Advances in Psychological Science ›› 2019, Vol. 27 ›› Issue (1): 128-140.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2019.00128
• Regular Articles • Previous Articles Next Articles
Received:
2018-04-29
Online:
2019-01-15
Published:
2018-11-23
CLC Number:
ZHAN Ze, WU Baopei. Ubiquitous harm: Moral judgment in the perspective of the theory of dyadic morality[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2019, 27(1): 128-140.
1 |
段蕾, 莫书亮, 范翠英, 刘华山 . (2012). 道德判断中心理状态和事件因果关系的作用:兼对道德判断双加工过程理论的检验. 心理学报,44(12), 1607-1617.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2012.01607 URL |
2 |
李占星, 朱莉琪 . (2015). 道德情绪判断与归因:发展与影响因素. 心理科学进展,23(6), 990-999.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2015.00990 URL |
3 | 塔西佗 . (1981). 塔西佗《编年史》 (王以铸, 崔妙因译). 北京: 商务印书馆. |
4 |
王鹏, 方平, 姜媛 . (2011). 道德直觉背景下的道德决策:影响因素探究. 心理科学进展,19(4), 573-579.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2011.00573 URL |
5 | 吴宝沛, 张雷 . (2012). 厌恶与道德判断的关系. 心理科学进展,20(2), 309-316. |
6 | 谢熹瑶, 罗跃嘉 . (2009). 道德判断中的情绪因素——从认知神经科学的角度进行探讨. 心理科学进展,17(6), 1250-1256. |
7 | 杨青松 . (2013). 时间距离对道德违规行为判断的影响及其作用机制(博士学位论文). 湖南师范大学. |
8 |
喻丰, 彭凯平, 韩婷婷, 柴方圆, 柏阳 . (2011). 道德困境之困境——情与理的辩争. 心理科学进展,19(11), 1702-1712.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2011.01702 URL |
9 | 张国清 . (2013). 罗尔斯难题: 正义原则的误读与批评. 中国社会科学, 39(10),22-40. |
10 |
张琨, 方平, 姜媛, 于悦, 欧阳恒磊 . (2014). 道德视野下的内疚. 心理科学进展,22(10), 1628-1636.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2014.01628 URL |
11 | 张梦圆, 苑明亮, 寇彧 . (2016). 论西方道德心理研究的新综合取向: 道德基础理论. 北京师范大学学报(社会科学版), 253(1), 50-59. |
12 |
Ames D.L., &Fiske S.T . (2015). Perceived intent motivates people to magnify observed harms. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, of the United States of America, 112(12), 3599-3605.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1501592112 URL pmid: 25733850 |
13 |
Atkinson Q.D., & Bourrat P. (2011). Beliefs about god, the afterlife and morality support the role of supernatural policing in human cooperation. Evolution and Human Behavior, 32(1), 41-49.
doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.07.008 URL |
14 |
Barrett H. C., Bolyanatz A., Crittenden A. N., Fessler D. M. T., Fitzpatrick S., Gurven M ., et al. Laurence, S. (2016). Small-scale societies exhibit fundamental variation in the role of intentions in moral judgment. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 113(17), 4688-4693.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1522070113 URL pmid: 27035959 |
15 | Berniūnas R., Dranseika V., & Sousa P . (2016). Are there different moral domains? Evidence from Mongolia. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 19(3), 275-282. |
16 | Björklund F., Haidt J., & Murphy S . (2000). Moral dumbfounding: When intuition finds no reason. Department of Psychology. Lund University. |
17 |
Bleske-rechek A., Nelson L. A., Baker J. P., Remiker M. W., & Brandt S. J . (2010). Evolution and the trolley problem: People save five over one unless the one is young, genetically related, or a romantic partner. Journal of Social, 4(3), 115-127.
doi: 10.1037/h0099295 URL |
18 | Bloom P., & Jarudi I. (2006). The Chomsky of morality. Nature, 443(7114), 909-910. |
19 |
Buchtel E. E., Guan Y., Peng Q., Su Y., Sang B., Chen S. X., & Bond M. H . (2015). Immorality east and west: Are immoral behaviors especially harmful, or especially uncivilized?. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 41(10), 1382-1394.
doi: 10.1177/0146167215595606 URL pmid: 26253486 |
20 |
Cameron C. D., Lindquist K. A., & Gray K . (2015). A constructionist review of morality and emotions: No evidence for specific links between moral content and discrete emotions. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 19(4), 371-394.
doi: 10.1177/1088868314566683 URL pmid: 25587050 |
21 |
Chakroff A., Dungan J., & Young L . (2013). Harming ourselves and defiling others: What determines a moral domain? Plos One, 8(9), e74434.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0074434 URL pmid: 24040245 |
22 |
Chakroff A., Russell P. S., Piazza J., & Young L . (2017). From impure to harmful: Asymmetric expectations about immoral agents. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 69, 201-209.
doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2016.08.001 URL |
23 | Darwin, C. (1871/1981). The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. |
24 |
DelGaizo A.L., &Falkenbach D.M . (2008). Primary and secondary psychopathic-traits and their relationship to perception and experience of emotion. Personality and Individual Differences, 45(3), 206-212.
doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2008.03.019 URL |
25 | Descioli P., & Kurzban R. (2009). Mysteries of morality. Cognition, 112(2), 281-299. |
26 |
Duke A.A., & Bègue L. (2015). The drunk utilitarian: Blood alcohol concentration predicts utilitarian responses in moral dilemmas. Cognition, 134, 121-127.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.09.006 URL pmid: 25460385 |
27 |
Eskine K. J., Kacinik N. A., & Prinz J. J . (2011). A bad taste in the mouth: Gustatory disgust influences moral judgment. Psychological Science, 22(3), 295-299.
doi: 10.1177/0956797611398497 URL pmid: 21307274 |
28 |
Fadda R., Parisi M., Ferretti L., Saba G., Foscoliano M., Salvago A., & Doneddu G . (2016). Exploring the role of theory of mind in moral judgment: The case of children with autism spectrum disorder. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 523.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00523 URL pmid: 4834434 |
29 |
Fehr B., &Russell J.A . (1991). The concept of love viewed from a prototype perspective. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60(3), 425-438.
doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.60.3.425 URL |
30 |
FeldmanHall O., Dalgleish T., Evans D., Navrady L., Tedeschi E., & Mobbs D . (2016). Moral chivalry: Gender and harm sensitivity predict costly altruism. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 7(6), 542-551.
doi: 10.1177/1948550616647448 URL pmid: 27478541 |
31 | Gleichgerrcht E., & Young L. (2013). Low levels of empathic concern predict utilitarian moral judgment. Plos One, 8(4), e60418. |
32 |
Godin G., Conner M., & Sheeran P . (2005). Bridging the intention-behaviour gap: The role of moral norm. British Journal of Social Psychology, 44(4), 497-512.
doi: 10.1348/014466604X17452 URL pmid: 16368016 |
33 |
Graham J., Haidt J., Koleva S., Motyl M., Iyer R., Wojcik S. P., & Ditto P. H . (2013). Moral foundations theory: The pragmatic validity of moral pluralism. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 47(12), 55-130.
doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-407236-7.00002-4 URL |
34 | Gray H. M., Gray K., & Wegner D. M . (2007). Dimensions of mind perception. Science, 315(5812), 619. |
35 |
Gray K. . (2012). The power of good intentions: Perceived benevolence soothes pain, increases pleasure, and improves taste. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 3(5), 639-645.
doi: 10.1177/1948550611433470 URL |
36 |
Gray K., &Keeney J.E . (2015). Impure or just weird? Scenario sampling bias raises questions about the foundation of morality. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 6(8), 859-868.
doi: 10.1177/1948550615592241 URL |
37 |
Gray K., Schein C., & Cameron C. D . (2017). How to think about emotion and morality: Circles, not arrows. Current Opinion in Psychology, 17, 41-46.
doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.06.011 URL pmid: 28950971 |
38 |
Gray K., Schein C., & Ward A. F . (2014). The myth of harmless wrongs in moral cognition: Automatic dyadic completion from sin to suffering. Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 143(4), 1600-1615.
doi: 10.1037/a0036149 URL pmid: 24635184 |
39 | Gray K., Waytz A., & Young L . (2012). The moral dyad: A fundamental template unifying moral judgment. Psychological Inquiry, 23(2), 206-215. |
40 |
Gray K., &Wegner D.M . (2009). Moral typecasting: Divergent perceptions of moral agents and moral patients. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(3), 505-520.
doi: 10.1037/a0013748 URL pmid: 19254100 |
41 |
Gray K., &Wegner D.M . (2010). Blaming god for our pain: Human suffering and the divine mind. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 14(1), 7-16.
doi: 10.1177/1088868309350299 URL pmid: 19926831 |
42 |
Gray K., &Wegner D.M . (2011). To escape blame, don't be a hero — Be a victim. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47(2), 516-519.
doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2010.12.012 |
43 |
Gray K., Young L., & Waytz A . (2012). Mind perception is the essence of morality. Psychological Inquiry, 23(2), 101-124.
doi: 10.1080/1047840X.2012.651387 URL pmid: 22754268 |
44 | Greene J., & Haidt J. (2002). How (and where) does moral judgment work?. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(12), 517-523. |
45 |
Greene J. D., Morelli S. A., Lowenberg K., Nystrom L. E., & Cohen J. D . (2008). Cognitive load selectively interferes with utilitarian moral judgment. Cognition, 107(3), 1144-1154.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.11.004 URL pmid: 2429958 |
46 |
Greene J. D., Nystrom L. E., Engell A. D., Darley J. M., & Cohen J. D . (2004). The neural bases of cognitive conflict and control in moral judgment. Neuron, 44(2), 389-400.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.09.027 URL pmid: 15473975 |
47 |
Guglielmo S., &Malle B.F . (2017). Information- acquisition processes in moral judgments of blame. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 43(7), 957-971.
doi: 10.1177/0146167217702375 URL pmid: 28903702 |
48 | Haidt J. . (2001). The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychological Review, 108(4), 814-834. |
49 | Haidt J. . (2007). The new synthesis in moral psychology. Science, 316(5827), 998-1002. |
50 | Haidt J., Graham J. , & Ditto, P. The Volkswagen of moral psychology. Retrieved October 28, 2015, from & Ditto, P. The Volkswagen of moral psychology. Retrieved October 28, 2015, from |
51 |
Haidt J., Mccauley C., & Rozin P . (1994). Individual differences in sensitivity to disgust: A scale sampling seven domains of disgust elicitors. Personality and Individual Differences, 16(5), 701-713.
doi: 10.1016/0191-8869(94)90212-7 URL |
52 |
Hamlin J. K., Wynn K., & Bloom P . (2010). 3-month-olds show a negativity bias in their social evaluations. Developmental Science, 13(6), 923-929.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00951.x URL pmid: 2966030 |
53 |
Hamlin J. K., Wynn K., Bloom P., & Mahajan N . (2011). How infants and toddlers react to antisocial others. Proceedings of the national academy of sciences of the United States of America, 108(50), 19931-19936.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1110306108 URL pmid: 22123953 |
54 |
Hofmann W., Wisneski D. C., Brandt M. J., & Skitka L. J . (2014). Morality in everyday life. Science, 345(6202), 1340-1343.
doi: 10.1126/science.1251560 URL pmid: 25214626 |
55 |
Hutcherson C.A., &Gross J.J . (2011). The moral emotions: A social-functionalist account of anger, disgust, and contempt. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100(4), 719-737.
doi: 10.1037/a0022408 URL pmid: 21280963 |
56 |
Kawai N., Kubo K., & Kubo-Kawai N . (2014). “Granny dumping”: Acceptability of sacrificing the elderly in a simulated moral dilemma. Japanese Psychological Research, 56(3), 254-262.
doi: 10.1111/jpr.12049 URL |
57 |
Laurin K., &Plaks J.E . (2014). Religion and punishment: Opposing influences of orthopraxy and orthodoxy on reactions to unintentional acts. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 5(7), 835-843.
doi: 10.1177/1948550614534698 |
58 |
Levine E.E., &Schweitzer M.E . (2014). Are liars ethical? On the tension between benevolence and honesty. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 53, 107-117.
doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2014.03.005 URL |
59 |
Levine E.E., &Schweitzer M.E . (2015). Prosocial lies: When deception breeds trust. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 126, 88-106.
doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2014.10.007 URL |
60 | Margoni F., & Surian L. (2017). Children’s intention-based moral judgments of helping agents. Cognitive Development, 41, 46-64. |
61 |
Marsh A.A., &Cardinale E.M . (2014). When psychopathy impairs moral judgments: Neural responses during judgments about causing fear. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(1), 3-11.
doi: 10.1093/scan/nss097 URL pmid: 22956667 |
62 |
Miller R. M., Hannikainen I. A., & Cushman F. A . (2014). Bad actions or bad outcomes? Differentiating affective contributions to the moral condemnation of harm. Emotion, 14(3), 573-587.
doi: 10.1037/a0035361 URL pmid: 24512250 |
63 |
Monroe A.E., &Malle B.F . (2017). Two paths to blame: Intentionality directs moral information processing along two distinct tracks. Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 146(1), 123-133.
doi: 10.1037/xge0000234 URL pmid: 28054816 |
64 |
Nichols S. . (2002). Norms with feeling: Towards a psychological account of moral judgment. Cognition, 84(2), 221-236.
doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00048-3 URL pmid: 12175573 |
65 |
Nobes G., Panagiotaki G., & Bartholomew K. J . (2016). The influence of intention, outcome and question-wording on children's and adults' moral judgments. Cognition, 157, 190-204.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2016.08.019 URL pmid: 27649094 |
66 |
Park G., Kappes A., Rho Y ., & Van Bavel, J. J. (2016). At the heart of morality lies neuro-visceral integration: Lower cardiac vagal tone predicts utilitarian moral judgment. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11(10), 1588-1596.
doi: 10.1093/scan/nsw077 URL pmid: 5040918 |
67 |
Peter D., Kelly A., & Robert K . (2012). Omissions and byproducts across moral domains. PloS One, 7(10), e46963.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0046963 URL pmid: 23071678 |
68 |
Pratt M. W., Golding G., Hunter W., & Sampson R . (2010). Sex differences in adult moral orientations. Journal of Personality, 56(2), 373-391.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1988.tb00891.x URL |
69 |
Rai T.S., &Fiske A.P . (2011). Moral psychology is relationship regulation: Moral motives for unity, hierarchy, equality, and proportionality. Psychological Review, 118(1), 57-75.
doi: 10.1037/a0021867 URL pmid: 21244187 |
70 |
Rai T.S., &Holyoak K.J . (2010). Moral principles or consumer preferences? Alternative framings of the trolley problem. Cognitive Science, 34(2), 311-321.
doi: 10.1111/j.1551-6709.2009.01088.x URL pmid: 21564214 |
71 |
Rizzo M. T., Cooley S., Elenbaas L., & Killen M . (2018). Young children’s inclusion decisions in moral and social-conventional group norm contexts. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 165, 19-36.
doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2017.05.006 URL pmid: 28645542 |
72 |
Royzman E., Atanasov P., Landy J. F., Parks A., & Gepty A . (2014). Cad or mad? Anger (not disgust) as the predominant response to pathogen-free violations of the divinity code. Emotion, 14(5), 892-907.
doi: 10.1037/a0036829 URL pmid: 24866519 |
73 | Royzman E. B., Kim K., & Leeman R. F . (2015). The curious tale of Julie and Mark: Unraveling the moral dumbfounding effect. Judgment and Decision Making, 10(4), 296-313. |
74 |
Rozin P., Lowery L., Imada S., & Haidt J . (1999). The CAD triad hypothesis: A mapping between three moral emotions (contempt, anger, disgust) and three moral codes (community, autonomy, divinity). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(4), 574-586.
doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.76.4.574 URL pmid: 10234846 |
75 |
Schein C., & Gray K. (2015). The unifying moral dyad: Liberals and conservatives share the same harm-based moral template. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 41(8), 1147-1163.
doi: 10.1177/0146167215591501 URL pmid: 26091912 |
76 |
Schein C., & Gray K. (2016). Moralization and harmification: The dyadic loop explains how the innocuous becomes harmful and wrong. Psychological Inquiry, 27(1), 62-65.
doi: 10.1080/1047840X.2016.1111121 URL |
77 |
Schein C., & Gray K. (2017). The theory of dyadic morality: Reinventing moral judgment by redefining harm. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 22(1), 32-70.
doi: 10.1177/1088868317698288 URL pmid: 28504021 |
78 |
Schein C., Ritter R. S., & Gray K . (2016). Harm mediates the disgust-immorality link. Emotion, 16(6), 862-876.
doi: 10.1037/emo0000167 URL pmid: 27100369 |
79 |
Schnall S., Haidt J., Clore G. L., & Jordan A. H . (2008). Disgust as embodied moral judgment. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34(8), 1096-1109.
doi: 10.1177/0146167208317771 URL pmid: 18505801 |
80 |
Scott S. E., Inbar Y., & Rozin P . (2016). Evidence for absolute moral opposition to genetically modified food in the United States. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11(3), 315-324.
doi: 10.1177/1745691615621275 URL pmid: 27217243 |
81 | Shweder R. A., Much, N. C, Mahapatra, M., & Park L . (1997). The "Big Three" of morality (autonomy, community, divinity) and the "Big Three" explanations of suffering. In A. M. Brandt & P. Rozin (Eds.), Morality and health(pp. 119-169). New York: Routledge. |
82 |
Sytsma J., & Machery E. (2012). The two sources of moral standing. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 3(3), 303-324.
doi: 10.1007/s13164-012-0102-7 URL |
83 |
Theriault J., Waytz A., Heiphetz L., & Young L . (2017). Examining overlap in behavioral and neural representations of morals, facts, and preferences. Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 146(11), 1586-1605.
doi: 10.1037/xge0000350 URL pmid: 28805441 |
84 | Tisak M.S., &Jankowski A.M . (1996). Societal rule evaluations: Adolescent offenders' reasoning about moral, conventional, and personal rules. Aggressive Behavior: Official Journal of the International Society for Research on Aggression, 22(3), 195-207. |
85 | Trivers R.L . (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 46(1), 35-57. |
86 |
Wagemans F., Brandt M. J., & Zeelenberg M . (2017). Disgust sensitivity is primarily associated with purity-based moral judgments. Emotion, 18(2), 277-289.
doi: 10.1037/emo0000359 URL pmid: 28872334 |
87 |
van der Toorn J., Nail P. R., Liviatan I., & Jost J. T . (2014). My country, right or wrong: Does activating system justification motivation eliminate the liberal-conservative gap in patriotism?. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 54, 50-60.
doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2014.04.003 URL |
88 |
Welch M., &Bryan J.L . (2000). Moral campaigns, authoritarian aesthetics, and escalation: An examination of flag desecration in the post-Eichman era. Journal of Crime & Justice, 23(1), 25-45.
doi: 10.1080/0735648X.2000.9721108 URL |
89 |
Wisneski D.C., &Skitka L.J . (2017). Moralization through moral shock: Exploring emotional antecedents to moral conviction. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 43(2), 139-150.
doi: 10.1177/0146167216676479 URL pmid: 27872393 |
90 |
Wright P. J., Tokunaga R. S., & Bae S . (2014). Pornography consumption and us adults' attitudes toward gay individuals' civil liberties, moral judgments of homosexuality, and support for same-sex marriage: Mediating and moderating factors. Communication Monographs, 81(1), 79-107.
doi: 10.1080/03637751.2013.871048 URL |
91 |
Young L., & Tsoi L. (2013). When mental states matter, when they don't, and what that means for morality. Social & Personality Psychology Compass, 7(8), 585-604.
doi: 10.1111/spc3.12044 URL |
[1] | ZHU Lin, LIU Jinru, LI Jing, LIU Conghui. Moral foreign language effect and its moderating variables: A systematic review and meta-analysis [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(1): 32-50. |
[2] | ZHANG Yinhua, LI Hong, WU Yin. The application of computational modelling in the studies of moral cognition [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2020, 28(7): 1042-1055. |
[3] | YAN Yu, LI Tong. An analysis of the reverse mechanism how the victim turn into an instigator on workplace incivility [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2018, 26(7): 1307-1318. |
[4] | HU Xiaomeng, YU Feng, PENG Kaiping. How does culture affect morality? The perspectives of between-culture variations, within-culture variations, and multiculturalism [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2018, 26(11): 2081-2090. |
[5] | LI Ming-Hui, RAO Li-Lin. Moral judgment from construal level theory perspective [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2017, 25(8): 1423-1430. |
[6] | YE Hongyan; ZHANG Fenghua. Embodiment in Moral Judgment [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2015, 23(8): 1480-1488. |
[7] | WU Bao-Pei;GAO Shu-Ling. Moral Hypocrisy: An Opportunistic Adaptive Strategy [J]. , 2012, 20(6): 926-934. |
[8] | SHEN Wang-Bing;LIU Chang. Critical Review on Psychological Studies on Moral Hypocrisy [J]. , 2012, 20(5): 745-756. |
[9] | WU Bao-Pei;CHANG Lei. On the Relationship Between Disgust and Moral Judgment [J]. , 2012, 20(2): 309-316. |
[10] | YU Feng;PENG Kai-Ping;HAN Ting-Ting;CHAI Fang-Yuan;BAI Yang. Dilemma of Moral Dilemmas: The Conflict between Emotion and Reasoning in Moral Judgments [J]. , 2011, 19(11): 1702-1712. |
[11] | DU Xiao-Xiao; ZHENG Quan-Quan. A Brief Review of the Knobe Effect [J]. , 2010, 18(1): 91-96. |
[12] | XIE Xi-Yao; LUO Yue-Jia. The Role of Emotion in Moral Judgment: Evidence from Cognitive Neuroscience [J]. , 2009, 17(6): 1250-1256. |
Viewed | ||||||
Full text |
|
|||||
Abstract |
|
|||||