | Levinson, S. C. (2000). Presumptive meanings: The theory of generalized conversational implicature. MIT Press. |
[52] |
Levy, R. (2008). Expectation-based syntactic comprehension. Cognition, 106(3), 1126-1177. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2007.05.006
URL pmid: 17662975 |
[53] |
Li, X., Li, X., & Qu, Q. (2022). Predicting phonology in language comprehension: Evidence from the visual world eye-tracking task in Mandarin Chinese. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 48(5), 531-547. https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000999
doi: 10.1037/xhp0000999 URL |
[54] |
Magnuson, J. S. (2019). Fixations in the visual world paradigm: Where, when, why. Journal of Cultural Cognitive Science, 3(2), 113-139. https://doi.org/10.1007/s41809-019-00035-3
doi: 10.1007/s41809-019-00035-3 URL |
[55] |
Magnuson, J. S., Tanenhaus, M. K., Aslin, R. N., & Dahan, D. (2003). The time course of spoken word learning and recognition: Studies with artificial lexicons. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132(2), 202-227. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.132.2.202
doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.132.2.202 URL |
[56] |
Mak, W. M., Tribushinina, E., Lomako, J., Gagarina, N., Abrosova, E., & Sanders, T. (2017). Connective processing by bilingual children and monolinguals with specific language impairment: Distinct profiles. Journal of Child Language, 44(2), 329-345. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0305000915000860
doi: 10.1017/S0305000915000860 URL pmid: 26847820 |
[57] |
Matin, E., Shao, K. C., & Boff, K. R. (1993). Saccadic overhead: Information-processing time with and without saccades. Perception & Psychophysics, 53(4), 372-380. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03206780
doi: 10.3758/BF03206780 URL |
[58] |
McMurray, B., Samelson, V. M., Lee, S. H., & Tomblin, J. B. (2010). Individual differences in online spoken word recognition: Implications for SLI. Cognitive Psychology, 60(1), 1-39. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2009.06.003
doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2009.06.003 URL pmid: 19836014 |
[59] |
McRae, K., Spivey-Knowlton, M. J., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (1998). Modeling the influence of thematic fit (and other constraints) in on-line sentence comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 38(3), 283-312. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1997.2543
doi: 10.1006/jmla.1997.2543 URL |
[60] |
Millis, K. K., & Just, M. A. (1994). The influence of connectives on sentence comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 33(1), 128-147. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1994.1007
doi: 10.1006/jmla.1994.1007 URL |
[61] | Mirman, D. (2014). Growth Curve Analysis and Visualization Using R. CRC Press. |
[62] |
Mirman, D., Dixon, J. A., & Magnuson, J. S. (2008). Statistical and computational models of the visual world paradigm: Growth curves and individual differences. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(4), 475-494. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2007.11.006
doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.11.006 URL pmid: 19060958 |
[63] | Nieuwland, M. S., Politzer-Ahles, S., Heyselaar, E., Segaert, K., Darley, E., Kazanina, N., ... Huettig, F. (2018). Large-scale replication study reveals a limit on probabilistic prediction in language comprehension. ELife, 7, 1-24. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.33468 |
[64] | Porretta, V., Kyröläinen, A.-J., van Rij, J., & Järvikivi, J. (2018). Visual world paradigm data:From preprocessing to nonlinear time-course analysis. In I.Czarnowski, R.Howlett, & L.Jain (Eds.), Intelligent decision technologies 2017. Smart innovation, systems and technologies (Vol. 73, pp. 268-277). Springer. |
[65] |
Pickering, M. J., & Gambi, C. (2018). Predicting while comprehending language: A theory and review. Psychological Bulletin, 144(10), 1002-1044. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000158
doi: 10.1037/bul0000158 URL pmid: 29952584 |
[66] |
Pyykkönen, P., & Järvikivi, J. (2010). Activation and persistence of implicit causality information in spoken language comprehension. Experimental Psychology, 57(1), 5-16. https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000002.
doi: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000002 URL pmid: 20176549 |
[67] | Pyykkönen-Klauck, P., & Crocker, M. W. (2016). Attention and eye movement metrics in visual world eye tracking. In P.Knoeferle, P.Pyykkönen-Klauck, & M. W.Crocker (Eds.), Visually Situated Language Comprehension (pp. 67-82). John Benjamins Publishing. |
[68] |
Rayner, K. (1978). Eye movements in reading and information processing. Psychological Bulletin, 85(3), 618-660. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.85.3.618
URL pmid: 353867 |
[69] | Runner, J. T., Sussman, R. S., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2003). Assignment of reference to reflexives and pronouns in picture noun phrases: Evidence from eye movements. Cognition, 89(1), B1-B13. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(03)00065-9 |
[70] |
Salverda, A. P., Brown, M., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2011). A goal-based perspective on eye movements in visual world studies. Acta Psychologica, 137(2), 172-180. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.09.010
doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.09.010 URL pmid: 21067708 |
[71] | Salverda, A. P., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2017). The visual world paradigm. In A. M. B. deGroot & P., Hagoort (Eds.), Research methods in psycholinguistics and the neurobiology of language: A practical guide (pp. 89-110). Wiley- Blackwell. |
[72] |
Saryazdi, R., & Chambers, C. G. (2021). Real-time communicative perspective taking in younger and older adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 47(3), 439-454.
doi: 10.1037/xlm0000890 URL |
[73] |
Saslow, M. G. (1967). Latency of saccadic eye movement. Journal of the Optical Society of America, 57(8), 1030-1033. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.2003.96.1.173
doi: 10.1364/josa.57.001030 URL pmid: 6035297 |
[74] |
Seedorff, M., Oleson, J., & McMurray, B. (2018). Detecting when timeseries differ: Using the bootstrapped differences of timeseries (BDOTS) to analyze visual world paradigm data (and more). Journal of Memory and Language, 102, 55-67. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2018.05.004
doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2018.05.004 URL pmid: 32863563 |
[75] |
Snedeker, J., & Trueswell, J. C. (2004). The developing constraints on parsing decisions: The role of lexical-biases and referential scenes in child and adult sentence processing. Cognitive Psychology, 49(3), 238-299. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2004.03.001
URL pmid: 15342261 |
[76] |
Stewart, A. J., Pickering, M. J., & Sanford, A. J. (2000). The time course of the influence of implicit causality information: Focusing versus integration accounts. Journal of Memory and Language, 42(3), 423-443. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1999.2691
doi: 10.1006/jmla.1999.2691 URL |
[77] |
Stone, K., Lago, S., & Schad, D. J. (2021). Divergence point analyses of visual world data: Applications to bilingual research. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 24(5), 833-841. https://doi.org/10.1017/s1366728920000607
doi: 10.1017/S1366728920000607 URL |
[78] |
Tanenhaus, M. K., Magnuson, J. S., Dahan, D., & Chambers, C. (2000). Eye movements and lexical access in spoken-language comprehension: Evaluating a linking hypothesis between fixations and linguistic processing. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 29(6), 557-580. https://doi.org/10.1023/a:1026464108329
URL pmid: 11196063 |
[79] |
Tanenhaus, M. K., Spivey-Knowlton, M. J., Eberhard, K. M., & Sedivy, J. C. (1995). Integration of visual and linguistic information in spoken language comprehension. Science, 268(5217), 1632-1634. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.7777863
URL pmid: 7777863 |
[80] | Traxler, M. J., Bybee, M. D., & Pickering, M. J. (1997). Influence of connectives on language comprehension: Eye tracking evidence for incremental interpretation. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 50A(3), 481-497. https://doi.org/10.1080/027249897391982 |
[81] |
Trueswell, J. C., Tanenhaus, M. K., & Garnsey, S. M. (1994). Semantic influences on parsing: Use of thematic role information in syntactic ambiguity resolution. Journal of Memory and Language, 33(3), 285-318. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1994.1014
doi: 10.1006/jmla.1994.1014 URL |
[82] |
Weber, A., & Cutler, A. (2004). Lexical competition in non-native spoken-word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 50(1), 1-25. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0749-596x (03)00105-0
doi: 10.1016/S0749-596X(03)00105-0 URL |
[83] |
Wei, Y., Mak, W. M., Evers-Vermeul, J., & Sanders, T. J. M. (2019). Causal connectives as indicators of source information: Evidence from the visual world paradigm. Acta Psychologica, 198, 102866. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2019.102866
doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2019.102866 URL |
[1] | Genying Huang, Yafang Li. Identification of Depression in Old Age Based on Eye Movement Characteristics [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2023, 31(suppl.): 142-142. |
[2] | KOU Juan, YANG Mengyuan, WEI Zijie, LEI Yi. The social motivation theory of autism spectrum disorder: Exploring mechanisms and interventions [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2023, 31(1): 20-32. |
[3] | WEI Zi-Han, LI Xingshan. Decision Process Tracing: Evidence from Eye-movement Data [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2015, 23(12): 2029-2041. |
[4] | ZHANG Yang-Yang;RAO Li-Lin;LIANG Zhu-Yuan;ZHOU Yuan;LI Shu. Process Test of Risky Decision Making: New Understanding, New Evidence Pitting Non-compensatory Against Compensatory Models [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2014, 22(2): 205-219. |
[5] |
Zhou Xiangxian;Jin Zhicheng.
Researches on Consumers’ Visual Attention to Print Advertisements Using Eye-Tracking Methodology [J]. , 2006, 14(2): 287-. |
Viewed | ||||||
Full text |
|
|||||
Abstract |
|
|||||